You are on page 1of 12

This article was downloaded by: [Schok, Michaela]

On: 27 April 2010


Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 921678656]
Publisher Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-
41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Aging & Mental Health


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.informaworld.com/smpp/title~content=t713404778

A model of resilience and meaning after military deployment: Personal


resources in making sense of war and peacekeeping experiences
Michaela L. Schok a; Rolf J. Kleber b;Gerty J. L. M. Lensvelt-Mulders c
a
Centre for Research and Expertise, Veterans Institute, 3940 AC Doorn, The Netherlands b Department
of Clinical & Health Psychology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands c Department of Theory
of Sciences and Methodology, University for Humanistics, Utrecht, The Netherlands

Online publication date: 27 April 2010

To cite this Article Schok, Michaela L. , Kleber, Rolf J. andLensvelt-Mulders, Gerty J. L. M.(2010) 'A model of resilience
and meaning after military deployment: Personal resources in making sense of war and peacekeeping experiences',
Aging & Mental Health, 14: 3, 328 — 338
To link to this Article: DOI: 10.1080/13607860903228812
URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13607860903228812

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.informaworld.com/terms-and-conditions-of-access.pdf

This article may be used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial or
systematic reproduction, re-distribution, re-selling, loan or sub-licensing, systematic supply or
distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden.

The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents
will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae and drug doses
should be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss,
actions, claims, proceedings, demand or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly
or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.
Aging & Mental Health
Vol. 14, No. 3, April 2010, 328–338

A model of resilience and meaning after military deployment: Personal resources in making
sense of war and peacekeeping experiences
Michaela L. Schoka*, Rolf J. Kleberb and Gerty J.L.M. Lensvelt-Muldersc
a
Centre for Research and Expertise, Veterans Institute, P.O. Box 125, 3940 AC Doorn, The Netherlands; bDepartment of
Clinical & Health Psychology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands; cDepartment of Theory of Sciences and
Methodology, University for Humanistics, Utrecht, The Netherlands
(Received 21 April 2009; final version received 30 July 2009)

The aim of the present study was to examine whether the specific personal resources of self-esteem, optimism and
perceived control, combined in the latent variable called ‘resilience’, were associated with cognitive processing of
war-zone experiences. Data were collected by questionnaires from a sample of 1.561 veterans who had
participated in various war or peacekeeping operations. Structural equation modelling was performed to assess
the expected relationships between the observed and latent variables. The construct of resilience was well-defined
and proved to be strongly associated with both construals of meaning, comprehensibility versus personal
significance, after military deployment. According to our model, higher resilience predicted less distrust in others
and the world, more personal growth and less intrusions and avoidance after military deployment.
Downloaded By: [Schok, Michaela] At: 12:31 27 April 2010

Keywords: veterans; resilience; meaning; distrust; growth

Introduction expectations among a sample of veterans that faced


In research on stress and trauma, there is a growing adversity by being in a war zone. Self-esteem, optimism
interest in the capacity to bounce back from adversity and perceived control may be thought of as core
strengthened and more resourceful. As Walsh (2006) resources that contribute to a resilient personality
describes, resilience ‘. . . enables people to heal painful (Major, Richards, Cooper, Cozzarelli, & Zubek, 1998).
wounds, take charge of their lives, and to go on to live They are expected to minimise the perception of threat
fully and love well’ (p. 5). Moreover, resilience among related to war-zone stressors and the need to make
adults represents a distinct and empirically separable meaning of the experience.
outcome trajectory from that normally associated with Taylor’s theory of cognitive adaptation (1983)
recovery from trauma (Bonanno, 2004). It is the assumes that self-enhancing biases can buffer current
capability of maintaining a stable trajectory of healthy threats and possible future setbacks by increasing
functioning across time and in the face of adversity self-esteem, developing an optimistic outlook, and
(Bonanno, 2004). What is more, resilience appears to regaining a sense of mastery over the event.
be a common phenomenon arising from ordinary Self-enhancement refers to the efforts to improve
human adaptive processes which has been overlooked self-image and restore self-esteem. These biases have
in general (Masten, 2001). To understand the human been shown to result in better adjustment among
potential to thrive under adverse circumstances, a more Bosnian civilians in the immediate aftermath of the
positive view of normative human capabilities is Balkan civil war and among survivors of the 11
necessary. September 2001 attack in New York City (Bonanno,
A growing body of empirical studies recognises that Field, Kovacevic, & Kaltman, 2002; Bonanno,
processing threatening events results in personal Rennicke, & Dekel, 2005). Furthermore, the adaptive
growth or psychological benefits (Helgeson, benefits of self-enhancement were most pronounced
Reynolds, & Tomich, 2006; Zoellner & Maercker, for those who had suffered the most severe and violent
2006). Moreover, veterans reported more positive than losses, suggesting a buffering role for self-enhancement
negative effects of their war and peacekeeping experi- (Westphal & Bonanno, 2007). Moreover, O’Donnell,
ences, especially when they perceived high threat (see Elliott, Jones Wolfgang, and Creamer (2007) suggest
for a review Schok, Kleber, Elands, & Weerts, 2008). that self-schemas are dominant in the development and
They gained more self-confidence, valued family and maintenance of posttraumatic stress symptoms. More
friends more, and believed that the experience had specifically, negative self-appraisals played the most
expanded their horizons. Personal resources, such as influential role in determining later posttraumatic
self-esteem, optimism and perceived control are stress symptomatology. Based on these findings, we
expected to support the processing of threatening expect that high self-esteem lowers perceived threat,
experiences. The present study examined these therefore, minimising the need to make sense of

*Corresponding author. Email: ml.schok@veteraneninstituut.nl

ISSN 1360–7863 print/ISSN 1364–6915 online


ß 2010 Taylor & Francis
DOI: 10.1080/13607860903228812
http://www.informaworld.com
Aging & Mental Health 329

threatening experiences which in turn decreases post- individuals perceive threat (Davis, Nolen-Hoeksema,
traumatic stress reactions. & Larson, 1998; Janoff-Bulman & Yopyk, 2004;
People who have a disposition to be optimistic and Taylor, 1983). First, people seek to understand the
therefore have generally positive expectations for the event in retrospect and try to comprehend what has
future reported less distress across a broad range of happened (Janoff-Bulman & Frantz, 1997; Joseph &
situations (Andersson, 1996) and coped more effec- Linley, 2005: Wong & Weiner, 1981). This means that
tively with stress (Scheier & Carver, 1985; Taylor, they have to make sense of the event by understanding
2003). Solberg Nes and Segerstrom (2006) pointed out what happened, how it happened, and why it hap-
that optimistic coping appears to be flexible with pened. Second, they have to find personal significance
regard to the demands of the stressor and is responsive in the event or gain from the experience for their
to the possibility of modifying either the stressor itself present life (Antoni et al., 2001; Helgeson, et al., 2006;
or one’s response to it. Therefore, dispositional opti- Joseph & Linley, 2005; Tedeschi & Calhoun, 1996).
mism is expected to lower perceived threat, decrease This implies that people evaluate and interpret what
the need to derive meaning from deployment, and the experience has brought them in terms of personal
reduce posttraumatic stress reactions in veterans. skills, relationships, life philosophy and world view,
Moreover, optimism is supposed to alleviate symptoms also referred to as benefit finding or posttraumatic
and improve adjustment to stressful events because it growth. This study will take both construals of
represents the opposite of negative affectivity. Negative meaning into account in studying the cognitive pro-
affectivity or neuroticism has been correlated with cessing of war and peacekeeping experiences. As
posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms in negative appraisals have been shown to be associated
soldiers deployed to Iraq (Engelhard, Huijding, van with the development of posttraumatic stress pathol-
Downloaded By: [Schok, Michaela] At: 12:31 27 April 2010

den Hout, & de Jong, 2007). ogy (Ali, Dunmore, Clark, & Ehlers, 2002; Bryant &
The belief that one can determine one’s own Guthrie, 2005; Ehlers & Clark, 2000; O’Donnell et al.,
behaviour, influence one’s environment, and bring 2007), this study focuses on personal resources of
about desired outcomes is known to help people cope resilience in relation to these two construals of meaning
(Thompson, 1981). When people are able to perceive after war and peacekeeping experiences.
events in their environment as controllable, or regard
their coping efforts as likely to be successful (Benight et Objectives
al., 1997), their distress is lower, and their physiological
The aim of the present study was to examine whether
responses are reduced (Taylor, 2003). Moreover,
personal resources, self-esteem, optimism, and per-
gaining a feeling of control over the threatening event
ceived control, as a latent variable called ‘resilience’ are
has proven to be an important part of the adjustment
correlated with meaning-making processes after war
process (Taylor, 1983). Therefore, the belief in personal
and peacekeeping experiences, and if they protect
control may be an advantage in restoring a sense of
against posttraumatic stress reactions. Figure 1 shows
mastery after one has experienced threatening events.
the model that is examined. We examined data from a
Those who are able to maintain or restore a sense of
large sample of Dutch veterans deployed during
control cope better with the situation and experience
various war and peacekeeping operations who were
less depression and anxiety (Affleck, Tennen, &
surveyed as part of a larger mixed method study
Gershman, 1985; Taylor, Helgeson, Reed, & Skokan,
intended to explore meaning after military deployment
1991; Thompson, Sobolow-Shubin, Galbraith,
(Schok, Kleber, Elands, Lensvelt-Mulder, & Weerts,
Schwankovsky, & Cruzen, 1993). Few studies among
2006). The following hypotheses were tested using this
veterans demonstrated that an internal locus of control
data:
(perception of high personal control) was associated
with fewer posttraumatic stress symptoms compared to (1) Self-esteem, control and optimism (as personal
an external of locus of control (Frye & Stockton, 1982; resources of resilience) are predictive of lower
Solomon, Mikulincer, & Benbenishty, 1989). This perceived threat from war-zone stressors and of
indicates that perceived control lowers perceived lower levels of intrusion and avoidance.
threat, decreases posttraumatic stress reactions and (2) Self-esteem, control and optimism (as personal
diminishes the need for meaning-making of the events resources of resilience) are not predictive of
veterans encountered during deployment. finding meaning in terms of comprehensibility
We define resilience as ‘high degrees of self-esteem, and personal significance.
optimism and perceived control’. Resilience is expected (3) Perceived threat related to war-zone stressors is
to lower perceived threat and decrease posttraumatic significantly and positively associated with
stress reactions. When perceived threat is low, the need intrusion and avoidance.
to search for meaning diminishes. When perceived (4) Perceived threat related to war-zone stressors is
threat is high, attributing (positive) meaning facilitates significantly associated with meaning in terms
mastery over the situation (Baumeister, 1991; of comprehensibility and personal significance.
Festinger, 1979; Kleber & Brom, 1992). (5) Positive meaning in terms of comprehensibility
Two construals of meaning-making play indepen- and personal significance is predictive of lower
dent roles in the cognitive adaptation process when levels of intrusion and avoidance.
330 M.L. Schok et al.

Meaning as
comprehensibility

Self-esteem +

– +
Intrusion/
Control Resilience Perceived threat
avoidance

Optimism +

Meaning as
personal
significance

Time

Figure 1. Hypothesised model of resilient resources in predicting meaning-making and posttraumatic stress reactions after
military deployment.

Method ‘hearing the wounded scream’. Eight items were added:


Downloaded By: [Schok, Michaela] At: 12:31 27 April 2010

Participants ‘I shot someone’, ‘I killed someone (with a weapon)’,


‘I wounded someone (with a weapon)’, ‘removing dead
A cross-sectional study was carried out between
bodies’, ‘presence of landmines’, ‘explosions (bom-
September 2004 and December 2004 among Dutch
bardments, missile attacks, etc.)’, ‘intimidation by
veterans who were registered at the Dutch Veterans
supervisor’, and ‘not authorised to intervene between
Institute. This national institute promotes social rec-
civilians and conflicting parties’.
ognition and provides services to improve the psycho-
The final instrument consisted of 24 items where
logical, social, and physical well-being of Dutch
the respondents were asked to mark the threatening
veterans and their families. A sample of 3000 male
events they experienced during deployment and the
veterans was drawn, consisting of former military
extent to which they perceived these events to be
personnel who had been deployed during the following
threatening on a Likert-type rating scale from 1 ‘not at
war and peacekeeping operations: (1) the (Dutch)–
all’ to 5 ‘very much’. A sum score was calculated to
Indonesian Decolonisation War (1945–1949), (2) the
measure perceived threat related to the assessed
Korean War (1950–1953), (3) the Dutch–Indonesian
war-zone stressors. Internal reliability of this scale
conflict on Irian Jaya (New-Guinea, 1950–1962), (4) was high ( ¼ 0.91).
United Nations Transitional Authority in Cambodia/
United Nations Advance Mission In Cambodia,
(UNTAC/UNAMIC) (1992–1993), (5) United Impact of Event Scale
Nations Protection Force in former Yugoslavia/ The Impact of Event Scale (IES) (Dutch version;
United Nations Peace Forces (UNPROFOR/UNPF) Brom, & Kleber, 1985; Horowitz, Wilner, & Alvarez,
(1992–1995) and (6) Implementation Force/ 1979; van der Ploeg, Mooren, Kleber, van der Velden,
Stabilization Force in former Yugoslavia (IFOR/ & Brom, 2004) assessed the emotional impact of
SFOR) (1995–present). From each military operation, traumatic events on a person by looking at intrusive
500 veterans were invited to participate in this study. thoughts and behaviours (subscale intrusion) and
The response rate was 52% (n ¼ 1.561) for the total emotional numbing and avoidance (subscale avoid-
sample and ranged from 46% to 56% per military ance). Respondents were asked to indicate the fre-
operation. quency of each of 15 symptoms, related to their
deployment experiences, in the last week ranging from
0 to 5 (0 ¼ not at all, 1 ¼ rarely, 3 ¼ sometimes,
Measures 5 ¼ often). Reliability for this scale was high ( ¼ 0.94).
War-zone stressors and perceived threat
War-zone stressors were measured using the compre- Meaning of War Scale
hensive Aftercare Questionnaire of the Royal Army in The Meaning of War Scale (MoWS) (Mooren &
the Netherlands (Royal Dutch Army, 2003). This scale Kleber, 2001; Mooren, Schok, & Kleber, 2009)
of the frequently used instrument assessed threatening consisted of 22 items with explicit reference to war
events experienced during deployment. The items and was adjusted to the experience of military
included, for example, ‘being fired (upon)’, ‘being deployment. Subscales were (a) viewing the people
taken hostage’, ‘being wounded’, ‘seeing dead bodies’, and the surrounding world in terms of trust and
Aging & Mental Health 331

distrust, and detachment, (b) growing or learning from control or beliefs regarding one’s ability to control
what happened, expressed by feelings of hope and lack an event rather than being controlled by fate.
of hope and life-goals and (c) adhering to a religion. Five items are phrased in a negative way and
The subscales ‘distrust’ and ‘adhering to religion’ are two items in a positive way (negative items were
operationalisations of meaning in terms of ‘compre- reversely scored). A sample item from this scale is:
hensibility’. They reflect aspects of understanding the ‘I have little control over the things that happen to me’.
events in retrospect to create a cognitive framework to Responses were given on 5-point scale ranging from 1
explain the experience. The subscale ‘growth’ reflects (strongly agree) to 5 (strongly disagree). A total sum
meaning in terms of ‘personal significance’ which score was calculated. Higher scores on the scale
emphasises personal gain from the experience. indicate higher levels of perceived control.
Examples of items of the subscale distrust included Cronbach’s alpha for the MS in the present study
‘Since my deployment I only trust myself’ and ‘Since was 0.81.
my deployment I don’t believe in justice anymore’.
Examples of items of the subscale ‘growth’ included
‘My deployment experiences have made me a stronger Life Orientation Test
person’ and ‘My deployment experiences gave my life The Life Orientation Test (LOT) was used to assess
more sense’. Examples of items of the subscale dispositional optimism, defined as generalised optimis-
‘adhering to religion’ included ‘My deployment experi- tic outcome expectancies (Scheier & Carver, 1985) and
ences have made my belief in God stronger’, and ‘Since is a well-known and validated instrument. This version
I have been deployed I pray more’. Respondents are consists of eight items. Four items are worded
asked to indicate the extent of their agreement with the optimistically and four are worded pessimistically.
Downloaded By: [Schok, Michaela] At: 12:31 27 April 2010

items on a 4-point scale ranging from 1 (strongly An example of an item is ‘I’m always optimistic about
disagree) to 4 (strongly agree). The three subscales my future’. Respondents are asked to indicate the
showed good reliability (0.85, 0.76, and 0.85 extent of their agreement with the items on a 5-point
respectively). scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly
agree). The negative items were reversed before scoring
Benefit Finding Scale so that higher scores indicate higher levels of disposi-
The Benefit Finding Scale (BFS) (Antoni et al., 2001) tional optimism. In the present study, Cronbach’s
has 17 items, each of which expresses some potential alpha was 0.81.
benefit that might be derived from the experience and
was made specific by referring to deployment experi-
ences. This scale assesses meaning in terms of ‘personal Data analyses
significance’. Responses were made on a Likert-type
scale from 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely). The items First, descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations
assessed benefits in a variety of domains, including were calculated. Second, the hypothesised model
acceptance of life’s imperfections, becoming more (Figure 1) was translated into a statistical model and
cognisant of the role of other people in one’s life, tested using structural equation modelling (SEM) by
and developing a sense of purpose in life. Internal AMOS 16.0. Path analysis was performed to assess the
reliability of this scale was high ( ¼ 0.92). expected relationships between the observed variables
for the complete sample.
In all model analyses, maximum likelihood was
The Rosenberg Self-Esteem List
used as the method of estimation. Fit measures used
The Rosenberg Self-Esteem (RSE) List (Rosenberg, were chi-square (2), goodness of fit index (GFI)
1965) was used to measure self-esteem. This (Joreskog & Sorbom, 1996), the Bentler–Bonett
well-known and validated questionnaire consists of normed fit index (NFI) (Bentler, 1990) and the root
10 items which measure attitudes towards the self, such means square error of approximation (RMSEA)
as self-acceptance and self-worth. For example, ‘I am (Steiger, 1990). 2 evaluates the fixed rather than the
positive about myself’. Respondents are asked to free parameters in a structural model and low and
indicate the extent of their agreement with the items non-significant values are desired; GFI indicates the
on a 4-point scale ranging from 1 (strongly agree) to 4 relative amount of the observed variances and covar-
(strongly disagree). Five negatively phrased items were iances accounted for by the model; NFI presents the
reverse scored and summed to an overall self-esteem proportion in the improvement of the overall fit of the
score. Higher scores on the RSE indicate higher levels proposed model relative to a null model. RMSEA is a
of self-esteem. Cronbach’s alpha for the RSE in the measure of the model’s parsimoniousness. Adequate fit
present study was 0.83. of the model to the data is indicated by values of 0.95
or greater for GFI and NFI, and 0.05 or smaller for the
Mastery Scale RMSEA (Byrne, 2001).
The Pearlin and Schooler’s Mastery Scale (MS) Missing data were imputed in SPSS 15.0 by EM
(Pearlin & Schooler, 1978) consists of seven items (expectation-maximisation) method before analysing
that are intended to assess global beliefs of perceived the data in AMOS. This was necessary because the
332 M.L. Schok et al.

maximum-likelihood estimator needs a full matrix to Results of SEM


provide the researcher with modification indices (MI). The model as depicted in Figure 1 was tested with the
These indices were used to build the model. After the total sample. Figure 2 shows the final model. The
model fit was established, the original data file, measurement variables, self-esteem, control and opti-
including missing data, was used to estimate the mism loaded on the latent variable of resilience as
model in AMOS, where missing data were imputed modelled. The overall fit of the model was satisfactory,
using Full Information Maximum Likelihood (FIML) 2 (1, N ¼ 1.561) ¼ 81.13, p ¼ 0.00, GFI ¼ 0.98,
a method that is superior to the EM algorithm (Kline, NFI ¼ 0.98, RMSEA ¼ 0.05, with factor loadings
2005). ranging from 0.75 to 0.80 indicating that the construct
Guidelines by Cohen (1988) were followed to of resilience was well-defined.
interpret standardised path coefficients with absolute In line with our hypothesis resilience, as measured
values less than 0.10 indicating a ‘small’ effect, values by self-esteem, optimism, and control, predicted lower
around 0.30 indicating a ‘medium’ effect and coeffi- perceived threat from exposure to war-zone stressors
cients with absolute values of 0.50 suggesting a ‘large’ and lower levels of intrusions and avoidance. Effect
effect. sizes were small to medium. Surprisingly, resilience
proved to be predictive of meaning in terms of
comprehensibility as reflected in distrust ( ¼ 0.67)
and adhering to religion ( ¼ 0.19). This indicates
Results
that high self-esteem, personal control, and optimism
Descriptive statistics are associated with less distrust and alienation, as well
Table 1 shows descriptive information on the total as less adherence to religion. Effect sizes were large to
Downloaded By: [Schok, Michaela] At: 12:31 27 April 2010

sample. Participants had an average age of 57.66 years small for these associations. Resilience was also
(SD ¼ 18.49, range 24–90). On average, veterans were predictive of meaning in terms of personal significance
deployed for 18.08 months (SD ¼ 17.47, range 1–275). as reflected in growth ( ¼ 0.47) and personal benefits
The mean length of time since leaving the service was ( ¼ 0.09). Higher self-esteem, control, and optimism
23.50 years (SD ¼ 20.71). Veterans were equally were associated with more growth gained from military
deployed during war and peacekeeping operations deployment and more personal benefits. Although
and most of them were married or living with a growth and personal benefits correlated highly, indi-
partner (80.8%; 1262). Most veterans had a low or cating overlap, the effect sizes differed. For resilience
middle level of education (72.9%; 1138). More than versus growth the effect size was large and for
half of the veterans served with the Land Forces resilience versus personal benefits, the effect size was
(65.2%; 1018), most veterans were working either as small.
professionals (40.3%; 629) or were deployed as con- Perceived threat related to war-zone stressors
script (27.7%; 432) and upon leaving the service most predicted higher levels of intrusion and avoidance
of them had a low rank (51.1%; 797) such as private or ( ¼ 0.29). The effect size indicated a medium effect.
corporal. Perceived threat was also predictive of meaning in
Veterans reported on average 7.55 war-zone terms of comprehensibility as reflected in distrust
stressors (SD ¼ 18.49, range 0–24). War-zone stressors ( ¼ 0.15) and adhering to religion ( ¼ 0.09). This
that were most reported by participants were shootings indicates that higher perceived threat relates to viewing
(70.9%; 1107), seeing human suffering (70.5%; 1100), people and the world more as distrustful, as well as less
presence of landmines (61.9%; 967), seeing dead bodies adherence to religion, although effect sizes were small.
(51.2%; 799) and seeing severely wounded people Furthermore, perceived threat was also predictive of
(50.5%; 788). meaning in terms of personal significance as reflected
Table 2 displays the correlations among the in personal benefits ( ¼ 0.16), but not for growth.
variables included in the model, along with means Higher perceived threat was associated with more
and standard deviations. Correlations ranged from personal benefits, although the effect size was small.
0.01 to 0.61 and almost all were significant. The highest Finally, meaning in terms of comprehensibility as
associations were found between self-esteem and opti- reflected in distrust predicted higher levels of intrusions
mism, self-esteem and control, and control and opti- and avoidance ( ¼ 0.25). This effect size indicated a
mism, intrusion/avoidance and distrust, personal medium effect. Adhering to a religion was not
benefits and growth and intrusion/avoidance and associated with intrusive and avoidant thoughts and
perceived threat. No significant associations were behaviours. Surprisingly, meaning as personal signifi-
found between self-esteem and personal benefits, cance as reflected in finding personal benefits predicted
control and personal benefits, perceived threat and higher levels of intrusions and avoidance ( ¼ 0.11),
growth, and perceived threat and religion. although the effect size was small. Growth was not
Furthermore, growth and personal benefits showed predictive of posttraumatic stress responses.
some significant overlap. All significant associations Overall, the model explained 39% of the variance
were in the expected direction, except for personal for intrusion/avoidance, 39% of the variance in
benefits versus intrusion/avoidance. distrust, 40% of the variance in growth, 15% of the
Aging & Mental Health 333

Table 1. Descriptives of total sample (n ¼ 1.561). variance in religion, and 3% of the variance in personal
benefits. The direct and indirect effects are presented in
Total sample (n ¼ 1.561) Table 3 and support the notion that direct effects were
Descriptives M SD small to large and indirect effects were rather small in
the model.
Age (years) 57.66 18.49
Duration of deployment 18.08 17.47
(months)
Time since leaving the military 23.50 20.71 Discussion
service (years) The present study draws attention to the need for a
Frequency of war-zone 7.55 5.44
stressors more comprehensive model of cognitive adaptation to
n % threatening events including resilient resources and
meaning-making processes. Resilience decreased the
Deployment perception of threat, and more strongly, minimised
War 731 46.8
Peacekeeping 808 51.8
intrusions and avoidance resulting from war-zone
Married/living together 1262 80.8 experiences according to our model. More specifically,
Education higher resilience predicted less distrust in others and
Low 660 42.3 the world and more personal growth. Personal
Middle 478 30.6 resources diminished veterans’ appraisals of danger in
High 295 18.9
Other 109 7.0 the war zone after deployment, although the nature of
Frequency of deployment this relationship needs further investigation.
1 954 61.1 Although the association between resilience and
Downloaded By: [Schok, Michaela] At: 12:31 27 April 2010

41 558 35.7 perceived threat was rather low, it was in the expected
Service direction. A stronger association was found between
Land forces 1018 65.2
Navy (incl. marines) 237 15.2 resilience and intrusions and avoidance, indicating that
Air force 44 2.8 self-esteem, control and optimism lower stress
Military police 29 1.9 responses resulting from deployment experiences.
Combination 182 12.1 More specifically, higher resilience was strongly
Status associated with perceiving a lower emotional impact
Conscripts 432 27.7
Professional 629 40.3 of war and peacekeeping experiences. As intrusive and
Voluntary 238 15.2 avoidant thoughts can be considered as attempts to
Combination 212 13.6 understand traumatic events and as markers of cogni-
Rank leaving the service tive processing, it appears that high self-esteem, per-
Low 797 51.1
sonal control and optimism diminish this need to
High 640 41.0
War-zone stressors (most 70.9 understand the experience. They may represent pro-
reported) tective resources against intrusive thoughts and beha-
Shootings (not aimed) 1107 70.9 viours and emotional numbing and avoidance.
Witnessing human suffering 1100 70.5 The strong association between resilience and the
Presence of landmines 967 61.9
Witnessing death bodies 799 51.2
two construals of meaning (comprehensibility and
Witnessing severely wounded 788 50.5 personal significance) came as a surprise. High
persons self-esteem, personal control, and a positive outlook
were shown to be negatively correlated to distrust,
Note: Due to missing data, sample sizes for descriptives vary.
indicating that these personal resources lower the

Table 2. Bivariate correlations (n ¼ 1.561).

Variables 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

1. Self-esteem (RSE) –
2. Control (MS) 0.57** –
3. Optimism (LOT) 0.61** 0.59** –
4. Perceived threat 0.08** 0.10** 0.10** –
5. Distrust (MoWS) 0.36** 0.46** 0.49** 0.23** –
6. Growth (MoWS) 0.40** 0.33** 0.40** 0.04 0.08** –
7. Religion (MoWS) 0.14** 0.18** 0.07** –.02 0.10** 0.13** –
8. Personal benefits (BFS) 0.03 0.01 0.10** 0.15** 0.07** 0.43** 0.33** –
9. Intrusion/avoidance (IES) 0.33** 0.39** 0.37** 0.40** 0.49** 0.08** 0.13** 0.15**
M 33.28 25.77 29.11 23.91 25.85 22.52 5.88 48.85 12.78
SD 4.41 4.89 5.05 20.33 5.19 2.50 2.12 12.77 15.45

Note: **p 5 0.01.


334 M.L. Schok et al.

Meaning as comprehensibility

Distrust

0.15***
0.23*** 0.25***
Religion
–0.67***
0.76*** –0.19***
Self-esteem
–0.09**

0.75*** 0.08**
–0.11*** 0.29*** Intrusion/
Control Resilience Perceived threat avoidance
0.36***
0.80***
Meaning as personal significance
Optimism 0.47*** 0.11***

0.16*** Growth
0.08**
–0.30
0.37***

Personal benefits
Downloaded By: [Schok, Michaela] At: 12:31 27 April 2010

Time

Figure 2. Final model of personal resources of resilience in predicting meaning-making and posttraumatic stress reactions after
military deployment (n ¼ 1.561). Model: 2(1, N ¼ 1.561) ¼ 81.13, p ¼ 0.00, GFI ¼ 0.98, NFI ¼ 0.98, RMSEA ¼ 0.05, **p 5 0.01,
***p 5 0.001.

Table 3. Standardised direct and indirect effects between variables (n ¼ 1.561).

Effects

Outcome Predictor Direct Indirect Total

Resilience Self-esteem 0.76*** – 0.76***


Control 0.75*** – 0.75***
Optimism 0.80*** – 0.80***
Perceived threat Resilience 0.11*** – 0.11***
Distrust Resilience 0.67*** 0.10*** 0.57***
Perceived threat 0.15*** 0.01 0.16***
Growth 0.23*** – 0.23***
Religion – – 0.02
Personal benefits – 0.09*** 0.09***
Growth Resilience 0.47*** 0.01 0.49***
Perceived threat – 0.05** 0.05**
Distrust – – –
Religion 0.08** – 0.08**
Personal benefits 0.37*** 0.03 0.40***
Religion Resilience 0.19*** 0.03 0.16***
Perceived threat 0.09** 0.06 0.03
Distrust – – –
Personal benefits 0.36*** – 0.36***
Personal benefits Resilience 0.08** 0.02 0.07**
Perceived threat 0.16*** – 0.16***
Distrust – – –
Growth – – –
Religion – – –
Intrusion/avoidance Resilience 0.30*** 0.16*** 0.46***
Perceived threat 0.29*** 0.06 0.35***
Distrust 0.25*** – 0.25***
Growth – 0.06** 0.06**
Religion – 0.01 0.01
Personal benefits 0.11*** 0.02 0.14***

Note: **p 5 0.01, ***p 5 0.001.


Aging & Mental Health 335

perception of a negative world view. Resilient medium effect size as well as distrust and growth. This
resources appear to increase the perception of a indicates that both construals of meaning are not
meaningful world by enhancing feelings of trust and entirely distinct phases in time within the
safety (Janoff-Bulman & Frantz, 1997). However, it meaning-making process.
may also be true that resilience prevents veterans from Furthermore, distrust and personal benefits were
becoming distrustful after exposure to stressors in the associated with intrusion and avoidance (see also
war zone. Self-esteem, personal control and optimism Schok, Kleber, Lensvelt-Mulders, Elands, & Weerts,
foster emotional stability and may, therefore, decrease accepted). This finding supports the notion that
the need for comprehensibility of what happened and successful processing of threatening experiences is
protect veterans against the shattering of their beliefs accomplished when beliefs about self, others and the
during stressful times (Bonanno, 2004). On the other world are related to inner safety and trust
hand, distrust may also lead to lower self-esteem, less (Janoff-Bulman, 1992) and when they facilitate con-
personal control, and more pessimism. Due to the nectedness with others and the world (Herman, 1992).
cross-sectional nature of the study, no conclusions can Moreover, although the effect was small, finding
be drawn about the causal relations among variables. personal benefits predicted higher levels of intrusion
Moreover, resilient resources also predicted finding and avoidance. Intrusive and avoidant thoughts may
personal significance as reflected in personal growth be considered as attempts to understand traumatic
from military deployment. Higher self-esteem, personal events and markers of cognitive processing (Helgeson
control and optimism proved to be strongly associated et al., 2006; Horowitz, 1997; Kleber & Brom, 1992).
with having learned from and feeling stronger through This implies that veterans in our sample are working
the military experiences they encountered. Therefore, through the implications of their deployment experi-
Downloaded By: [Schok, Michaela] At: 12:31 27 April 2010

higher resilience may support reflection and awareness ences for their lives, and those implications are
of what the experience of deployment brought them in disturbing, but they may facilitate cognitive growth
terms of personals skills, relationships and life philos- at the same time. These experiences have to be
ophy. The final model showed that highly resilient assimilated into a coherent narrative and become
veterans benefit most from military deployment. Britt, part of an internalised and evolving story of the self
Adler, and Bartone (2001) also found that personal (McAdams, 2001; Neimeyer, 2006). As noted by
resources were related to the tendency to find meaning Neimeyer (2006), this narrative ‘. . . offers a meaningful
in work during deployment among Bosnian peace- explanation for identity-transforming life events, cap-
keepers. Soldiers, who were committed to life tasks, felt turing the richness of lived experience’ (p. 142).
in control and saw events as challenging, identified The concept of resilience may also include social
more with their peacekeeper role, believed their con- resources that support bouncing back from adversity
tribution was important and were personally engaged strengthened and more resourceful. For example, lack
in the mission. of social support has been one of the strongest
Resilience as measured by self-esteem, personal predictors of PTSD, especially in military samples
control and optimism may be seen as the opposite of (Brewin, Andrews, & Valentine, 2000). Moreover, a
negative affectivity (Watson & Clark, 1984). Negative negative homecoming reception was associated with
affectivity has been shown to have adverse effects on posttraumatic stress symptoms (Bolton, Litz, Glenn,
mental health (Engelhard et al., 2007; Taylor, 2003). In Orsillo, & Roemer, 2002; Frye & Stockton, 1982).
contrast, self-esteem, optimism, and control foster the Bolton et al. (2002) suggested that homecoming
tendency to experience positive emotional states which reception is relevant to a soldier’s attempt to legitimise
prompt individuals to pursue novel and creative and validate his or her actions and sacrifices made
thoughts and actions (Frederickson, 2001). Positive during a mission. Veterans in our sample who were
emotions tend to broaden one’s thoughts and action deployed during successful missions abroad may
repertoire; negative emotions narrow them. Highly receive more societal approval for their effort, like
resilient people tend to experience positive emotions for example the mission to Cambodia. In contrast, the
during times of stress (Tugade & Frederickson, 2004). military operation to former Yugoslavia in Srebrenica
Re-creating a meaningful world after trauma may well ended with an enormous tragedy. Veterans received
be served by using positive emotions in the regulation little social recognition for their military effort and
of overwhelming negative emotions (Folkman, 2008; were blamed for their lack of military performance to
Folkman & Moskowitz, 2000). This may explain how protect the local people. Less societal approval will
self-esteem, personal control, and optimism lower make the process of meaning-making more difficult
stress responses and foster assigning positive meaning after homecoming. Besides personal resources,
to threatening experiences. research on resilience should also focus on social
Both construals of meaning have been included in resources that support cognitive adaptation to stressful
the model based on the assumption that they present events after military deployment.
different phases in cognitive adjustment after military Several limitations of the present study should be
deployment. However, in the final model, associations considered. First, the study was cross-sectional.
were found between the two constructs of meaning. This design limits the conclusions that can be drawn
For example, religion and personal benefits showed a with regard to causality amongst the variables.
336 M.L. Schok et al.

For example, in the model we expected that high benefit finding among women under treatment for
self-esteem, control and optimism lead to positive early-stage breast cancer. Health Psychology, 20, 20–32.
meaning-making. However, being able to make posi- Baumeister, R.F. (1991). Meanings of life. New York:
tive meaning out of adversity may also improve Guilford.
Benight, C.C., Antoni, M.H., Kilbourn, K., Ironson, G.,
self-esteem, mastery and a positive outlook. In addi-
Kumar, M.A., Fletcher, M.A., et al. (1997). Coping self-
tion, effects may also occur in both directions efficacy buffers psychological and physiological distur-
indicating a feedback loop. The exact nature of bances in HIV-infected men following a natural disaster.
causality between resilient resources and Health Psychology, 16, 248–255.
meaning-making has to be addressed by the use of a Bentler, P.M. (1990). Fit indexes, LaGrange multipliers,
longitudinal design. Another point for consideration is constraint changes and incomplete data in structural
the retrospective nature of this study. Self-report models. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 25, 163–172.
measurement of exposure has been shown to be Bolton, E.E., Litz, B.T., Glenn, D.M., Orsillo, S., &
inconsistent over time (Bramsen, Dirkzwager, Esch, Roemer, L. (2002). The impact of homecoming reception
& Ploeg, 2001). This may be because posttraumatic on the adaptation of peacekeepers following deployment.
Military Psychology, 14, 241–251.
stress responses may alter a person’s interpretation of
Bonanno, G.A. (2004). Loss, trauma, and human resilience.
the events. Intrusions and avoidance are indicative of Have we underestimated the capacity to thrive after
cognitive processing and a signal that a person is trying extremely aversive events? American Psychologists, 59,
to integrate the experience into existing meaning 20–28.
structures of reality. Alterations in a person’s apprai- Bonanno, G.A., Field, N.P., Kovacevic, A., & Kaltman, S.
sals of the events may therefore be normal in the (2002). Self-enhancement as a buffer against extreme
process of mental adaptation. adversity: Civil war in Bosnia and traumatic loss in the
Downloaded By: [Schok, Michaela] At: 12:31 27 April 2010

If longitudinal research confirms the positive United States. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
relations between resilience and meaning as measured 28, 184–196.
in our model, personal resources, such as self-esteem, Bonanno, G.A., Rennicke, C., & Dekel, S. (2005). Self-
enhancement among high-exposure survivors of the
personal control and optimism should be reinforced in
September 11th terrorist attack: Resilience or social
people who are exposed to threatening events.
maladjustment? Journal of Personality and Social
Veterans who are not functioning well after military Psychology, 88, 984–998.
deployment may benefit greatly from self-enhancing Bramsen, I., Dirkzwager, A.J.E., van Esch, S.C.M., &
interventions. By enhancing their self-esteem and van der Ploeg, H.M. (2001). Consistency of self-reports
optimistic outlook on life and increasing their personal of traumatic events in a population of Dutch peacekeepers:
control, they are better able to process and make sense Reason for optimism? Journal of Traumatic Stress, 14,
of their war-zone experiences. As most veterans adapt 733–740.
well after military deployment experiences and only a Brewin, C.R., Andrews, B., & Valentine, J.D. (2000). Meta-
minority develops actual PTSD (Engelhard, van den analysis of risk factors for posttraumatic stress disorder in
trauma-exposed adults. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Hout, Weerts, Arntz, Hox, & McNally, 2007), resilient
Psychology, 68, 748–766.
veterans are an important avenue for future research
Britt, T.W., Adler, A.B., & Bartone, P.T. (2001). Deriving
and should serve as an example in clinical practice. benefits from stressful events: The role of engagement in
Their stories can inform future veterans about what to meaningful work and hardiness. Journal of Occupational
expect of military deployment, how to handle difficult Health Psychology, 1, 53–63.
situations, and they can provide instrumental and Brom, D., & Kleber, R.J. (1985). De Schokverwerkingslijst.
emotional support on mental adaptation upon return- [The Impact of Event Scale]. Nederlands Tijdschrift voor de
ing home. Psychologie, 40, 164–168.
Bryant, R.A., & Guthrie, R.M. (2005). Maladaptive apprai-
sals as a risk factor for posttraumatic stress. Psychological
References Science, 16, 749–752.
Byrne, B.M. (2001). Structural equation modelling with
Affleck, G., Tennen, H., & Gershman, K. (1985). Cognitive AMOS: Basic concepts, applications, and programming.
adaptations to high-risk infants: The search for mastery, Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
meaning, and protection from future harm. American Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the social
Journal of Mental Deficiency, 89, 653–656. sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ali, T., Dunmore, E., Clark, D., & Ehlers, A. (2002). The Davis, C.G., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Larson, J. (1998).
role of negative beliefs in posttraumatic stress disorder: Making sense of loss and benefiting from the experience:
A comparison of assault victims and non victims. Two construals of meaning. Journal of Personality and
Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 30, 249–257. Social Psychology, 75, 561–574.
Andersson, G. (1996). The benefits of optimism: Ehlers, A., & Clark, D.M. (2000). A cognitive model of
A meta-analytic review of the Life Orientation Test. posttraumatic stress disorder. Behaviour Research and
Personality and Individual Differences, 21, 719–725. Therapy, 38, 319–345.
Antoni, M.H., Lehman, J.M., Kilbourn, K.M., Boyers, A.E., Engelhard, I.M., Huijding, J., van den Hout, M.A., & de Jong,
Culver, J.L., Alferi, S.M., et al. (2001). P.J. (2007). Vulnerability associations and symptoms of
Cognitive-behavioral stress management intervention post-traumatic stress disorder in soldiers deployed to Iraq.
decreases the prevalence of depression and enhances Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 1415–1424.
Aging & Mental Health 337

Engelhard, I.M., van den Hout, M.A., Weerts, J., Arntz, A., betekenisgeving na oorlogs-en geweldservaringen
Hox, J.J.C.M., & McNally, R.J. (2007). Deployment- [Making sense of threatening events: A questionnaire on
related stress and trauma in Dutch soldiers returning from meaning after war and violence]. Psychologie &
Iraq. British Journal of Psychiatry, 191, 140–145. Gezondheid, 39, 101–109.
Festinger, L. (1979). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Neimeyer, R.A. (2006). Complicated grief and the recon-
Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. struction of meaning. Clinical Psychology: Science and
Folkman, S. (2008). The case for positive emotions in the Practice, 13, 141–145.
stress process. Anxiety, Stress and Coping, 21, 3–14. O’Donnell, M.L., Elliott, P., Jones Wolfgang, B., & Creamer,
Folkman, S., & Moskowitz, J.T. (2000). Positive affect and the M. (2007). Posttraumatic appraisals in the development
other side of coping. American Psychologist, 55, 647–654. and persistence of posttraumatic stress symptoms. Journal
Fredrickson, B.L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in of Traumatic Stress, 20, 173–182.
positive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of Pearlin, L.I., & Schooler, C. (1978). The structure of coping.
positive emotions. American Psychologist: Special Issue, Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 19, 2–21.
56, 218–226. Poulin, M., & Cohen Silver, R. (2008). World benevolence
Frye, J., & Stockton, R.A. (1982). Discriminant analysis of beliefs and well-being across the life span. Psychology and
post-traumatic stress disorder among a group of Vietnam Aging, 23, 13–23.
veterans. American Journal of Psychiatry, 139, 52–56. Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image.
Helgeson, V.S., Reynolds, K.A., & Tomich, P.L. (2006). A Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
meta-analytic review of benefit finding and growth. Royal Dutch Army (2003). Questionnaire Aftercare – Royal
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74, 797–816. Army. Amersfoort (the Netherlands): Division Individual
Herman, L.J. (1992). Trauma and recovery. New York: Basic Care & Division Behavioural Sciences.
Books. Scheier, M.F., & Carver, C.S. (1985). Optimism,
Horowitz, M.J. (1997). Stress response syndrome. coping and health: Assessment and implications for
Downloaded By: [Schok, Michaela] At: 12:31 27 April 2010

San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. generalized outcome expectancies. Health Psychology, 4,


Horowitz, M.J., Wilner, N., & Alvarez, W. (1979). Impact of 219–247.
Event Scale: A study of subjective stress. Psychosomatic Schok, M.L., Kleber, R.J., Elands, M., Lensvelt-Mulders, G.
Medicine, 41, 209–218. & Weerts, M.P. (2006). Suspicious minds at risk:
Janoff-Bulman, R. (1992). Shattered assumptions: Towards a The role of meaning in the processing of war and
new psychology of trauma. New York: The Free Press. peacekeeping experiences. In The International
Janoff-Bulman, R., & Frantz, C.M. (1997). The impact of Society for Traumatic Stress Studies 22nd Annual
trauma on meaning: From meaningless world to mean- Meeting Final Program and Proceedings, The
ingful life. In M. Power & C.R. Brewin (Eds.), The Psychobiology of Trauma and Resilience across The Life
transformation of meaning in psychological therapies Span (p. 88). Northbrook: The International Society for
(pp. 91–106). Chichester, England: Wiley. Traumatic Stress Studies.
Janoff-Bulman, R., & Yopyk, D.J. (2004). Random out- Schok, M.L., Kleber, R.J., Elands, M., & Weerts, J.M.P.
comes and valued commitments. In J. Greenberg, (2008). Meaning as a mission: A review of empirical
S.L. Koole, & T. Psyszczynski (Eds.), Handbook of studies on appraisals of war and peacekeeping experiences.
experimental existential psychology (pp. 122–138). Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 357–365.
New York: The Guilford Press. Schok, M.L., Kleber, R.J., Lensvelt-Mulders, G.J.L.M.,
Joreskog, K.G., & Sorbom, D. (1996). LISREL 8: Structural Elands, M., & Weerts, J. Suspicious minds at risk? The
equation modeling with the SIMPLIS command language. role of meaning in processing war and peacekeeping
Chicago, IL: Scientific Software International. experiences. Journal of Applied Social Psychology
Joseph, S., & Linley, P.A. (2005). Positive adjustment to (accepted).
threatening events: An organismic valuing theory of Solberg Nes, L., & Segerstrom, S.C. (2006). Dispositional
growth through adversity. Review of General Psychology, optimism and coping: A meta-analytic review. Personality
9, 262–280. and Social Psychology Review, 3, 235–251.
Kleber, R.J., & Brom, D. (1992). Coping with trauma: Solomon, Z., Mikulincer, M., & Benbenishty, R. (1989).
Theory, prevention and treatment. Amsterdam/Lisse: Swets Locus of control and combat related post-traumatic stress
& Zeitlinger B.V. disorder: The intervening role of battle intensity, threat
Kline, R.B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural appraisal and coping. British Journal of Clinical
equation modeling (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. Psychology, 28, 131–144.
Major, B., Richards, C., Cooper, M.L., Cozzarelli, C., & Steiger, J.H. (1990). Structural model evaluation and
Zubek, J. (1998). Personal resilience, cognitive appraisals, modification: An interval estimation approach.
and coping: An integrative model of adjustment to Multivariate Behavioral Research, 25, 173–180.
abortion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Taylor, S.E. (1983). Adjustment to threatening events. A
74, 735–752. theory of cognitive adaptation. American Psychologist, 38,
Masten, A.S. (2001). Ordinary magic: Resilience processes in 1161–1173.
development. American Psychologist, 56, 227–238. Taylor, S.E. (2003). Health psychology. New York:
McAdams, D.P. (2001). The psychology of life stories. McGraw-Hill.
Review of General Psychology, 5, 100–122. Taylor, S.E., Helgeson, V.S., Reed, G.M., & Skokan, L.A.
Mooren, G.T.M., & Kleber, R.J. (2001). The impact of war (1991). Self-generated feelings of control and adjustment
in Bosnia and Hercegovina: Devastated trust. International to physical illness. Journal of Social Issues, 47, 91–109.
Journal of Mental Health, 30, 6–21. Tedeschi, R.G., & Calhoun, L.G. (1996). The posttraumatic
Mooren, T., Schok, M., & Kleber, R.J. (2009). De zin van growth inventory: Measuring the positive legacy of
ingrijpende gebeurtenissen: Een vragenlijst over trauma. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 9, 455–471.
338 M.L. Schok et al.

Thompson, S.C. (1981). Will it hurt less if I can control it? Walsh, F. (2006). Strengthening family resilience (2nd ed.).
A complex answer to a simple question. Psychological New York: The Guilford Press.
Bulletin, 90, 89–101. Watson, D., & Clark, L.A. (1984). Negative affectivity: The
Thompson, S.C., Sobolew-Shubin, A., Galbraith, M.E., disposition to experience aversive emotional states.
Schwankovsky, L., & Cruzen, D. (1993). Maintaining Psychological Bulletin, 96, 465–490.
perceptions of control: Finding perceived control in low- Westphal, M., & Bonanno, G.A. (2007). Posttraumatic
control circumstances. Journal of Personality and Social growth and resilience to trauma: Different sides of the
Psychology, 64, 243–304. same coin or different coins? Applied Psychology: An
Tugade, M.M., & Frederickson, B.L. (2004). Resilient International Review, 56, 417–427.
individuals use positive emotions to bounce back from Wong, P.T.P., & Weiner, B. (1981). When people ask
negative emotional experiences. Journal of Personality and ‘why’ questions and the heuristics of attributional search.
Social Psychology, 2, 320–333. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 650–663.
van der Ploeg, E., Mooren, G.T.T., Kleber, R.J., van der Zoellner, T., & Maercker, A. (2006). Posttraumatic growth in
Velden, P.G., & Brom, D. (2004). Construct validation of clinical psychology – A critical review and introduction of
the Dutch version of the impact of event scale. a two component model. Clinical Psychology Review, 26,
Psychological Assessment, 16, 16–26. 626–653.
Downloaded By: [Schok, Michaela] At: 12:31 27 April 2010

You might also like