You are on page 1of 14

Award for

Distinguished Professional
Contributions to Independent
or Institutional Practice in
the Private Sector
This award recognizes outstanding service delivery by a
licensed psychologist who is primarily engaged in the practice

2006
of psychology in a private sector setting. The award is intended
to recognize outstanding practitioners in psychology.
Nominations are considered for psychologists working in any
area of clinical specialization, health services provision, or
consulting, and services provided to any patient population or
professional clientele in an independent or institutional practice

AWARD
setting are considered. Contributions can be judged
distinguished by virtue of peer recognition, advancement of the
public’s recognition of psychology as a profession, relevant
professional association honors, or other meritorious

winners accomplishments denoting excellence as a practitioner,


including advancement of the profession. In previous years,
this award was called the Award for Distinguished
Contributions to Applied Psychology as a Professional Practice.
The name was changed in 2003 to better describe this award.

1972 Carl R. Rogers 1989 Patrick H. DeLeon


1973 David Wechsler Florence W. Kaslow
1974 Noble H. Kelley 1990 George Stricker
1975 George W. Albee 1991 Stanley R. Graham
1976 John C. Flanagan 1992 Herbert Freudenberger
David Shakow 1993 Charles D. Spielberger
1977 George Katona 1994 Robert D. Weitz
1978 Arthur L. Benton 1995 Patricia M. Bricklin
Distinguished Professional 1979 Herbert Dörken 1996 Christine A. Courtois
Contributions 1980 Zygmunt A. Piotrowski Robert J. Resnick
Douglas W. Bray 1997 Jack G. Wiggins Jr.
1981 Jack I. Bardon 1998/
The 2006 recipients of the
APA Professional Contribution Awards 1982 Milton Theaman 1999 Dorothy W. Cantor
were selected by the 2005 Board of Professional 1983 Donald R. Peterson 2000 Mathilda B. Canter
Affairs (BPA). Members of the 2005 BPA were 1984 Nicholas A. Cummings 2001 Reuben J. Silver
Rosie Phillips Bingham, PhD (Chair); 1985 Rogers Wright 2002 William R. Safarjan
Kristin A. Hancock, PhD; Keith N. Humphreys, PhD;
1986 Wilbert E. Fordyce 2003 Philip G. Levendusky
Larry C. James, PhD; Jennifer F. Kelly, PhD;
Richard J. Klimoski, PhD; Annette M. La Greca, PhD; 1987 Carl Zimet 2004 Miki Paul
Barbara T. Roberts, PhD; and Peter L. Sheras PhD. Alfred Wellner 2005 Edward A. Wise
1988 Stanley Moldawsky 2006 Jeffrey J. Magnavita
George D. Goldman
to be able to experience the contrast of living in the city of
Philadelphia and visiting his grandparents’ farm in the
country on weekends. These weekend visits gave him a
profound appreciation for the physical labor required to
manage a farm and for the beauty of the natural world.
It was during World War II that his parents were mar-
ried, and soon thereafter his father was drafted. He fought
in the Battle of the Bulge and received the Purple Heart
after being hit with shrapnel above the eye. When the war
ended, just like many GIs returning from World War II
and their wives, the couple was eager to make up for lost
time and start a family. Magnavita was the middle of three
boys, part of the baby boom generation that followed the
war. Growing up in a combined Italian and Germanic fam-
ily offered a rich cultural experience. The large, extended
family gave him the opportunity to observe the complexi-
ties of the relational world and to develop an interest in the
variations of personality styles.
Inspired by courses in the social and biological sciences
in high school, Magnavita attended Temple University and
majored in psychology. He was initially attracted to the
Jeffrey J. Magnavita idea of becoming an experimental psychologist, as he was
Award for Distinguished Professional Contributions captivated by the empiricism of learning theory and en-
to Independent or Institutional Practice in the joyed conducting conditioning experiments with rats. He
Private Sector became interested in clinical work when he experienced the
thrill of immersion in counseling and clinical psychology
Citation labs and experiential training groups. His enthusiasm was
further stirred when he read Joseph F. Rychlak’s pioneer-
“For his passionate devotion to the advancement of the ing work on personality, psychopathology, and psychother-
practice and science of psychotherapy. As a full-time clini- apy. Rychlak tackled the challenging conundrum of at-
cian and affiliate professor, Jeffrey J. Magnavita has pub- tempting to make sense out of the disparate approaches to
lished numerous volumes and articles on the treatment of clinical science. It was not until many decades after this
personality disorders and the practice of intensive psy- introduction to Rychlak’s seminal texts, Introduction to
chotherapy. His major accomplishment is his theoretical Personality and Psychopathology (1973) and A Philosophy
modeling of integrative and unified approaches to psy- of Science for Personality Theory (1968), that Magnavita
chotherapy using audiovisual analysis. His most recent understood the enormity of the theoretical and conceptual
achievement is a unified relational approach for patients challenges that were tackled in these groundbreaking texts.
with personality dysfunction and complex clinical syn- Over two decades later, when writing his volume Theories
dromes. He is currently undertaking the development of of Personality: Contemporary Approaches to the Science of
personality systematics, which seeks to identify the unify- Personality (Magnavita, 2002b), he found his original text-
ing processes and principles of psychotherapy, psychopa- books from his undergraduate years and reread them with a
thology, and personality theory.” new level of understanding.
After graduating from Temple University, he pursued
Biography graduate studies in counseling and psychology at Villanova
Jeffrey J. Magnavita was born in Philadelphia, Pennsylva- University, where he was awarded an assistantship and was
nia, in 1953. His parents were first-generation immigrants: assigned to overseeing the audiovisual needs of his depart-
His paternal grandparents left Italy in the early 1900s and ment. After graduating with a master’s degree, he was ac-
settled in Philadelphia, and his maternal grandparents emi- cepted to a one-year post-master’s-degree training program
grated during the same period from Eastern Europe, from a in child therapy at the Irving Schwartz Institute, a division
region then called Transylvania. His paternal grandfather of the Philadelphia Psychiatric Center. The Irving Schwartz
opened a barbershop, where Magnavita’s father and uncle Institute primarily treated inner-city children from single-
also worked. His maternal grandparents’ dream was to own parent families. It was here that Magnavita developed a
a farm, to which end they purchased 40 acres in Bucks keen appreciation for the strong influence that dysfunc-
County, Pennsylvania. As a child, Magnavita was fortunate tional social and community systems exert on families,

880 November 2006 ● American Psychologist


Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0003-066X/06/$12.00
Vol. 61, No. 8, 880 – 892 DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.61.8.880
which results in the development of psychopathology in using the conceptualization of triangular configurations de-
young children. During his tenure, he received intensive veloped by David Malan.
training in the traditional child guidance approach, which His next book required that he expand his theoretical
entailed the child being seen in play psychotherapy and the system; incorporating systemic and Bowenian theory, he
mother being seen by a separate therapist for supportive published Relational Therapy for Personality Disorders
treatment. (Magnavita, 2000b), which was featured in the Behavioral
At the Philadelphia Psychiatric Center, Magnavita was Science Book Club. In this volume, he presented a typol-
able to attend intellectually invigorating and clinically chal- ogy of dysfunctional families, which he considered to be
lenging seminars with doctoral-level interns, which reaf- the first step toward creating a relational diagnostic system
firmed his desire to pursue a doctorate himself. He applied for personality dysfunction. Under the editorship of Florence
to a number of programs, and after serving six months of Kaslow, he was the volume editor for the Comprehensive
his training program, he was accepted into a doctoral pro- Handbook of Psychotherapy: Vol. 1. Psychodynamic/Object
gram at the University of Connecticut. With the generous Relations (Magnavita, 2002a) and was the editor for the
support and encouragement of Irving Schulman, the direc- Handbook of Personality Disorders: Theory and Practice (see
tor of the Philadelphia Psychiatric Center and Magnavita’s Magnavita, 2004a, 2004b, 2004c), which was the main selec-
primary supervisor, he was encouraged to take advantage tion for a month in the Behavioral Science Book Club.
of his opportunity, and so he left the Philadelphia Psychiat- The prevalence of individuals and families in his prac-
ric Center. tice suffering from personality dysfunction sparked his in-
At the University of Connecticut, he was awarded a terest in developing effective treatment strategies. On the
graduate assistantship, which included coteaching courses basis of extensive audiovisual reviews of hundreds of hours
and making field visits to master’s-level students in practi- of psychotherapy and a review of major systems of psy-
cum settings. His program permitted considerable flexibil- chotherapy, he began to evolve an increasingly unified the-
ity, with a combination of studies including counseling, oretical framework for treating personality dysfunction and
school, and clinical psychology. In lieu of a foreign lan- related clinical syndromes with a multimodal psychother-
guage requirement, he was able to substitute a specializa- apy approach. Influenced by the theoretical work of Theo-
dore Millon, he began to emphasize the centrality of the
tion in family therapy. These studies confirmed his interest
personality system as a way of organizing the complexity
in systems theory. After completing his course work, he
of human functioning and explaining maladaptive pro-
was accepted as an intern at an APA-approved program in
cesses, calling this personality systematics. He presented
clinical psychology at Elmcrest Psychiatric Institute in
his evolving theoretical framework in his latest volume,
Portland, Connecticut. While serving as an intern, he en- Personality-Guided Relational Psychotherapy: A Unified
tered a two-year family therapy training program, which Approach (Magnavita, 2005a). Recently, Magnavita has
emphasized audiovisual recording of family psychotherapy been pursuing his interest in identifying the overarching
sessions of the families of hospitalized patients. He was unifying principles of psychotherapy; he takes the view
awarded a certificate of completion and became a licensed that the field has evolved from a single-school approach
marriage and family therapist. After completing his intern- to an integrative approach and predicts that the next stages
ship, he served as a staff psychologist in an inpatient ado- will be approaching unification. Most recently, he collaborated
lescent unit, where he was supervised for his postdoctoral with Jack Anchin on developing a unification paradigm. To-
year. He received his license as a psychologist in 1982. He gether, they coedited a special issue of the Journal of Psy-
served two more years in inpatient psychiatry; in his final chotherapy Integration on the topic of unification.
year, he served as program director for a newly established Magnavita is an affiliate professor in clinical psychology
child unit. at the University of Hartford, where he teaches courses in
Magnavita left the hospital in 1984 to open a group psychotherapy and serves on dissertation committees. He is
practice offering multimodal treatment in Glastonbury, a fellow of APA and recently completed an APA video
Connecticut. He also began work as a consultant for an series on treating personality disorders (Magnavita & Carl-
inpatient unit at Manchester Memorial Hospital, where he son, 2006). He serves on the editorial board of Clinical
served as a team consultant and psychodiagnostician. Psychology/In Session: Psychotherapy in Practice and Psy-
While in practice, he attended a three-year intensive week- chotherapy Theory, Research, Practice, Training.
end training program in short-term dynamic psychotherapy Magnavita is an avid reader of history, biography, and
in Montreal, Canada. In 1997, his first volume, entitled science. He enjoys skiing, bicycling, kayaking, and vaca-
Restructuring Personality Disorders: A Short-Term Dy- tioning with his family in Wellfleet on Cape Cod. Mag-
namic Approach (Magnavita, 1997), was published. This navita resides in South Glastonbury, Connecticut, with his
book integrated the work of leading pioneers in short-term wife Anne, his three daughters—Elizabeth, Emily, and
psychotherapy with the work of major historical figures, Caroline—a cairn terrier, and a guinea pig.

November 2006 ● American Psychologist 881


Handbook of personality disorders: Theory and practice
(pp. 528 –553). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Selected Bibliography
Magnavita, J. J. (2005a). Personality-guided relational psy-
Magnavita, J. J. (1993a). The evolution of short-term dy- chotherapy: A unified approach. Washington, DC: Ameri-
namic psychotherapy: Treatment of the future? Profes- can Psychological Association.
sional Psychology: Research and Practice, 24, 360 –365.
Magnavita, J. J. (2005b). Systems theory foundations of
Magnavita, J. J. (1993b). The treatment of passive–aggres- personality disorders: Components of a unification para-
sive personality disorder. Part I. International Journal of digm. In S. Strack (Ed.), Handbook of personology and
Short-Term Psychotherapy, 8(1), 29 – 41. psychopathology (pp. 140 –163). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Magnavita, J. J. (1994). Premature termination of short- Magnavita, J. J. (2005c). Using the MCMI-III for treatment
term dynamic psychotherapy. International Journal of planning and to enhance clinical efficacy. In R. J. Craig (Ed.),
Short-Term Psychotherapy, 9(4), 213–228. New directions in interpreting the Millon Clinical Multiaxial
Inventory–III (MCM-III) (pp. 165–184). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Magnavita, J. J. (1997). Restructuring personality disorders: A
short-term dynamic approach. New York: Guilford Press. Magnavita, J. J. (2006). The centrality of emotion in unify-
ing and accelerating psychotherapy. Journal of Clinical
Magnavita, J. J. (1998). Challenges in the treatment of per- Psychology, 62, 585–596.
sonality disorders: When the disorder demands comprehen- Magnavita, J. J. (in press-a). A systemic family perspective
sive integration. In Session: Psychotherapy in Practice, to child and adolescent personality disorders. In A. Free-
4(4), 5–17. man & M. Reinecke (Eds.), Personality disorders in chil-
dren and adolescents. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Magnavita, J. J. (2000a). Integrative relational therapy of
complex clinical syndromes: Ending the multigenerational Magnavita, J. J. (in press-b). Towards unification of clini-
transmission process. Journal of Clinical Psychology/In cal science: The next wave in the evolution of psychother-
Session: Psychotherapy in Practice, 56, 1051–1064. apy? Journal of Psychotherapy Integration.

Magnavita, J. J. (2000b). Relational therapy for personality Magnavita, J. J., & Carlson, J. (Producers). (2006). Person-
disorders. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. ality disorders [Video series]. Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Magnavita, J. J. (2002a). Psychodynamic approaches to
psychotherapy: A century of innovation. In F. Kaslow (Se- Magnavita, J. J., & MacFarlane, M. M. (2004). Family
ries Ed.) & J. Magnavita (Vol. Ed.), Comprehensive hand- treatment of personality disorders: Historical overview and
book of psychotherapy: Vol. 1. Psychodynamic/object rela- current perspectives. In M. M. MacFarlane (Ed.), Family
tions (pp. 1–12). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. treatment of personality disorders: Interpersonal ap-
proaches to relationship change (pp. 3–39). Binghamton,
Magnavita, J. J. (2002b). Theories of personality: Contem- NY: Haworth Press.
porary approaches to the science of personality. Hoboken,
NJ: Wiley.

Magnavita, J. J. (2004a). Classification, prevalence, and etiol-


In Search of the Unifying Principles of
ogy of personality disorders: Related issues and controversy. Psychotherapy: Conceptual, Empirical, and
In J. J. Magnavita (Ed.), Handbook of personality disorders: Clinical Convergence
Theory and practice (pp. 3–23). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Jeffrey J. Magnavita
Glastonbury Psychological Associates
Magnavita, J. J. (2004b). The relevance of theory in treat- and University of Hartford
ing personality dysfunction. In J. J. Magnavita (Ed.),
Handbook of personality disorders: Theory and practice
(pp. 56 –77). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Magnavita, J. J. (2004c). Toward a unified model of treat- The search for the principles of unified psychotherapy is
ment for personality dysfunction. In J. J. Magnavita (Ed.), an important stage in the advancement of the field.

882 November 2006 ● American Psychologist


Converging evidence from various streams of clinical vergence. Now, at the beginning of the second century of
science allows the identification of some of the major the evolution of psychotherapy, converging streams of evi-
domains of human functioning, adaptation, and dence suggest that psychologists are entering a new phase
dysfunction. These principles, supported by animal of unification, which has major implications for the field of
modeling, neuroscience, and developmental psychotherapy and clinical science.
psychopathology, along with new theoretical constructs, The manner in which psychotherapy is delivered, psy-
are demonstrating that human functioning is best chotherapists are trained, and researchers investigate the
conceptualized as holistic and highly interrelated at all processes and mechanisms of change are all contingent on
domain levels of the total ecological system. Incorporating coherent theoretical models that combine notions about
these findings with evidence accrued over the past century how development progresses, personality unfolds, psycho-
of psychotherapy practice and study allows psychologists pathology is expressed, and psychotherapeutic change oc-
to begin to explore the development of a unified system of curs. These issues have traditionally been of concern to
psychotherapy that is rooted in the clinical sciences. One clinical scientists and theorists. Rychlak (1973) wrote that
such attempt to develop a unified framework is presented personality theory requires a “theory of illness, theory of
as a starting point. cure, and therapeutic techniques” (p. 21). There are signs
Keywords: unifying psychotherapy, personality systematics, that clinical science is entering a phase of unification in
unified clinical science, relational psychotherapy, affective which a paradigmatic shift may be occurring toward a new
convergence in psychotherapy holism that challenges the way in which psychologists con-
ceptualize and treat psychological dysfunction in individu-
The development of psychotherapy is one of the many re- als, couples, families, and society. As Cappas, Andres-Hy-
markable advances witnessed during the 20th century. Psy- man, and Davidson (2005) summarized, “Psychology faces
chotherapy is well established and accepted as a treatment the challenge of understanding and treating the human be-
for a spectrum of psychological and relational disturbances. ing as a whole person with biological, psychological, so-
More people than ever before seek out psychotherapy for cial, and cultural, and spiritual dimensions” (p. 381). Clini-
psychological distress and relational difficulties. Help-seek- cians require broader domain models that capture these
ing behavior for emotional and relational disturbances does naturally occurring self-organizing systems.
not carry the same stigma for most people that was com-
monplace a few decades ago. Characters in TV shows and The Trend Toward Unification Science
movies are depicted in psychotherapy sessions, which has The evidence for unification can be seen in the trend
contributed to the public’s acceptance of psychological sci- among many scientific disciplines toward converging in
ence as an aid to coping with the demands and stress of support of the view that complex systems can be fully un-
living in modern societies. A recent poll by Newsweek derstood and studied only when they are conceptualized as
(Adler, 2006) showed that nearly 20% of Americans have holistic. In ecology, the concepts of holism and intercon-
seen a psychotherapist. There are estimated to be over 400 nectedness are the foundation of the environmental move-
schools of psychotherapy, each offering a different flavor ment. The entire ecosystem is an intricate set of interrela-
of psychotherapy. Within this smorgasbord of approaches, tionships among various domains and is not made up of
there are about a half dozen schools of psychotherapy that
separable parts (Callenbach, 1998). In biological science,
predominate. Many in the field observe that these previ-
systems biologists are trying to discover guiding principles
ously distinctive models are melding; their differences are
and embrace complexity (Stolovizky & Califano, 2006).
increasingly minor, and the models show evidence of con-
Wilson (1998) has also urged scientists to work toward
unification in his highly touted volume Consilience. Stern-
berg and Grigorenko (2001) have cogently argued for a
Editor’s Note unified psychology that is multiparadigmatic, multidisci-
Jeffrey J. Magnavita received the Award for Distinguished plinary, and uses a multiple perspective approach. Also in
Professional Contributions to Independent or Institutional psychology, earlier efforts were made by Staats (1983) in
Practice in the Private Sector. Award winners are invited his volume Psychology’s Crisis of Disunity: Philosophy
to deliver an award address at the APA’s annual conven- and Method for a Unified Science, which made the case
tion. A version of this award address was delivered at the that disunity in the field was a hindrance to scientific and
114th annual meeting, held August 10 –13, 2006, in New theoretical advances. He warned that too much overspecial-
Orleans, Louisiana. Articles based on award addresses are ization would lead to increasingly fragmented and discon-
reviewed, but they differ from unsolicited articles in that nected knowledge sets. Millon, a leading personologist and
they are expressions of the winners’ reflections on their clinical theorist, encouraged clinicians to think in a unifica-
work and their views of the field. tion paradigm:

November 2006 ● American Psychologist 883


Whether we work with “part functions” that focus on behav- sus (Magnavita, in press). In the 1980s, the psychotherapy
ior, cognitions, unconscious processes, biological defects, and integration movement emerged when well-articulated mod-
the like, or whether we address contextual systems that focus els of therapy were available to study and practice (Stricker
on the larger environment, the family, the group, or the socio- & Gold, 1993). Many practitioners welcomed the opportu-
economic and political conditions of life, the crossover point, nity to try new models and to choose what worked for
the unit that links parts to contexts, is the person. (Millon,
their patients. They welcomed the integration movement,
Grossman, Meagher, Millon, & Everly, 1999, p. xi)
which was less dogmatic, more conceptually fluid, and al-
Mayer (2005) also endorsed an integrative framework in lowed for much greater therapeutic flexibility than single
which he encouraged the study of personality, as a system, school models. Individual psychotherapists experimented
along a molecular–molar dimension from subatomic parti- with couples, group, and marital modalities to increase
cles to the larger ecological system. Influenced by this their range of effectiveness. The field was roiling with new
movement and inspired by these and many other individu- theories, methods, strategies, and claims of effectiveness
als, I have been working toward developing a unified ap- such as personal testimonials.
proach to psychopathology, personality theory, and psycho-
therapy (Magnavita, 2005a, 2005b). I have come to this Toward Unification
perspective after years of practicing psychotherapy and Clinical science and psychotherapy are reaching a level of
psychodiagnostics, using audiovisual analysis of psycho- sophistication that is fueling rapid conceptual, research, and
therapy, and experimenting with a variety of psychothera- technical advances never before possible. Neuroscientific
peutic techniques and strategies. investigations are beginning to verify some important te-
Unification Phase of Psychotherapy: The Emergence of nets of psychotherapy, such as the nature of unconscious
a New Paradigm processing (Hassin, Uleman, & Bargh, 2005). In addition,
converging neurobiological evidence is providing insight
Psychotherapy has many branches, which by and large about the basis of trauma, personality disorders, and the
have their origins to one degree or another in psychoanaly- action of psychotherapy (Gabbard, 2005). We are now able
sis. Originally psychoanalysis emerged slowly but became to view the activation of brain networks as they are at
the dominant form of conceptualization and treatment for work with the tools of neuroscience, such as functional
psychological disorders during the first half of the 20th magnetic resonance imaging, positron-emission tomography
century (Magnavita, 2002a). Other models quickly flour- scans, and electrophysiological measures. Interdisciplinary
ished and, in the early part of the 20th century, competed collaboration is opening up new avenues of investigation
with one another, often developing intense rivalries. One of that are impacting how psychologists think about psycho-
the most notable contentious struggles was between psy- therapy and psychopathology. Animal research is being
choanalysis and behavioral therapy. Most psychologists translated so that models can be developed for human be-
have seen the tape of or have read about John Watson’s havior. Researchers studying infants are adopting methods
(Watson & Rayner, 1920) challenge to psychoanalysis, from affective science to examine the nature and process of
when he demonstrated that he could induce a “neurosis” affective development and attachment, and they are collab-
with the basic principles of classical conditioning. orating with clinicians to help understand how to better
intervene when attachment fails (Fonagy, Gergely, Jurist,
Psychotherapy Integration
& Target, 2002). The nature of nonverbal communicative
A new phase in the evolution of psychotherapy became processes, so vital to relational capacities, and the develop-
apparent in the middle of the 20th century. Often referred ment of social communication are being investigated with
to as the beginning of the psychotherapy integration move- audiovisual technology and advanced nonlinear statistical
ment, rapprochement began when Dollard and Miller techniques, to capture the complexity of human function
(1950) wrote their landmark volume, Personality and Psy- and communication. For example, Ekman and Davidson’s
chotherapy, which translated psychoanalytic constructs into (1994) work in affective science on facial representation of
the language of behaviorism. Constructs such as transfer- emotion has provided an empirical foundation for reading
ence and defenses could be understood using principles of nonverbal communication, and Gottman’s (1979) use of
learning theory. Dollard and Miller clearly demonstrated multiple physiological measures, videotaping of couples’
that the same clinical phenomenon was being mapped with interaction patterns, and nonlinear statistics has helped
different languages. During this period, single school alle- identify the factors that predict marital stability. The devel-
giance was still very common, often with limited interac- opment of relatively inexpensive audiovisual equipment
tion among adherents of differing theories. The single has allowed clinical scientists to visually explore and ana-
school stage in the evolution of psychotherapy was neces- lyze relationships among partners, infant–maternal dyads,
sary because new innovations had to be developed, tested, and therapist–patient dyads. Even psychotherapists in solo
and refined or discarded if they lacked support or consen- practice can avail themselves of these technologies for ana-

884 November 2006 ● American Psychologist


lyzing and refining their therapeutic understanding and for Patients who consult psychologists for treatment are en-
conducting patient follow-up studies documented by video- titled to the best explanation that science has to offer about
taped interviews. how their disorders developed and what psychologists
know from the empirical research and clinical literature
Rationale for Consideration of Unified Psychotherapy about how best to ameliorate their condition. Psychologists
It seems evident that some distinct principles of unification need a theoretical framework with which to orient their
science can be gleaned from the various areas of clinical psychodiagnostic assessment, so that the necessary infor-
mation is gathered to formulate a coherent, multiperspec-
science, as well as other scientific disciplines (Anchin, in
tive treatment plan that addresses the specific concerns that
press). As Rychlak (1973) pointed out a third of a century
are presented, as well others that unfold as additional infor-
ago, a substantive theory of psychotherapy must include
mation is gathered. Single domain models of psychother-
interrelated and consistent constructs about personality,
apy and change are useful but insufficient to address the
psychopathology, and the process and techniques of psy-
spectrum of human dysfunction and maladaptation encoun-
chotherapeutic change. To construct such a theory requires tered by psychologists in a variety of settings. As psycho-
a holistic framework and some understanding of the inter- analysts have always averred as a rationale for depth ther-
connectedness of domains. Thus, there must be unity in the apy, treating the symptoms is superficial and does not
clinical sciences, or we risk a lack of coherence among address the root cause. There is some truth in this justifica-
constructs. The danger is that researchers may toil without tion. Most disorders do not have a single root cause (e.g.,
knowing how their domain area is impacted and how it genetic, dynamic, developmental, or familial; Magnavita,
impacts the larger system, potentially missing some vital 2006, in press). Rather, dysfunction exists when multiple
influence or process. Of course, studying smaller parts of converging pathways interact in a manner and at various
any system is valuable to the advancement of clinical sci- developmental junctures to create dysfunction, such as
ence, but especially for clinicians, there should always be symptoms or chronic patterns of maladaptation. The com-
an emphasis on how these findings translate into the larger plexity of processes and subsystems that converge in vari-
systems within which they operate. Researchers should ous forms of psychopathology and family dysfunction
seek to explain how their findings can be translated to clin- make a systemic approach necessary. Many current clinical
ical issues. theorists believe that personality is the major point of con-
Clinicians have been known to apply one approach to vergence for human study and that examining the etiology,
each patient regardless of what is actually needed. There is developmental progression, and treatment are critical chal-
a danger of overreliance on treatment manuals without suf- lenges for clinical scientists.
ficient grounding in clinical science. Theoretical frame-
works are critical tools for understanding any complex phe- Organizing Principles of Clinical Unification Science
nomena. Theory shapes conceptualization by limiting the After an extensive review of the literature and various
ways in which psychologists know where to look, how stages in developing models of psychotherapy based on
they bias clinical and empirical data, and how they orient findings from clinical science, the following principles are
their interventions. I previously discussed the relevance of offered as points of reference for others interested in pursu-
integrative theory in treating personality dysfunction (Mag- ing unified psychotherapy. These principles should be ap-
navita, 1997, 2000, 2002b). Personality disorders have only plicable to all valid therapeutic orientations.
recently been considered amenable to treatment. However,
even integrative theory falls short when psychologists at- The Domains of Human Functioning Exist as
tempt to coherently identify the factors and solutions for Interconnected, Nested Ecological Systems, Which Can
complex problems, such as personality dysfunction. Treat- Be Organized From the Molecular to the Molar Level
ing personality dysfunction, and to a lesser degree any It is becoming apparent to most scientists and clinical theo-
clinical syndrome, requires making sense out of the seem- rists that recent scientific advances in computer technology
ing chaos of interrelated interactions among neurobiologi- and communications have increased the human level of
cal, individual, couple, family, social, and ecological sys- connectedness. Each domain subsystem, from the neuronal
tems (Magnavita, 2004). In previous volumes and chapters, to the sociopolitical, is made up of interconnected struc-
I have explored the levels and component domains of per- tures and processes, which relate to other networks in non-
sonality dysfunction and have suggested methods for inter- linear ways. An ecological model requires psychologists to
vention drawing from a plethora of approaches. The reality take into account the context in which behavior occurs, as
of working with those who suffer from personality dys- well as the feedback systems that continually modify and
function is that no single theory, method, or technique will regulate a system. Even when operating within one level of
suffice. Rather, various perspectives, methods, and strate- a system, it is vital to understand the impact on other lev-
gies are required. els. For example, psychopharmacologists, who prescribe

November 2006 ● American Psychologist 885


medications that improve individual symptom expression ety, which requires the overutilization of defenses; this can
such as depression, anxiety, impotence and so forth, often undermine adaptive functioning. The overarching goal of
see their patients or their patients’ partner or family sabo- psychotherapeutic action at this domain level is to regulate
tage medication compliance. Often the system is not com- emotional functioning so that emotion is used adaptively
fortable with the implications of bottom-up change. An and to restructure the dysfunctional cognitions to enhance
understanding of other levels of the system will allow for overall functioning.
other options to be considered. Interpersonal– dyadic matrix: Domains and processes
Part–Whole Relationships Characterize Human that operate between self and other. The interpersonal–
Functioning, Incorporating Networks of Subdomain and dyadic matrix primarily accounts for processes that occur
Larger Domain Systems between two people, in regulating the tension between inti-
macy and separateness. According to Berscheid (1999), we
Parts do not exist as isolated elements but in the context in are witnessing “the greening of a new science of interper-
which they are observed and in which they operate. When sonal relationships” (p. 260). She wrote, “We are born into
dealing with complex phenomenon, it is unrealistic to sepa- relationships, we live our lives in relationships with others,
rate parts from their networks and not expect that there
and when we die, the effects of our relationships survive in
will be a loss of explanatory power. Human beings are em-
the lives of the living, reverberating throughout the tissue
bedded in multiple nested structures/networks that offer
differing perspectives of phenomena (Bronfenbrenner, of their relationships” (Berscheid, 1999, pp. 261–262). The
1979). Using portions of Bronfenbrenner’s (1979) ecologi- primary attachment system is the maternal–infant dyad,
cal model, along with theoretical elements from an exten- which is the foundational dyad that begins to shape the
sive review of the theories of psychotherapy, psychopathol- emergent self (intrapsychic structures/processes) from the
ogy, developmental psychology, and personality theory, I genetic and temperamental substrate. Attachment systems
constructed a unified framework (Magnavita, 2005a). This develop and are codified, in the rich nutrient of attunement,
unified, component systems model attempts to account for regulation, and repair and then become a robust predictor
the major domains of human functioning that have been of adult interpersonal styles (Schore, 2003). The interper-
identified during the era of contemporary clinical science. sonal matrix offers a window into the rich tapestry of at-
Incorporating an ever-widening perspective, it is possible tachment experiences that are recreated in relationships
to divide the total human ecosystem into the following four with others and in therapeutic relationships. Using a chaos
nested matrices from the microlevel to the macrolevel. theory model, Butz, Chamberlain, and McCown (1997)
Intrapsychic– biological matrix: Domains and pro- wrote, “The interplay of individual needs and the needs of
cesses that operate inside the person. The intrapsychic– the relationship sometimes flows smoothly, and sometimes
biological matrix basically subsumes the processes and is turbulent and chaotic” (p. 73).
structures that occur within an individual (intraindividual). Attachment experiences build the capacity for and inter-
These include neurobiological processes–structures, affec- est in developing intimacy as development progresses. “In-
tive– defensive functions, cognitive schemata, and tempera- timacy is not only important to family relationships, it is
mental predispositions. On the basis of genetic, tempera- the necessary, albeit not sufficient, ingredient for human
mental, and neurobiological structures, each individual’s survival” (L’Abate, 1986, p. 227). “In the process of relat-
intrapsychic structure is shaped by the attachment–rela- ing to another over time, we weave the fabric of separate-
tional system. Expanding on the work of Ezriel (1952) and ness and togetherness into the complex tapestry of inti-
Malan (1979), using a triangular configuration, the bottom
macy” (Butz et al., 1997, p. 74). Expanding on the work of
corner of the triangle can be depicted as the cognitive sche-
Menninger (1958) and Malan (1979), this level can also be
mata (templates of self– other) and affective system engen-
dered by attachment and traumatic experiences. Feeling depicted by a triangular configuration, which captures the
that has not been processed (i.e., identified, experienced, processes of dyadic interpersonal relationships. The lower
and contextualized in a narrative) is activated in later at- corner represents past relationships and the relational sche-
tachment experiences and gives rise to anxiety, located on mas and attachment systems that are used to guide current
the upper-right corner. This nonmetabolized affect is chan- relationships (upper-right corner) and expected relation-
neled into anxiety, which is modulated by the defenses on ships (i.e., therapeutic relationship) in the upper-left corner.
the upper-left side. Various constellations of defenses are All relationships are based on internalized relational sche-
used at the intrapsychic level depending on developmental mata based on attachment experiences and family and ex-
stage and progression of personality development. The trafamilial relationships.
more primitive the defenses, the more compromised the Four main findings summarizing the research with im-
system. When this subsystem lacks differentiation, emo- plications for psychotherapy and relational researchers are
tions are not experienced, resulting in unmanageable anxi- presented by Anderson, Reznik, and Glassman (2005):

886 November 2006 ● American Psychologist


1. Significant-other representations are activated auto- Culture also confers identity and so is linked to all four
matically in transference. levels of the human personality system. Prager (1995) de-
2. Affect arises relatively automatically in transference scribed what she termed the cultural community:
when the significant-other representation is activated.
3. The relational self is activated relatively automati- A community involves generations that move through it, with
cally when the significant-other representation is customary ways of handling the transitions of generations. To
activated. continue to function, a community also adapts with changing
4. Some self-regulatory processes in the relational self times, experimenting with and resisting new ideas in ways
are evoked in the response to “threat” (e.g., negative that maintain core values while learning from changes that are
cues) in transference and may be automatic. (p. 423) desired or required. (p. 81)

Relational–triadic matrix: Domains and processes that Complexity and Chaos Are Characteristics of the
operate in three-way relationships. The relational–triadic Biopsychosocial System
matrix depicts what transpires in unstable dyads (Bowen, Margaret Mead (1978) wrote, “Stated at its simplest, any
1978), and “it is the instability of dyads that produces rela- whole system—a single organism, an island ecosystem, our
tionship triangles” (Guerin, Fogarty, Fay, & Kautto, 1996, planet, the solar system—is subject to imbalances within
p. 8). Again, the processes can be depicted with a triangu- itself” (p. 150). Ecological and human systems are complex
lar configuration. At the bottom of the triangle is a third and subject to chaotic states. Gribbin (2004) described a
(triangulated) person, and the top two corners represent complex system as one “that is made up of several simpler
two people in a dyad. An unstable dyad is one in which components interacting with one another” (p. 144). Human
each member is relatively undifferentiated, which refers to systems also self-organize in a persistent drive for survival,
both emotional differentiation, or what has been termed adaptation, and replication. Chaos theory has offered a new
emotional intelligence, as well as self– other differentiation, conceptual tool for understanding complexity in systems
which refers to appropriate boundaries. Since this type of (Gleick, 1987). Chaos theory views the functioning of
dyad does not effectively maintain intimacy and closeness complex systems as basically unpredictable and prone to
in a regulated way—they tend to try to fuse or are overly random fluctuations inherent in the system that can cause
distant, or they fluctuate wildly between the two poles— destabilization and reordering. According to chaos theory,
they tend to spill their emotions into third parties in an small perturbations in the components of a system may
attempt to stabilize the dyad. “When individuals are caught lead to cascading effects with enormous consequence.
in triangles, their freedom of movement is severely circum- Within complex systems there appear to be patterns that
scribed” (Guerin et al., 1996, p. 24), and “a person emo- replicate the order of each level. This is comparable to
tionally trapped in a triangle is likely, by virtue of being what many clinicians call parallel process where, for exam-
trapped, to suffer some loss of function” (p. 31). In three- ple, staff issues on an inpatient treatment unit are mirrored
somes, as opposed to triangles, there is a lack of emotional in patient conflict. Psychotherapists often describe seeing
reactivity. Triangles are in effect defenses, which can allow someone’s core issues played out in various, often chaotic-
a relationship to continue without change. appearing relational patterns.
Sociocultural–familial matrix: Processes and domains
that operate among larger social systems. The sociocul- Relationships Are the Central Process of Human
tural–family matrix is the macrosystem in which the indi- Adaptation, Function, and Dysfunction
vidual, couples, families and groups, and culture operate The relational matrix is the ultimate unifying principle of
and have mutual influence. “The defining features of the human development and of psychotherapy. It plays a cen-
human species—such as using language and passing on tral role in shaping and influencing human function and
inventions and adaptations to subsequent generations—are dysfunction. Human beings are essentially relational, con-
our cultural heritage” (Rogoff, 2003, p. 64). Cultural values ceived and raised in a richly complex relational matrix,
and memes, which are social units transmitted like genes which is shaped by evolutionary processes to ensure sur-
only socially (Dawkins, 1976), have a powerful impact on vival and transmission of the gene pool. Family units re-
the components and processes identified as occurring at main the main units of socialization within society and
other levels of the personality system. Prager (1995) wrote, transmit values and mores through memes (Dawkins, 1976)
“Cultural values with respect to intimacy likely affect the just as genes transmit biological data. Humans are defined
importance individual dyad members place on intimacy in by relationships and the attachment drive is necessary for
their families” (p. 237). The value placed on intimacy var- survival and the development of civilization.
ies from culture to culture. “Especially apparent is the in- The primacy of relationships. It has been well docu-
terplay between partners’ personality characteristics, such mented in primate studies (Harlow & Harlow, 1962) and
as private self-consciousness, need strengths, relationship naturalistic observation of infant deprivation (Spitz, 1947),
beliefs, and intimate relationships” (Prager, 1995, p. 238). that a lack of sufficient attachment can lead to a cata-

November 2006 ● American Psychologist 887


strophic breakdown of the developing infants’ survival ca- 1999) and is of great interest to relational scientists
pacity, which may not be reparable (Blum, 2002). It be- (Synder, 1999). Emotions may be uniquely evolved ad-
came clear from Harlow and Harlow’s (1962) work that aptation mechanisms, which have assured human sur-
primates require an affectionate attachment for optimal de- vival, so far. Emotion orients our response system by
velopment; food alone is not enough. Harlow and Harlow alerting us to what we need to pay attention to in our
were able to move beyond the conditioning paradigm of stimulus field and what we need to respond to and how
their era, demonstrating the necessity of studying the quickly we need to do so. All psychopathology seems to
mother–infant relationship. The importance of part–whole involve problems with emotional regulation and func-
relationships was observed from a clinician’s perspective tions. “Emotional disturbances are common to almost all
by Winnicott (1964), who said “there is no such thing as psychopathological conditions, ranging from eating dis-
an infant, only a mother– child dyad” (p. 88). The process orders to schizophrenia” (Berenbaum, Raghavan, Le,
of psychotherapy uses relational factors to stimulate heal- Vernon, & Gomez, 2003, p. 206). Emotion is also a pri-
ing and growth. The quality of the therapeutic relationship mary force in conceptualizing how personality is orga-
is probably the most robust aspect of therapeutic outcome nized (Block, 2002; Magnavita, 2002a). Barlow (2000),
(Norcross, 2002). Techniques and strategies are also impor- a major figure in cognitive– behavior treatment, wrote,
tant, but it is through the quality of the psychotherapeutic “The growing recognition of the necessity of focusing
relationship that they can be delivered to and used by the on emotional experience and expression as a scientific
patient. subject in its own right is highlighted by the appearance
The basic building block of the relational matrix—The of the new APA [American Psychological Association]
attachment system. The primacy of the attachment sys- journal Emotion” (p. 1249). It is now commonly ac-
tem in human development and psychopathology is well cepted that the study of cognition and emotion are in-
documented (King, Ferris, & Lederhendler, 2003). When separable (Magnavita, in press). Researchers have sup-
the communicative elements of the infant–mother attach-
ported what clinicians have observed: “The affect
ment system are not optimal, emotional and self develop-
associated with the representation emerges in facial ex-
ment will most likely be disrupted. “Dysregulation of this
pressions evoked rather immediately in transference”
central integrating process will undermine successful self-
organization, which may produce various forms of distur- (Anderson et al., 2005, p. 425).
bances in emotional regulation and lead to mental suffer- The power of emotion to act as an attractor state can
ing” (Siegel, 1999, p. 87). Attachment experience is be illustrated in the relationships among trauma, mem-
retained cognitively, as relational schemata: ory, affective experience, neurobiological structure–func-
tion, and personality dysfunction. Evidence from multi-
Specifically, as bundles of knowledge, significant-other repre- ple lines of research shows that trauma can induce a
sentations are used to interpret and respond to new people, state of overwhelming affect, releasing too much corti-
and in the process the chronic tendency to have particular sol, which initiates cascading feedback loops, likely af-
kinds of relationships as linked with these significant others fecting the volume of the hippocampus (Teicher, 2002),
comes into play, much as relational schemas do. (Anderson et
which is in part responsible for emotional processing
al., 2005, p. 423)
(Davidson, 2000). The emotional circuit may become
Affective processes prime the relational system and acti- stuck in positive feedback loops whereby the implicit
vate neurobiological structures (i.e., pruning nonactivated memory of the situation is activated without conscious
neuronal connections and networks), spawn language de- awareness when triggered by environmental or internal
velopment, and create a crucible for the emergent self. stimuli. Conscious memory of the event is often lost and
only retained in the overactive emotional reaction pat-
Affect Is a Convergent Force for Human Development terns. Emotion becomes a convergent point through
and Function of Personality Systems which the system expresses its dysfunction. Psychother-
Ancient Greek philosophers were interested in the emo- apy can restore the system by dyadic emotional regula-
tions, but the first contemporary in-depth exploration of tion, reorganization, and restoring memory by reconsoli-
emotion was Darwin’s (1872/1998) treatise, The Expres- dating the experience cortically, thus transferring the
sion of the Emotions in Man and Animal, considered by disconnected memory of traumatic incidents to long-
many the beginning of affective science. Since then, a term narrative memory. Anderson et al. (2005) wrote,
renewed interest in the scientific study of affect has “The simplest affective finding emerging from our work
been experiencing an explosive growth spurt across dis- is that the activation of significant-other representations
ciplines. It is an exciting time for those who are inter- elicits the affect typically experienced in relation to the
ested in affective science (Davidson, 2000). Emotion significant other, as in the theory of schema-triggered
may play a crucial role in consciousness (Damasio, affect” (p. 438).

888 November 2006 ● American Psychologist


Personality and Psychopathology Are Emergent schema, core issues, and so forth. Thus, these are essential
Phenomena That Can Be Depicted by a Holographic organizational patterns in which a number of processes
Representation converge and are replicated. Complex systems are prone to
Personality, psychopathology, mind, and consciousness random fluctuations or induced fluctuations, which can re-
may be considered emergent phenomena of complex per- sult in a major reorganization of the system. In fact, small
sonality and relational systems. Schwartz and Begley perturbations in systems can lead to a cascade of major
(2002) described this term: “An emergent phenomenon is change. Developmental trajectories are a good example of
one whose characteristics or behaviors cannot be explained complex systems establishing steady states, experiencing
in terms of the sum of its parts; if mind is emergent, then disorganization, and reorganizing at a higher level of
it cannot be explained by the brain” (p. 350). Personality complexity.
and psychopathology are emergent phenomena that exist as Emotion primes the attachment system and ensures opti-
an expression of a complex system’s function and adapta- mal development for the neonate. This dyadic regulation is
tion. Although psychologists can describe and measure per- conducted like a jazz riff: The infant expresses itself and
sonality and psychopathology, they are not concrete entities the mother responds; the infant reacts and the frame ex-
but expressions of a system viewed and understood in rela- pands. Emotional dysregulation is a common feature of
tional terms. many personality and affective disorders. On the basis of
It is possible to view various levels of the system to his synthesis of neuroscientific findings, Cozolino (2002)
enhance the understanding of the factors and processes that summarized the complexity of interactions and mutual
converge in particular constellations, but each perspective feedback systems:
is limited. There has been interest recently in using the Nowhere are these organizing principles more evident than in
concept of a hologram to represent phenomena in physics psychotherapy. Clients continue to see themselves, the world,
and neuroscience; the hologram has useful features for rep- and others as a function of their experiences in early relation-
resenting personality and psychopathological expression as ships, despite much evidence to the contrary. This anachronis-
well (Talbot, 1991). A hologram is a three-dimensional tic world view is mediated through the power of negative
representation, the perception of which changes depending learning during the early development of the social brain, and
on the angle from which it is viewed. It is an image that is resistant to change. Self-identity is an emotional and con-
represents multiple part–whole relationships expressed in ceptual spin-off of the self experienced in a relationship; in
other words, the individual emerges from the dyad. The core
unity. The view changes with each perspective taken. A sense of self, world, and others generated from negative early
holonic assessment requires that many views of the person- experiences affects all domains of life, making the individual
ality functioning and psychopathology are considered so vulnerable to deficits in social and occupational functioning,
that a three-dimensional view can be held in consciousness. physical illness, and psychopathology later in life. (p. 179)
Of course, this is never fully achievable. A holonic ap-
proach eschews single perspective views of complex emer- Complex systems evolve, moving through various stages
gent phenomenon. by a dialect process between stable state and reorganiza-
tion. This process occurs by increasing the differentiation
Complex Systems Are Self-Organizing and Tend to of part–whole relationships, which can lead to destabiliza-
Replicate Themselves at Different Domain Levels tion or chaos, which usually leads to self-organization at a
In a previous publication, I explored the use of a combina- higher level of differentiation and a new level of integra-
tion of general system theory (von Bertalanffy, 1968) and tion. Thus, change, including positive change, is seldom
chaos theory as a lens with which to view personality, psy- linear and more prone to disruption.
chopathology, and psychotherapy (Magnavita, 2005b).
The Challenges for Psychotherapists
Other clinical theorists have been exploring and developing
similar frameworks to understand psychotherapy (Angyal, Psychotherapists are continually required to process enor-
1941; Butz et al., 1997). mous amounts of data, organize the information, and
Chaos theory basically views complex systems as self- through a process of pattern recognition, make educated
organizing entities, which are prone to states of random judgments about the cause of psychopathology. They need
fluctuations and disequilibrium, and therefore are nonlinear to figure out how to harness change processes and deter-
in their dynamic process. Within a system are attractor mine where in the ecological system the most efficient in-
states where elements of the system are drawn together or tervention can occur to alleviate suffering and enhance ad-
converge. Attractor states or patterns may be represented in aptation. The practice of psychotherapy requires extended
various ways by different theorists and can include early training to develop internal models of psychopathology,
maladaptive schema, reenactment patterns, kindling (net- family dysfunction, and developmental processes which
works that are activated together are more prone to being can be used to organize the massive amounts of informa-
activated again), transference, repetitive maladaptive tion that are communicated by individuals and families.

November 2006 ● American Psychologist 889


The use of a unified framework demands much of the psy- bances. Clinical Psychology: Practice and Science, 10,
chotherapist, as multiple domains of knowledge need to be 206 –226.
absorbed and organized. This requires more than technical
skill alone; a deep understanding of complex systems and Berscheid, E. (1999). The greening of relationship science.
their domains is necessary. American Psychologist, 54, 260 –266.
Conclusions
Block, J. (2002). Personality as an affect-processing sys-
The call from the National Institute of Mental Health to tem: Toward an integrative theory. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
enter a stage of translation research makes unification sci-
ence an urgent challenge to clinical scientists. Clinicians Blum, D. (2002). Love at Goon Park: Harry Harlow and
who are inundated with massive amounts of information the science of affection. Cambridge, MA: Perseus.
from multiple sources need a framework with which to
organize this knowledge and apply it appropriately in their Bowen, M. (1978). Family therapy in clinical practice.
treatment of individuals, couples, families, and social sys- New York: Aronson.
tems that are in states of chaos and dysfunction. A compo-
nent system model of unified psychotherapy allows various Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human develop-
vital subsystems of the total ecological biopsychosocial ment: Experiments by nature and design. Cambridge, MA:
sphere to be elaborated and corroborated using new tools Harvard University Press.
of clinical science such as those of neuroscience, nonlinear
statistics, and audiovisual and computer technology.
Butz, M. R., Chamberlain, L. L., & McCown, W. G.
(1997). Strange attractors: Chaos, complexity, and the art
Author’s Note
of family therapy. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Jeffrey J. Magnavita, Glastonbury Psychological Associ-
ates, Glastonbury, Connecticut, and Graduate Institute of
Professional Psychology, University of Hartford. Callenbach, E. (1998). Ecology: A pocket guide. Berkeley:
Correspondence concerning this address should be sent University of California Press.
to Jeffrey J. Magnavita, Glastonbury Medical Arts Center,
300 Hebron Avenue, Suite 215, Glastonbury, CT 06033. Cappas, N. M., Andres-Hyman, R., & Davidson, L. (2005).
E-mail: MagnaPsych@aol.com What psychotherapists can begin to learn from neuro-
science: Seven principles of brain-based psychotherapy.
References Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 42,
374 –383.
Adler, J. (2006, March 27). Freud in our midst. Newsweek,
43– 49. Cozolino, L. J. (2002). The neuroscience of psychotherapy:
Building and rebuilding the human brain. New York: Nor-
Anchin, J. C. (in press). Pursuing a unifying paradigm for ton.
psychotherapy: Tasks, dialectical considerations, biopsy-
chosocial metatheory. Journal of Psychotherapy Integra- Damasio, A. (1999). The feeling of what happens: Body
tion. and emotion in the making of consciousness. New York:
Harcourt.
Anderson, S. M., Reznik, I., & Glassman, N. S. (2005).
The unconscious relational self. In R. R. Hansen, J. S. Ule- Darwin, C. R. (1998). The expression of the emotions in
man, & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The new unconscious (pp. 421– man and animal (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University
481). New York: Oxford University Press. Press. (Original work published in 1872).

Angyal, A. (1941). Foundations for a science of personal- Davidson, R. J. (2000). Affective style, psychopathology,
ity. New York: Commonwealth Fund. and resilience: Brain mechanisms and plasticity. American
Psychologist, 55, 1196 –1214.
Barlow, D. H. (2000). Unraveling the mysteries of anxiety
and its disorders from the perspective of emotion theory. Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. New York: Oxford
American Psychologist, 55, 1247–1263. University Press.

Berenbaum, H., Raghavan, C., Le, H.-N., Vernon, L. L., & Dollard, J., & Miller, N. E. (1950). Personality and psy-
Gomez, J. J. (2003). A taxonomy of emotional distur- chotherapy. New York: McGraw-Hill.

890 November 2006 ● American Psychologist


Ekman, P., & Davidson, R. J. (Eds.). (1994). The nature of Magnavita, J. J. (2002b). Theories of personality: Contem-
emotion: Fundamental questions. New York: Oxford Press. porary approaches to the science of personality. Hoboken,
NJ: Wiley.
Ezriel, H. (1952). Notes on psychoanalytic group therapy:
Interpretation and research. Psychiatry, 15, 119 –126. Magnavita, J. J. (Ed.). (2004). Handbook of personality
disorders: Theory and practice. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E., & Target, M. (2002).
Affect regulation, mentalization and the development of the Magnavita, J. J. (2005a). Personality-guided relational psy-
self. New York: Other Press. chotherapy: A unified approach. Washington, DC: Ameri-
can Psychological Association.
Gabbard, G. O. (2005). Mind, brain, and personality disor-
ders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 648 – 655. Magnavita, J. J. (2005b). Systems theory foundation of
personality, psychopathology, and psychotherapy. In S.
Gleick, J. (1987). Chaos: Making a new science. New Strack (Ed.), Handbook of personology and psychopathol-
York: Viking/Penguin Books. ogy (pp. 140 –163). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Gottman, J. M. (1979). Marital interaction: Experimental Magnavita, J. J. (2006). Emotion in short-term psychother-
investigations. New York: Academic Press. apy: An introduction. Journal of Clinical Psychology: In
Session, 62, 517–522.
Gribbin, J. (2004). Deep simplicity: Bringing order to
chaos and complexity. New York: Random House. Magnavita, J. J. (in press). Toward unification of clinical
science: The next wave in the evolution of psychotherapy?
Guerin, P. J., Fogarty, T. F., Fay, L. F., & Kautto, J. G. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration.
(1996). Working with relational triangles: The one-two-
three of psychotherapy. New York: Guilford Press. Malan, D. H. (1979). Individual psychotherapy and the
science of psychodynamics. London: Butterworth.
Harlow, H. F., & Harlow, M. K. (1962). Social deprivation
in monkeys. Scientific American, 203, 136 –146. Mayer, J. D. (2005). A tale of two visions: Can a new
view of personality help integrate psychology? American
Hassin, R. R., Uleman, J. S., & Bargh, J. A. (Eds.). (2005). Psychologist, 60, 294 –307.
The new unconscious. New York: Oxford University Press.
Mead, M. (1978). Culture and commitment: New relation-
King, J. A., Ferris, C. F., & Lederhendler, I. L. (Eds.). ships between generations in the 1970⬘s. New York: Co-
(2003). Roots of mental illness in children. New York: lumbia University Press.
New York Academy of Science.
Menninger, K. (1958). Theory of psychoanalytic technique.
L’Abate, L. (1986). Systemic family therapy. New York: New York: Basic Books.
Brunner/Mazel.
Millon, T., Grossman, S., Meagher, S., Millon, C., & Ev-
Magnavita, J. J. (1997). Restructuring personality disor- erly, G. (1999). Personality-guided therapy. Hoboken, NJ:
ders: A short-term dynamic approach. New York: Guilford Wiley.
Press.
Norcross, J. C. (Ed.). (2002). Psychotherapy relationships
Magnavita, J. J. (2000). Relational therapy for personality that work: Therapist contributions and responsiveness to
disorders. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. patients. New York: Oxford University Press.

Magnavita, J. J. (2002a). Psychodynamic approaches to Prager, K. J. (1995). The psychology of intimacy. New
psychotherapy: A century of innovations. In F. W. Kaslow York: Guilford Press.
(Series Ed.) & J. J. Magnavita (Vol. Ed.), Comprehensive
handbook of personality disorders: Psychodynamic/object Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human develop-
relations (Vol. 1, pp. 1–12). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. ment. New York: Oxford University Press.

November 2006 ● American Psychologist 891


Rychlak, J. F. (1973). Introduction to personality and psy- Stricker, G., & Gold, J. R. (Eds.). (1993). Comprehensive
chotherapy. A theory construction approach. Boston: handbook of psychotherapy integration. New York: Plenum
Houghton Mifflin. Press.

Schore, A. N. (2003). Affect regulation and the repair of Synder, D. K. (1999). Affective reconstruction in the con-
the self. New York: Norton. text of a pluralistic approach to couple therapy. Clinical
Psychology: Science and Practice, 6, 348 –365.
Schwartz, J. M., & Begley, S. (2002). The mind and the
brain: Neuroplasticity and the power of mental force. New
York: HarperCollins. Talbot, M. (1991). The holographic universe. New York:
Harper Perennial.
Siegel, D. J. (1999). The developing mind: Toward a neu-
robiology of interpersonal experience. New York: Guilford Teicher, M. H. (2002). Scars that won’t heal: The neurobi-
Press. ology of child abuse. Scientific American, 286, 68 –75.

Spitz, R. (Producer). (1947). Grief: A peril in infancy von Bertalanffy, L. (1968). General system theory: Foun-
[Silent film]. (Available from the Archives of the His- dations, development, applications. New York: Braziller.
tory of Psychology, Penn State University, http://www
.mediasales.psu.edu) Watson, J. B., & Rayner, R. (1920). Conditioned emotional
reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 3, 1– 4.
Staats, A. W. (1983). Psychology’s crisis of disunity: Phi-
losophy and method for a unified science. New York: Prae-
Wilson, E. O. (1998). Consilience: The unity of knowledge.
ger.
New York: Knopf.
Sternberg, R. J., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2001). Unified psy-
chology. American Psychologist, 56, 1069 –1079. Winnicott, D. W. (1964). The child, the family and the out-
side world. Harmondsworth, Middlesex, England: Penguin.
Stolovizky, G., & Califano, A. (2006, March/April). Sys-
tems biology: Making sense of oceans of biological data.
Update: New York Academy of Sciences, 20 –33.

892 November 2006 ● American Psychologist

You might also like