You are on page 1of 9

Gait & Posture 27 (2008) 294–302

www.elsevier.com/locate/gaitpost

Proprioceptive contribution of postural control as assessed from


very slow oscillations of the support in healthy humans
M. Vaugoyeau a,*, S. Viel a, B. Amblard a, J.P. Azulay a,b, C. Assaiante a
a
Groupe ‘‘Développement et Pathologie de l’Action’’, UMR 6196, CNRS, 31 chemin J. Aiguier, 13402 Marseille Cedex 20, France
b
Department of Neurology, University Hospital Timone, Marseille, France
Received 1 August 2005; received in revised form 22 November 2006; accepted 9 April 2007

Abstract

Maintaining erect human posture depends on graviceptive information. This can come from at least of three origins: vestibular, visual and
somaesthetic. We hypothesize here that subject’s use proprioception rather than visual or vestibular cues for their control of upright body
posture and this even when subjects stand on a tilting body support surface. In order to find experimental evidence for this hypothesis, we
exclude in our experiments visual cues (eyes close) and by keeping frequency and amplitude of the tilt stimulus so low that it would be below
the detection threshold for vestibular semi-circular canal stimuli. The orientations of body segments were analysed during various phases of
the perturbation cycle. Segmental stabilisations were defined in terms of both the global anchoring index calculated during the whole
perturbation cycle and an appropriate sequential anchoring index calculated during various phases in the perturbation cycle. We show that
subjects tend to align their bodies with the space vertical and do so better for their heads than for their upper bodies and lower bodies. A further
finding is that stabilisation is related to the tilt stimulus in the form that it is minimal at the turning points of the tilt, where peak tilt velocity is
minimal with the sinusoidal stimulus used.
These finding suggest first that proprioceptive cues are predominant in the control of body orientation in quasi-static condition and second
that the head and trunk stabilisation strategies used as the basis of postural control depend on the properties of the moving support.
# 2007 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Postural orientation; Segmental stabilisation; Proprioception; Semi-circular canals; Humans

1. Introduction provided by the hydrostatic pressure in large blood vessels


might be another good candidate for monitoring body
Under static conditions, postural control involves verticality. Dietz et al. [2] have suggested that the Golgi
adopting a given body orientation, and maintaining this tendon organ might monitor the forces exerted by the
posture despite the perturbing effects of gravity and other muscles counteracting the gravitational forces, which
external forces. It has been clearly established that visual, therefore serve as graviceptors. According to Bisdorff
vestibular and somatosensory cues provide the information et al. [3], the perception of body verticality by seated
required to estimate verticality. Other somaesthetic grav- subjects may thus mainly depend on proprioceptive/contact
iceptive information may, also be used to control body cues. Bronstein [4] has also stressed the prominent role
verticality. Mittelstaedt [1] has provided evidence for the played by somatosensory cues in the perception of body
existence of graviceptive sensors around the kidneys. This verticality.
author also put forward the idea that the information In the present study, we addressed the question as to
whether somaesthetic graviceptive information may suffice
to actively maintain a vertical posture while standing in
* Corresponding author at: CNRS, UMR 6196, Groupe DPA, CNRS, 31
chemin J. Aiguier, 13402 Marseille Cedex 20, France.
quasi-static condition. For this purpose, we used an original
Tel.: +33 4 91 16 43 63; fax: +33 4 91 77 50.84. experimental procedure making it possible to isolate the
E-mail address: vaugoyau@dpm.cnrs-mrs.fr (M. Vaugoyeau). somaesthetic graviceptive cues. Standing subjects with their

0966-6362/$ – see front matter # 2007 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.gaitpost.2007.04.003
M. Vaugoyeau et al. / Gait & Posture 27 (2008) 294–302 295

eyes closed were subjected to small angular sinusoidal


perturbations of the support platform on which they were
standing, applied well below the detection threshold of the
semi-circular canal system. The anterior and posterior semi-
circular canals detecting pitch and roll rotations are known
to play a particularly important role in the detection of fast
postural sways, but not low frequency sways such as those
occurring during quiet stance [5,6]. The threshold level at
which the vestibular detection of sway is possible on the
pitch plane is around the 18/s reported to be the threshold
value at which the conscious perception of pitch motion
occurs [7–10]. In fact, according to Henn et al. [11], the
threshold value at which angular acceleration detection
occurs is approximately 0.28/s2. The levels of both the
frequency and the amplitude of the oscillating supporting
platform used here were therefore selected so that they were
not detectable by the semi-circular canals. Although the
otolithic system is theoretically capable of detecting all
frequencies up to null level [12], we were confident that
since the subjects had no use of visual cues, somesthethic
cues were the main type of information used by the present
subjects for postural control.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Subjects and experimental procedure

Ten healthy subjects (aged 28.6  8 years) took part in the


experiment, which was approved by the local ethical committee,
and all the subjects gave their informed consent prior to the
study.
Subjects stood on a motorised one-directional rotating plat-
form with their eyes closed, and the platform was rotated
sinusoidally at 0.01 Hz (108 peak to peak) in either the pitch
or roll direction, depending on the subjects’ position on the
platform. They had to maintain a vertical posture as steadily
as possible, keeping their feet 15 cm apart without flexing their
knees. The trial lasted for 106 s, including a whole cycle of
Fig. 1. Characteristics of the supporting platform movement. The first curve
angular platform movement. The maximum angular accelerations
represents the angular displacements of the supporting platform. The second
of the platform were thus well below the vestibular canal’s represents the angular velocity of the supporting platform and the third
detection threshold, namely 0.28/s2 [11]. Therefore, if any angu- represents the angular acceleration of the supporting platform. The narrow
lar head accelerations occurred beyond this threshold value, they indicate the peak of inclination, of velocity and of acceleration.
would not result directly from the platform movements and would
not be involved in correcting the experimentally induced postural
disturbances. 2.2. Data collection and kinematics analysis
Fig. 1 represents the characteristics of the supporting platform
movements in term of angular displacements, velocity and accel- Data collection was performed with the ELITE automatic
eration. We can note that the maximum velocity was 0.318/s and the motion analyser working at 100 Hz, using passive body markers
maximum acceleration was 0.028/s2. [13]. The subject performed the task facing a couple of ELITE
Preliminary experiments were performed to assess the visual cameras in the case of lateral perturbation, or in side view (right
contribution to both segmental orientation and stabilisation. Five profile) in the case of antero-posterior perturbation. In the case of
subjects were tested with their eyes open and closed while a lateral lateral oscillations, they wore 10 markers (15 mm in diameter) on
perturbation was applied to the supporting platform. Since no the following anatomical points: left and right infra-orbital margins
significant effects of vision were observed, we continued to (markers 1, 2); left and right acromions (3, 4); left and right superior
explore the effects of perturbations to the supporting platform anterior iliac spines (5, 6); left and right great trochanters (7, 8) and
under eyes closed conditions, which seemed to be the most left and right medial malleoli (9, 10). In the case of anterior–
relevant conditions for investigating the proprioceptive contribu- posterior platform oscillations, the subjects wore seven markers on
tion to postural control. Data about the role of vision are presented the right side of the body: the meatus and infra-orbital margin
in Section 3. (markers 1, 2); acromion (3); iliac crest (4), trochanter (5); tibial
296 M. Vaugoyeau et al. / Gait & Posture 27 (2008) 294–302

plate (6) and medial malleolus (7). During both tasks two markers at each trial as follows, as shown in Fig. 2.
were placed on the platform to measure its movements. In each
experiment, each subject was subjected to the two types of dis- s 2r  s 2a
AI ¼
turbances, namely lateral and anterior–posterior platform oscilla- s 2r þ s 2a
tions, occurring in a pseudo-random order.
where sa is the standard deviation of the angular distribution
2.3. Controlled variables about the roll or pitch axis of the segment under investiga-
tion with respect to the allocentric reference (absolute
2.3.1. Mean orientation vertical direction) value and sr is the corresponding standard
At each trial, the mean orientation of each body segment (head, deviation of the angular distribution with respect to the
shoulders, trunk and pelvis in response to lateral oscillations of the moving platform.
platform; and head, trunk and thigh in response to antero-posterior A positive value indicates a better segmental stabilisation along
oscillations of the platform) was calculated during a whole cycle the absolute vertical or horizontal axis than in response to the
(100 s) of platform movement on the plane of the perturbation. A moving platform, whereas a negative value indicates a better
reference orientation value was obtained for each body segment in segmental stabilisation on the platform than. The segmental
each subject during 10 s of upright stance on a stable support with anchoring index was therefore used to compare the level of
the eyes open. Likewise, the mean instantaneous orientation of stabilisation of a given segment achieved by the subject in relation
each body segment was calculated during each tenth of a cycle to the gravity vertical or to the biased orientation of the supporting
(10 s) of platform movement, in order to assess the time course of platform.
the segmental orientation process.
2.4. Statistical analysis
2.3.2. Attenuation factor
The attenuation factor (AF) of the changes induced in the At least two trials were run with each subject on each variable
segmental orientation due to the movement of the supporting investigated under both experimental conditions. At each trial, the
platform is a variable used to assess the amplitude of the disor- reference value was subtracted from the mean orientation of each
ientation induced in each segment by the postural perturbation. It body segment in each subject. The average difference among all
was assessed using the following formula: subjects was then compared to zero using a single-sample analysis
t-test.
sP  sS Anchoring indexes, moving anchoring indexes and attenuation
AF ¼
sP þ sS factor were also compared to zero, using a single-sample analysis
(t-test) against the null hypothesis. Since these indices were in the
where sP is the standard deviation of the absolute distribution 1 to +1 range, we used a z transform to convert the values into an
(with respect to the gravity vertical) of the orientations of the unbiased Gaussian distribution. Comparisons were made between
supporting platform and sS is the standard deviation of the the head and trunk orientation and stabilisation performances in
corresponding distribution of the absolute orientations of a response to the two kinds of disturbance, using an ANOVA.
given segment. AF = 1 would correspond to complete Changes in the ability to use a given mode of segmental
attenuation, which means that a given segment has remained stabilisation during platform perturbations were assessed in terms
vertical in response to the perturbation. By contrast, AF = 0 of the coefficient of correlation between the moving anchoring
would mean that no attenuation had occurred, in other words, index obtained with a given segment and the corresponding slope of
that a given segment has moved with the same amplitude as the supporting platform, regardless of the direction in which the
platform was tilted. A coefficient of correlation differing signifi-
the supporting platform. Lastly, if AF is negative, this means
cantly from zero would show the existence of some linkage
that the subject has moved more than the supporting platform
between the segmental stabilisation strategy used and the slope
and may run the risk of falling off it. The main advantage of of the supporting platform.
this index is that it takes into account the whole signal,
whereas peak-to-peak analysis would focus only on two short
periods of time during the behaviour of interest. 3. Results
2.3.3. Anchoring index
3.1. Role of proprioception
Segmental stabilisation was defined in terms of the global
anchoring index calculated during the whole cycle of perturbation
[14–18] and with the moving anchoring index calculated during 3.1.1. Postural orientation
various phases of the perturbation cycle. The moving anchoring 3.1.1.1. Mean orientation. The mean orientation of the various
index was calculated based on the formula developed for the segments was never significantly different from the reference value,
anchoring index during successive 10-s steps (10 s) in each trial, on either plane of perturbation
in order to establish the time course of the segmental responses to
very slow angular perturbations of the supporting platform, thus 3.1.1.2. Sequential orientation and attenuation factor. The
showing the existence of any changes in these responses. time course of the segmental orientation process occurring in
The segmental anchoring index was used to compare the response to the lateral and antero-posterior perturbations is shown
stabilisation of a given segment with respect to both an external in Fig. 3, respectively, in the various segments of interest. This
reference value and the moving platform. This index was calculated sequential orientation process showed few variations with time.
M. Vaugoyeau et al. / Gait & Posture 27 (2008) 294–302 297

Fig. 2. (Left upper part) Diagram of the head roll angle with respect to the external axis, ua, and of the head roll angle with respect to the support, ur. (Right upper
part) Roll angular displacement of the supporting platform (upper trace), the head (middle trace) and the relative angular movement of the head with respect to
the supporting platform (lower trace). (Left lower part) Diagram of the absolute and relative roll dispersions of the head, according to the definition of the
anchoring index (AI). In this example, AI is positive, which means that the head is stabilized in space independently of the support movements. (Right lower
part) Principles used to calculate the moving anchoring index during successive 12-s steps in the perturbation cycle.

The damping of the oscillations induced in the anatomical stabilisation achieved at the various anatomical levels. No signifi-
segments was assessed in terms of the attenuation factor (AF). cant differences were detected between the AI values obtained on
All the segmental movements were noticeably damped in compar- the head, shoulder, trunk and pelvis under lateral perturbation
ison with movements of the supporting platform, since the AF conditions or between those obtained on the head, trunk and pelvis
differed significantly from zero in the case of lateral (t = 8.38, under antero-posterior perturbation conditions.
p < 0.0001; t = 12.23, p < 0.0001; t = 17.46, p < 0.001; t = 7.48,
p < 0.0001, for head, shoulders, trunk, and pelvis, respectively) as
well as antero-posterior platform perturbations (t = 3.891,
3.1.2.2. Moving anchoring index. In order to determine
whether any changes occurred in the segmental stabilisation stra-
p < 0.001; t = 8.33, p < 0.001; t = 10.12, p < 0.0001, for head,
tegies used during the long cycle (100 s) of the very slow angular
trunk and thigh, respectively).
platform perturbations, the head and trunk AI were calculated in
In the case of lateral perturbations, these AF ranged roughly
successive 10-s steps during each trial. This sequential AI is shown
between 0.3 and 0.6. No significant between-segments differences
in Fig. 5 for each segment. The coefficients of correlation between
were observed in the AF between the head, shoulders, trunk and
the sequential AI and the time course of the absolute slope of the
pelvis. Nor were any significant differences observed in the case of
supporting platform were also calculated and are given in Fig. 5.
antero-posterior perturbations, between the AF occurring in the
Similar stabilisation strategies were observed at both head and
head, trunk and thigh.
trunk level under both support perturbation conditions. These
segments were well stabilized in space during the perturbation,
3.1.2. Postural stabilisation but when the maximum absolute platform slope was reached, the
3.1.2.1. Global anchoring indexes. The mean anchoring strategy preferentially used to stabilize the head and trunk was lost
indexes of each anatomical segment (with S.D.s) are shown in and the value of the anchoring index decreased to zero. In other
Fig. 4. words, the segmental stabilisation performances depended on the
With both kinds of platform perturbations, the anchoring absolute slope of the supporting platform. The coefficients of
indexes (AI) were significantly positive in all the segments, show- correlation were significantly negative with all segments under
ing that good stabilisation of the segments was achieved in space (in both conditions of perturbation (in the case of lateral perturbation
the case of lateral platform perturbations: t = 7.19, p < 0.0001; of the support: t = 3.15, p = 0.01; t = 4.78, p = 0.001; for head and
t = 6.54, p < 0.0001; t = 8.57, p < 0.001; t = 3.35, p = 0.01, for trunk, respectively; and in the case of antero-posterior support
head, shoulders, trunk, and pelvis respectively; and in the case of perturbations: t = 4.78, p = 0.001; t = 5.61, p = 0.01; for head and
antero-posterior platform perturbations: (t = 4.46, p < 0.01; trunk, respectively). The negative values of the coefficients indi-
t = 9.04, p = 0.0001; t = 6.81, p = 0.0001, for head, trunk and thigh cated that an increase in the slope of the supporting platform
respectively). Comparisons were also carried out on the degrees of resulted in a decrease in the value of the anchoring index.
298 M. Vaugoyeau et al. / Gait & Posture 27 (2008) 294–302

Fig. 3. Sequential orientation of the various segments (left) and mean and standard deviation of attenuation factor the various anatomic levels studied, in
response to lateral (part a) and antero-posterior (part b) movement of the supporting platform when no visual cues were available.

Fig. 4. (Left part) Mean global anchoring indexes with respect to the supporting platform and standard deviation of head, shoulders, trunk and pelvis when
lateral perturbations to were applied to the supporting platform and no visual cues were available. (Right part) Mean global anchoring indexes with respect to the
supporting platform and standard deviation of head, trunk and thigh when antero-posterior perturbations were applied to the supporting platform and no visual
cues were available.
M. Vaugoyeau et al. / Gait & Posture 27 (2008) 294–302 299

Fig. 5. (Left part) Moving anchoring indexes of head and trunk when lateral perturbations were applied to the supporting platform. (Right part) Moving
anchoring indexes of head and trunk with respect to the antero-posterior perturbation of the supporting platform. The gray points and lines represent the variation
of the inclination of the support. The coefficients of correlation (CC) were calculated between the segmental moving anchoring index and the corresponding
absolute orientation of the supporting platform.

3.2. Role of vision open and closed. The significant positive anchoring index obtained
indicated that the segments were all stabilized on space under both
visual conditions.
3.2.1. Postural orientation
3.2.1.1. Mean orientation. The subjects were able to maintain
a good vertical position under both visual conditions, as shown by
the absence of any significant effects of vision observed in the 4. Discussion
ANOVA analysis.
In this experiment, the subjects were able to keep their
3.2.1.2. Attenuation factor. Under lateral support disturbance body upright despite the absence of visual cues and semi-
conditions, the averaged AF recorded in each segment is given in circular canal information from the support platform
Fig. 6. The AF ranged roughly between 0.3 and 0.6 in both movements. On both planes of perturbation, the subjects’
conditions with and without vision. No significant visual effects head and trunk stabilisation strategies were lost when the
were detected in any of the segments. support was tilted to the maximum, corresponding to the
minimum velocity. This finding suggests that the head and
3.2.2. Postural stabilisation trunk stabilisation strategies used as the basis of postural
The AI are shown in Fig. 6. No significant differences were control depend on the properties of the movement of the
observed between the AI values recorded with the subjects’ eyes support in term of acceleration, velocity and inclination.

Fig. 6. Part a: Mean attenuation factor and standard deviation of head, shoulders, trunk and pelvis with vision (in white) and without vision (in black) when
lateral perturbation were applied to the supporting platform. Part b: Mean global anchoring index and standard deviation of head, shoulders, trunk and pelvis
with vision (in white) and without vision (in black) when lateral perturbations were applied to the supporting platform.
300 M. Vaugoyeau et al. / Gait & Posture 27 (2008) 294–302

4.1. Proprioceptive inputs suffice to control vertical of upright stance in patients with bilateral vestibular loss and
body orientation in healthy adults control subjects when visual and somatosensory orientation
cues are removed. Postural responses to sinusoidal tilts of a
In this study, the mean orientation of the body segments motion platform in the sagittal plane (28, f = 0.05, 0.1, 0.2
never differed from the reference values measured with the and 0.4 Hz) were studied in normal subjects and vestibular
eyes open and without support motion. In addition, the time loss patients. They found that absence of vestibular cues and
course of the segmental orientation process occurring when removal of vision had little effect on stabilization of upright
lateral and antero-posterior perturbations were applied to the body posture in space. They conclude that in the absence of
support showed few variations. These findings clearly show both vestibular and visual cues, somatosensory graviception
that in the absence of vision, postural perturbations applied still provided some information on body orientation during
below the vestibular canal detection threshold did not affect slow perturbation of the support. These findings confirm the
upright stance in healthy young subjects. This means that predominance of somatosensory cues for postural control in
somatosensory cues, possibly along with otholitic informa- quasi-static condition as suggested by the model proposed
tion, easily sufficed for the subjects to efficiently maintain by Mergner and Rosemeier [26].
both their overall and segmental body orientation. These data and our results taken together suggest a major
Although the otolithic organs were previously thought to influence of somesthetic information, in perception of body
provide the subject with information about the vertical pull orientation and in postural orientation control in quasi-static
of gravity, some authors have challenged this idea. condition.
Stoffregen and Riccio [19] put forward the alternative Nevertheless, the vestibular inputs may become much
hypothesis that perceived orientation may be mainly more useful for more dynamical situations (for more rapid
determined by the dynamically defined direction of balance; tilts). It has also been demonstrated that when proprioceptive
thanks to sensory integration processes [20,21]. and visual cues are unavailable, postural control appears to
As described Bacsi and Colebatch [25], the vestibular require intact vestibular function [25].
system may contribute to postural control by two ways: by
way of the conscious perception or by the way of reflex 4.2. Segmental stabilisation depends on the movement
responses to imposed displacements. Concerning the of the support
conscious perception of body verticality, Bringoux et al.
[22] reported, using a perturbation comparable to our as In the absence of both vestibular canalar and visual cues,
concerns peak velocity or acceleration of the tilt stimuli, that the subjects were able to stabilize their body segments in
gravity-based somaesthetic cues are more informative than space in response to support perturbations. This shows the
otolithic cues for the perception of a quasi-static body importance of the contribution of somesthetic cues to
orientation. On the other hand, Bronstein [4] established that segmental stabilisation. However, the time course of the
the subjective postural vertical was never biased in patients segmental stabilisation showed that the segmental stabilisa-
with acute unilateral vestibular disorders. In a recent study, tion strategy used depended on the direction in which the
Bringoux et al. [23] reported that blindfolded bilateral support disturbances were applied.
labyrinthine-defective subjects performed as accurately as Gianna et al. [28] have established that the lowest
healthy subjects when asked to detect the slow changes frequency at which motion direction is detectable during
occurring in their body orientation when they were passively passive lateral whole-body acceleration in healthy subjects
displaced from the upright to an angle of 0.058/s. These is about 4.84 cm/s2 (range 2.9–6.3). In our experiments, the
results, suggest that the accurate perception of body highest linear acceleration (peak value) at the head level in
orientation under quasi-static conditions may be mainly strictly en bloc-subjects worked out at 3.35 cm/s in subjects
allowed on somatosensory rather than otolithic information. 1 m 70 in height. This means that subjects’ head acceleration
Recent data by Teasdale et al. [24] have also suggested that was mostly below the threshold value reported by Gianna
under static conditions, the otoliths are poor detectors of the et al. [28]. In addition, because of the damping known to
gravity force direction and that the accuracy of the occur at the head level, the subjects in our study must have
perception of body orientation improves when propriocep- adopted a segmental strategy decreasing the linear head
tive information can be dynamically integrated. But, tthese acceleration rather than an en bloc strategy. They therefore
study concern more the body orientation perception than the probably stabilised their bodies using the proprioceptive
control of postural orientation. inputs rather than the vestibular system. In addition,
Concerning the implication of reflex responses to experiments carried out under microgravity condition
imposed displacements, Mergner and Rosemeier [26] have showed that segmental stabilisation does not depend
proposed a ‘new concept’ of postural control, which crucially on static vestibular afferents [29].
postulates that the vestibular signal is used in a feed Interestingly, we also established that the segmental
forward way, while the body-on-support adjustments are stabilisation strategies were correlated with the angle at
handled mainly by proprioceptive control mechanisms. With which the support was tilted. More specifically, the fact that
this in mind, Maurer et al. [27] have investigated the control head and trunk anchoring indexes became null when the
M. Vaugoyeau et al. / Gait & Posture 27 (2008) 294–302 301

largest tilt angles were applied to the supporting platform, [6] Nashner LM, Shupert CL, Horak FB, Black FO. Organization of
indicates that the subjects loose the reference frame used to posture controls: an analysis of sensory and mechanical constraints..
Prog Brain Res 1989;80:411–8.
stabilize their segments. However, since the movements [7] Benson AJ, Hutt EC, Brown SF. Thresholds for the perception of
applied to the supporting platform were sinusoidal, the whole body angular movement about a vertical axis. Aviat Space
greatest amplitudes corresponded to the lowest speeds. Environ Med 1989;60:205–13.
The fact that the segmental stabilisation in space may be [8] Peterka RJ, Benolken MS. Role of somatosensory and vestibular cues
in attenuating visually induced human postural sway. Exp Brain Res
dependent of the orientation of the support was never
1995;105:101–10.
described up to now. This is an interesting result, since [9] Schweigart G, Heimbrand S, Mergner T, Becker W. Perception
segmental stabilisation, and especially that of the head, was of horizontal head and trunk rotation: modification of neck input
supposed to be devoted to a better body equilibrium during following loss of vestibular function. Exp Brain Res 1993;95:
dynamic destabilisation, such as during skiing or surfing. 533–546.
Our results seem to indicate that this ability is not [10] Fitzpatrick R, McCloskey DI. Proprioceptive, visual and vestibular
thresholds for the perception of sway during standing in humans. J
necessarily spontaneously present, but may have to be Physiol 1994;478:173–86.
trained while moving on an irregular and/or tilted terrain. [11] Henn V, Cohen B, Young LR. Visual-vestibular interaction in motion
In conclusion, there are three kind of sensory information perception and the generation of nystagmus. Neurosci Res Program
available to control posture: the visual, the vestibular and the Bull 1980;18:457–651.
somatosensory information. Our data confirm the predomi- [12] Nashner LM. A model describing vestibular detection of body sway
motion. Acta Otolaryngol 1971;72:429–36.
nance of proprioceptive information in the control of [13] Ferrigno G, Pedotti A. ELITE: a digital dedicated hardware system for
postural orientation in quasi-static condition, the vestibular movement analysis via real-time TV signal processing. IEEE Trans
system requiring large postural change to detect any change Biomed Eng 1985;32:943–50.
in body orientation. Concerning the segmental stabilization, [14] Assaiante C, Amblard B. Ontogenesis of head stabilization in space
during locomotion in children: influence of visual cues. Exp Brain Res
this study shows that head and trunk stabilisation depends on
1993;93:499–515.
the parameters of the support’s destabilisation. Further [15] Assaiante C, Thomachot B, Aurenty R. Hip stabilization and lateral
studies on patients with proprioceptive integration deficits, balance control in toddlers during the first four months of autonomous
such as parkinsonian patients or deafferented patients, are walking. Neuroreport 1993;4:875–8.
now required to be able to understand more clearly the role [16] Assaiante C, Thomachot B, Aurenty R, Amblard B. Organization of
of proprioception in postural control. lateral balance control in tolders during the first year of autonomous
walking. J Motor Behav 1998;30:114–29.
[17] Nadeau S, Amblard B, Mesure S, Bourbonnais D. Head and trunk
stabilization strategies during forward and backward walking in
Acknowledgments healthy adults. Gait Posture 2003;18:134–42.
[18] Borel L, Harlay F, Magnan J, Chays A, Lacour M. Deficits and
recovery of head and trunk orientation and stabilization after unilateral
We are grateful to the subjects who participated in this vestibular loss. Brain 2002;125:880–94.
study. We thank Jessica Anderson-Blanc for revising the [19] Stoffregen TA, Riccio GE. An ecological theory of orientation and the
English. This research was supported by the French Medical vestibular system. Psychol Rev 1988;95:3–14.
Research Foundation (FRM). [20] Riccio GE, Martin EJ, Stoffregen TA. The role of balance dynamics in
the active perception of orientation. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept
Perform 1992;18:624–44.
[21] Riccio GE, Stoffregen TA. Gravitoinertial force versus the direction of
Conflict of interest balance in the perception and control of orientation. Psychol Rev
1990;97:135–7.
We declare that we have no conflict of interest. [22] Bringoux L, Nougier V, Barraud PA, Marin L, Raphel C. Con-
tribution of somesthetic information to the perception of body
orientation in the pitch dimension. Quart J Exp Psychol A
2003;56:909–23.
References [23] Bringoux L, Schmerber S, Nougier V, Dumas G, Barraud PA, Raphel
C. Perception of slow pitch and roll body tilts in bilateral labyrinthine-
[1] Mittelstaedt H. Somatic versus vestibular gravity reception in man. defective subjects. Neuropsychologia 2002;40:367–72.
Ann NY Acad Sci 1992;656:124–39. [24] Teasdale N, Nougier V, Barraud PA, Bourdin C, Debu B, Poquin D,
[2] Dietz V, Gollhofer A, Kleiber M, Trippel M. Regulation of bipedal et al. Contribution of ankle, knee, and hip joints to the perception
stance: dependency on ‘‘load’’ receptors. Exp Brain Res 1992;89: threshold for support surface rotation. Percept Psychophys
229–31. 1999;61:615–24.
[3] Bisdorff AR, Wolsley CJ, Anastasopoulos D, Bronstein AM, Gresty [25] Bacsi AM, Colebatch JG. Evidence for reflex and perceptual vestibular
MA. The perception of body verticality (subjective postural vertical) contributions to postural control. Exp Brain Res 2005;160:22–8.
in peripheral and central vestibular disorders. Brain 1996;119: [26] Mergner T, Rosemeier T. Interaction of vestibular, somatosensory and
1523–34. visual signals for postural control and motion perception under
[4] Bronstein AM. The interaction of otolith and proprioceptive informa- terrestrial and microgravity conditions—a conceptual model. Brain
tion in the perception of verticality. The effects of labyrinthine and Res Brain Res Rev 1998;28:118–35.
CNS disease. Ann NY Acad Sci 1999;871:324–33. [27] Maurer C, Mergner T, Bolha B, Hlavacka F. Vestibular, visual, and
[5] Nashner LM. Vestibular postural control model. Kybernetika 1972;10: somatosensory contributions to human control of upright stance.
106–10. Neurosci Lett 2000;281:99–102.
302 M. Vaugoyeau et al. / Gait & Posture 27 (2008) 294–302

[28] Gianna C, Heimbrand S, Gresty M. Thresholds for detection of motion [29] Amblard B, Assaiante C, Fabre JC, Mouchnino L, Massion J. Volun-
direction during passive lateral whole-body acceleration in normal tary head stabilization in space during oscillatory trunk movements in
subjects and patients with bilateral loss of labyrinthine function. Brain the frontal plane performed in weightlessness. Exp Brain Res
Res Bull 1996;40:443–7. 1997;114:214–25.

You might also like