You are on page 1of 173

Title

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Author(s)

Kelly, Helen; Brady, Marian C.; Enderby, Pam

Publication date

2010

Original citation

Kelly H, Brady MC, Enderby P. Speech and language therapy for


aphasia following stroke. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews
2010, Issue 5. Art. No.: CD000425. DOI:
10.1002/14651858.CD000425.pub2.

Type of publication

Article (peer-reviewed)

Link to publisher's
version

http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD000425.pub3
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD000425.pub2
Access to the full text of the published version may require a
subscription.

Rights

Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration.

Item downloaded
from

http://hdl.handle.net/10468/1418

Downloaded on 2016-07-17T07:51:08Z

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke


(Review)
Kelly H, Brady MC, Enderby P

This is a reprint of a Cochrane review, prepared and maintained by The Cochrane Collaboration and published in The Cochrane Library
2010, Issue 7
http://www.thecochranelibrary.com

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

TABLE OF CONTENTS
HEADER . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
ABSTRACT . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
PLAIN LANGUAGE SUMMARY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
BACKGROUND . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
OBJECTIVES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
METHODS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
RESULTS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
DISCUSSION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
AUTHORS CONCLUSIONS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
REFERENCES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
CHARACTERISTICS OF STUDIES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
DATA AND ANALYSES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 1.1. Comparison 1 SLT versus no SLT, Outcome 1 Functional communication. . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 1.2. Comparison 1 SLT versus no SLT, Outcome 2 Receptive language: auditory comprehension. . . . .
Analysis 1.3. Comparison 1 SLT versus no SLT, Outcome 3 Receptive language: reading comprehension. . . . .
Analysis 1.4. Comparison 1 SLT versus no SLT, Outcome 4 Receptive language: other. . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 1.5. Comparison 1 SLT versus no SLT, Outcome 5 Receptive language: gesture comprehension (unnamed).
Analysis 1.6. Comparison 1 SLT versus no SLT, Outcome 6 Expressive language: naming. . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 1.7. Comparison 1 SLT versus no SLT, Outcome 7 Expressive language: general. . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 1.8. Comparison 1 SLT versus no SLT, Outcome 8 Expressive language: written. . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 1.9. Comparison 1 SLT versus no SLT, Outcome 9 Expressive language: repetition. . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 1.10. Comparison 1 SLT versus no SLT, Outcome 10 Severity of impairment: Aphasia Battery Score (+ PICA).
Analysis 1.11. Comparison 1 SLT versus no SLT, Outcome 11 Severity of impairment: Aphasia Battery Score (3-month
follow up). . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 1.12. Comparison 1 SLT versus no SLT, Outcome 12 Psychosocial: MAACL. . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 1.13. Comparison 1 SLT versus no SLT, Outcome 13 Number of drop-outs (any reason). . . . . . . .
Analysis 2.1. Comparison 2 SLT versus social support and stimulation, Outcome 1 Functional communication. . .
Analysis 2.2. Comparison 2 SLT versus social support and stimulation, Outcome 2 Receptive language: auditory
comprehension. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 2.3. Comparison 2 SLT versus social support and stimulation, Outcome 3 Receptive language: other. . . .
Analysis 2.4. Comparison 2 SLT versus social support and stimulation, Outcome 4 Expressive language: single words.
Analysis 2.5. Comparison 2 SLT versus social support and stimulation, Outcome 5 Expressive language: sentences. .
Analysis 2.6. Comparison 2 SLT versus social support and stimulation, Outcome 6 Expressive language: picture
description. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 2.7. Comparison 2 SLT versus social support and stimulation, Outcome 7 Expressive language: overall spoken.
Analysis 2.8. Comparison 2 SLT versus social support and stimulation, Outcome 8 Expressive language: written. . .
Analysis 2.9. Comparison 2 SLT versus social support and stimulation, Outcome 9 Severity of impairment: Aphasia Battery
Score. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 2.10. Comparison 2 SLT versus social support and stimulation, Outcome 10 Number of drop-outs for any
reason. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 3.1. Comparison 3 Functional SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 1 Functional
communication. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 3.2. Comparison 3 Functional SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 2 Functional
communication: catalogue ordering. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 3.3. Comparison 3 Functional SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 3 Expressive language:
spoken. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 3.4. Comparison 3 Functional SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 4 Expressive language:
written. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 4.1. Comparison 4 Intensive SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 1 Receptive language:
auditory comprehension (change from baseline). . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)
Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

1
1
2
2
4
4
7
28
31
32
32
38
89
97
98
99
100
100
101
102
103
104
105
106
106
107
108
109
110
110
111
112
113
113
114
114
115
116
117
117
118
i

Analysis 4.2. Comparison 4 Intensive SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 2 Expressive language:
spoken (change from baseline scores).
. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 4.3. Comparison 4 Intensive SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 3 Written language: (change
from baseline scores). . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 4.4. Comparison 4 Intensive SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 4 Severity of impairment:
Aphasia Battery Score. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 4.5. Comparison 4 Intensive SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 5 Number of drop-outs for
any reason. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 5.1. Comparison 5 Volunteer-facilitated SLT (SLTA) versus professional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 1 Functional
communication. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 5.2. Comparison 5 Volunteer-facilitated SLT (SLTA) versus professional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 2 Receptive
language: auditory comprehension. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 5.3. Comparison 5 Volunteer-facilitated SLT (SLTA) versus professional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 3 Receptive
language: reading comprehension. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 5.4. Comparison 5 Volunteer-facilitated SLT (SLTA) versus professional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 4 Receptive
language: other. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 5.5. Comparison 5 Volunteer-facilitated SLT (SLTA) versus professional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 5 Expressive
language: spoken. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 5.6. Comparison 5 Volunteer-facilitated SLT (SLTA) versus professional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 6 Expressive
language: repetition. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 5.7. Comparison 5 Volunteer-facilitated SLT (SLTA) versus professional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 7 Expressive
language: written. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 5.8. Comparison 5 Volunteer-facilitated SLT (SLTA) versus professional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 8 Severity of
impairment: Aphasia Battery Score. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 5.9. Comparison 5 Volunteer-facilitated SLT (SLTA) versus professional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 9 Number of
drop-outs for any reason. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 6.1. Comparison 6 Group SLT (SLTA) versus 1-to-1 SLT (SLTB), Outcome 1 Receptive language: auditory
comprehension. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 6.2. Comparison 6 Group SLT (SLTA) versus 1-to-1 SLT (SLTB), Outcome 2 Receptive language: other. .
Analysis 6.3. Comparison 6 Group SLT (SLTA) versus 1-to-1 SLT (SLTB), Outcome 3 Expressive language: spoken.
Analysis 6.4. Comparison 6 Group SLT (SLTA) versus 1-to-1 SLT (SLTB), Outcome 4 Expressive language: repetition.
Analysis 6.5. Comparison 6 Group SLT (SLTA) versus 1-to-1 SLT (SLTB), Outcome 5 Expressive language: written.
Analysis 6.6. Comparison 6 Group SLT (SLTA) versus 1-to-1 SLT (SLTB), Outcome 6 Severity of impairment: Aphasia
Battery Score. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 6.7. Comparison 6 Group SLT (SLTA) versus 1-to-1 SLT (SLTB), Outcome 7 Severity of impairment: Aphasia
Battery Score (3-month follow up). . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 6.8. Comparison 6 Group SLT (SLTA) versus 1-to-1 SLT (SLTB), Outcome 8 Number of drop-outs for any
reason. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 7.1. Comparison 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 1 Functional
communication. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 7.2. Comparison 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 2 Receptive language:
auditory comprehension - word. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 7.3. Comparison 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 3 Receptive language:
other auditory comprehension. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 7.4. Comparison 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 4 Receptive language:
auditory comprehension (treated items). . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 7.5. Comparison 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 5 Receptive language:
reading comprehension. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 7.6. Comparison 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 6 Receptive language:
other.
. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 7.7. Comparison 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 7 Expressive language:
spoken naming. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)
Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

119
119
120
121
121
122
123
123
124
124
125
126
127
128
128
129
130
130
131
132
132
133
134
135
136
137
137
138
ii

Analysis 7.8. Comparison 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 8 Expressive language:
spoken sentence construction. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 7.9. Comparison 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 9 Expressive language:
other spoken tasks. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 7.10. Comparison 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 10 Expressive language:
spoken (treated items). . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 7.11. Comparison 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 11 Expressive language:
repetition. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 7.12. Comparison 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 12 Expressive language:
written. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 7.13. Comparison 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 13 Severity of
impairment. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 7.14. Comparison 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 14 Number of dropouts for any reason. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 8.1. Comparison 8 Operant training SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 1 Receptive language:
auditory comprehension. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 8.2. Comparison 8 Operant training SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 2 Receptive language:
other.
. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 8.3. Comparison 8 Operant training SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 3 Expressive
language: spoken. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 8.4. Comparison 8 Operant training SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 4 Expressive
language: written. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 8.5. Comparison 8 Operant training SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 5 Severity of
impairment. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 9.1. Comparison 9 Semantic SLT (SLTA) versus phonological SLT (SLT B), Outcome 1 Functional
communication. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 9.2. Comparison 9 Semantic SLT (SLTA) versus phonological SLT (SLT B), Outcome 2 Receptive language:
auditory. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 9.3. Comparison 9 Semantic SLT (SLTA) versus phonological SLT (SLT B), Outcome 3 Receptive language:
reading. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 9.4. Comparison 9 Semantic SLT (SLTA) versus phonological SLT (SLT B), Outcome 4 Expressive language:
repetition. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 9.5. Comparison 9 Semantic SLT (SLTA) versus phonological SLT (SLT B), Outcome 5 Number of drop-outs for
any reason. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 10.1. Comparison 10 Filmed programmed instruction SLT (SLT A) versus non-programmed activity SLT (SLTB),
Outcome 1 Receptive language: auditory. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Analysis 10.2. Comparison 10 Filmed programmed instruction SLT (SLT A) versus non-programmed activity SLT (SLTB),
Outcome 2 Receptive language: reading. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
ADDITIONAL TABLES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
APPENDICES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
WHATS NEW . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
HISTORY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
CONTRIBUTIONS OF AUTHORS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
DECLARATIONS OF INTEREST . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
SOURCES OF SUPPORT . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
INDEX TERMS
. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

139
140
141
142
142
143
144
144
145
146
147
147
148
149
149
150
150
151
151
152
161
166
167
167
167
168
168

iii

[Intervention Review]

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke


Helen Kelly2, Marian C Brady1 , Pam Enderby3
1 Nursing, Midwifery and Allied Health Professions Research Unit, Glasgow Caledonian University, Glasgow, UK. 2 (a) Nursing,
Midwifery and Allied Health Professions Research Unit, University of Stirling, Stirling, UK, (b) Speech and Hearing Sciences, Queen
Margaret University, Edinburgh, UK. 3 School of Health and Related Research, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, UK

Contact address: Marian C Brady, Nursing, Midwifery and Allied Health Professions Research Unit, Glasgow Caledonian University,
Cowcaddens Road, Glasgow, G4 0BA, UK. m.brady@gcal.ac.uk.
Editorial group: Cochrane Stroke Group.
Publication status and date: Edited (no change to conclusions), published in Issue 7, 2010.
Review content assessed as up-to-date: 8 November 2009.
Citation: Kelly H, Brady MC, Enderby P. Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke. Cochrane Database of Systematic
Reviews 2010, Issue 5. Art. No.: CD000425. DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD000425.pub2.
Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

ABSTRACT
Background
Aphasia is an acquired language impairment following brain damage which affects some or all language modalities: expression and
understanding of speech, reading and writing. Approximately one-third of people who have a stroke experience aphasia.
Objectives
To assess the effectiveness of speech and language therapy (SLT) for aphasia following stroke.
Search strategy
We searched the Cochrane Stroke Group Trials Register (last searched April 2009), MEDLINE (1966 to April 2009) and CINAHL
(1982 to April 2009). In an effort to identify further published, unpublished and ongoing trials we handsearched the International
Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, searched reference lists of relevant articles and contacted other researchers and
authors.
Selection criteria
Randomised controlled trials comparing SLT versus no SLT, SLT versus social support or stimulation, and one SLT intervention
versus another SLT intervention. SLT refers to a formal speech and language therapy intervention that aims to improve language and
communication abilities and in turn levels of communicative activity and participation. Social support and stimulation refers to an
intervention which provides social support or communication stimulation but does not include targeted therapeutic interventions.
Direct comparisons of different SLT interventions refers to SLT interventions that differ in terms of duration, intensity, frequency or
method of intervention or in the theoretical basis for the SLT approach.
Data collection and analysis
Two review authors independently extracted the data and assessed the quality of included trials. We sought missing data from study
investigators if necessary.
Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)
Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Main results
We included 30 trials (41 paired comparisons) in the review: 14 subcomparisons (1064 participants) compared SLT with no SLT;
six subcomparisons (279 participants) compared SLT with social support and stimulation; and 21 subcomparisons (732 participants)
compared two approaches to SLT. In general, the trials randomised small numbers of participants across a range of characteristics (age,
time since stroke and severity profiles), interventions and outcomes. Suitable statistical data were unavailable for several measures.
Authors conclusions
This review shows some indication of the effectiveness of SLT for people with aphasia following stroke. We also observed a consistency in
the direction of results which favoured intensive SLT over conventional SLT, though significantly more people withdrew from intensive
SLT than conventional SLT. SLT facilitated by a therapist-trained and supervised volunteer appears to be as effective as the provision
of SLT by a professional. There was insufficient evidence to draw any conclusions in relation to the effectiveness of one SLT approach
over another.

PLAIN LANGUAGE SUMMARY


Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke
Language problems following a stroke are called aphasia (or dysphasia). About one-third of all people who experience stroke develop
aphasia, which can affect one or more areas of communication (speaking, understanding spoken words, reading and writing). Speech
and language therapists are involved in the assessment, diagnosis and treatment of aphasia at all stages of recovery, and work closely with
the person with aphasia and their carers. There is no universally accepted treatment that can be applied to every person with aphasia.
We identified 30 trials involving 1840 randomised participants that were suitable for inclusion in this review. Overall, the review shows
evidence from randomised trials to suggest there may be a benefit from speech and language therapy but there was insufficient evidence
to indicate the best approach to delivering speech and language therapy.

BACKGROUND
The term aphasia (less commonly referred to as dysphasia) is used
to describe an acquired loss or impairment of the language system following brain damage (Benson 1996) and excludes other
communication difficulties attributed to sensory loss, confusion,
dementia or speech difficulties due to muscular weakness or dysfunction such as dysarthria. The most common cause of aphasia is
a cerebrovascular accident (commonly known as stroke), mainly
to the left hemisphere, where the language function of the brain
is usually situated for right-handed people. About one-third of all
people who experience a stroke develop aphasia (Engelter 2006;
Laska 2001). The aphasic population is heterogeneous, with individual profiles of language impairment varying in terms of severity
and degree of involvement across the modalities of language processing, including the expression and comprehension of speech,
reading, writing and gesture (Code 2003; Parr 1997). Variation in
severity of expressive impairments, for example, may range from
the individual experiencing occasional word-finding difficulties
to having no effective means of communication. The severity of
aphasia can also change over time as one area of language difficulty

may improve while others remain impaired. The impact and the
consequential implications of having aphasia for the individuals
themselves, their families and society highlight the importance of
the effective management and rehabilitation of language difficulties caused by aphasia.
The primary aim of speech and language therapy (SLT*) in aphasia
management and rehabilitation is to maximise individuals ability to communicate. Speech and language therapists are typically
responsible for the assessment, diagnosis and, where appropriate,
rehabilitation of aphasia arising as a result of stroke. The ability to
successfully communicate a message via spoken, written or nonverbal modalities (or a combination of these) within day-to-day
interactions is known as functional communication. Recent developments have seen speech and language therapists working closely
with the person with aphasia, and in partnership with their families and carers to maximise the individuals functional communication. There is no universally accepted treatment that can be
applied to every patient with aphasia and therapists select from a
variety of methods to manage and facilitate rehabilitation includ-

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

ing, for example, impairment-based therapy and social participation approaches. We undertook this review update to incorporate
new evidence, new systematic review methodologies and to reflect
recent developments in clinical practice. Details of the differences
between this version and the original review published in 1999 are
detailed below.
* For the purposes of clarity within this review we have reserved
the abbreviation of SLT for speech and language therapy alone.

Amendments to the original review


Following close inspection of the original review and detailed discussion amongst this review team, we made adjustments to the
review, many of which reflect changes in Cochrane procedures,
review methodologies, and style and structure in the time since the
publication of the original review. These amendments were ratified
by the Cochrane Stroke Group Editorial Board on 23 November
2006.

Types of outcome measures


We have refined the Types of outcome measures to a single primary outcome measure of functional communication. Secondary
outcomes include other measures of communication (receptive or
expressive language, or both), psychosocial outcomes, patient satisfaction with the intervention, number of participant drop-outs
for any reason, non-compliance with the allocated intervention,
economic outcomes (such as cost to the patient, carers, families,
health service and society) and carer or family satisfaction. Data
relating to death, morbidity and cognitive skills were extracted in
the original review but, on reflection, we did not consider these to
be relevant indicators of the effectiveness of a speech and language
therapy intervention and we therefore excluded them from this
update. The original review had measures of overall functional status (e.g. Barthel Index) as one of a number of primary outcomes.
As described above, we focused on a single primary outcome (in
line with the current review methodology).

Data extraction tool


Background
We have updated the Background section to include a definition
of speech and language therapy and aphasia, and to reflect current
approaches and rationale to speech and language therapy interventions and outcomes.

We could not obtain the original data extraction tool, therefore


two of the review authors (HK and MB) created and piloted a new
one before use.

Search methods for identification of studies


Objectives
We amended the Objectives to a single statement according to the
standard format of Cochrane reviews; that is, to examine the effectiveness of speech and language therapy interventions for aphasia
following stroke.

Types of studies
It was unclear whether or not quasi-randomised controlled trials
were included in the original review. We have excluded quasirandomised trials in this update.

Re-running the original search strategy for the MEDLINE and


CINAHL databases raised over 12.6 million references. Therefore, Brenda Thomas, the Cochrane Stroke Group Trials Search
Co-ordinator, devised up-to-date search strategies. The International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders (previously named the British Journal of Disorders of Communication, the
European Journal of Disorders of Communication and the International Journal of Disorders of Communication) was handsearched
from 1969 to 2005. This journal has been indexed by MEDLINE
since 2006 and was thus included in our electronic searches from
this date.

Description of studies
Types of interventions
We have compressed the Types of interventions into three broad
categories: SLT versus no SLT intervention, SLT versus social support or stimulation, and SLT intervention A versus SLT intervention B (where A and B refer to two different types of therapeutic
interventions or approaches).

The original review listed studies other than identified randomised


controlled trials in the Characteristics of excluded studies table, including single case or case series studies. As there are a vast number
of such studies, the updated table now only presents potentially
relevant studies that appear to be randomised but which were excluded for other reasons (for example quasi-randomised or where
aphasia-specific data could not be extracted).

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Comparisons
Mid-trial outcome scores were included in the original review. We
have focused our reporting on post-intervention and follow-up
scores. We have not included analysis of the number of participants
who deteriorated on particular outcome measures.

Other amendments
As we were unable to obtain the extraction sheets for the trials included in the original review, we cross-checked the data extracted
for the original review with the available published and unpublished data. We made some amendments, including exclusion of
some studies and categorising the methods of allocation concealment used in the included trials.
In this review update we took the decision to exclude quasi-randomised studies and so one study, included in the original review,
has been excluded from this review update (Hartman 1987).
On review of the data from another trial (Kinsey 1986), we decided
that the reported comparison was not a therapy intervention as
such, but rather a comparison of task performance (computerbased or with a therapist). We thus excluded this trial from the
review update.
The allocation concealment for one study (MacKay 1988) was
considered inadequate in the original review. We failed to get
confirmation of the method of allocation from the authors and
therefore we amended the allocation for this trial to unclear.
The original review included a matched control group of no SLT
intervention for one trial (Prins 1989). However, unlike the other
groups in this trial, this group was not randomised, therefore we
have excluded it from this update.
Another study (Shewan 1984) had been excluded from the original
review on the grounds that it was not a randomised controlled
trial. Discussion with the trialists has since revealed that it was a
randomised controlled trial, and we have now included it in the
review.
The original review included outcomes relating to the impact of
SLT on the emotional wellbeing of family members (Lincoln
1984a). We do not feel that such outcomes directly relate to the
aims of this review and so we have not included these measures.

1. SLT is more effective than no SLT;


2. SLT is more effective than social support and stimulation;
3. One SLT intervention (SLTA) is more effective than
another SLT intervention (SLTB).
SLT intervention A or B refers to variations in intervention that
differ in duration, intensity, frequency, method or in the theoretical
basis of the approach to the intervention (for example, cognitive
neurological versus psychosocial based interventions).

METHODS

Criteria for considering studies for this review

Types of studies
Randomised controlled trials that evaluated (one or more) interventions designed to improve language or communication. We
included trials that recruited participants with mixed aetiologies
or impairments provided it was possible to extract the data specific
to individuals with post-stroke aphasia. We did not employ any
language restriction.

Types of participants
Adults who had acquired aphasia as a result of a stroke.

Types of interventions
The groupings presented in the original review were compressed
into three broad groups for this review update. We have included
trials which reported a comparison between a group that received
a SLT intervention designed to have an impact on communication
and a group that received:
no SLT intervention; or
social support and stimulation; or
an alternative SLT intervention.

New information added to the review


Following an extensive search up to April 2009, we identified an
additional 20 trials as suitable for inclusion in the review. There are
now 30 included trials involving 1840 randomised participants.

OBJECTIVES
To examine the effectiveness of SLT for aphasia after stroke and
in particular if:

Speech and language therapy (SLT)

We considered SLT interventions to be any form of targeted practice tasks or methodologies with the aim of improving language or
communication abilities. These are typically delivered by speech
and language therapists. In the UK, Speech and language therapist is a protected professional title and refers to individuals holding a professional qualification recognised by the Royal College of
Speech and Language Therapists and registered with the Health
Professions Council, UK. For the purposes of this review we have

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

extended this definition to include therapists belonging to a body


of similar professional standing elsewhere in the world.
We are aware that the speech and language therapy profession
does not exist in many countries and so in trials conducted in
such settings where other clinical staff (for example, medical or
nursing staff ) led targeted interventions that aimed to improve
participants communicative functioning we have included these
interventions within this review as speech and language therapy
interventions.
We also recognise that current rehabilitation practice may include
SLT interventions that aim to improve communicative functioning but are delivered by non-therapists (family members, SLT assistants, SLT students, voluntary support groups). Where those
delivering the intervention have received training from a speech
and language therapist and deliver an intervention designed by a
speech and language therapist, we have described these as volunteer-facilitated SLT interventions.
Social support and stimulation

Daily Living (CADL) (Holland 1980) or the Communicative Effectiveness Index (CETI) (Lomas 1989).

Secondary outcomes

Given the lack of a comprehensive, reliable, valid and globally accepted functional communication evaluation tool, surrogate outcome measures of communication ability include formal measures
of receptive language (oral, written and gestural), expressive language (oral, written and gestural) or overall level of severity of
aphasia where receptive and expressive language are measured using language batteries. Such tools might include, for example, the
Western Aphasia Battery (WAB) (Kertesz 1982) or the Porch Index of Communicative Abilities (PICA) (Porch 1967). Other secondary outcomes of relevance to this review include psychosocial
impact (i.e. impact on psychological or social wellbeing including depression, anxiety and distress); patient satisfaction with intervention; number of drop-outs (i.e. the number of participants
dropping out at treatment or follow-up phases for any reason);
compliance with allocated intervention (i.e. the number of participants voluntarily withdrawing from their allocated intervention);
economic outcomes (such as costs to the patient, carers, families,
health service and society), and carer and family satisfaction. Measures of overall functional status (e.g. Barthel) were extracted in
the original review as one of a number of primary outcomes. We
also extracted these data, where available, but this information is
now presented as a patient descriptor within the Characteristics of
included studies table. A full list of outcome measures included in
the review and their references can be found in Appendix 4.

Social support and stimulation refers to an intervention that provides social support or stimulation but does not include targeted
therapeutic interventions that aim to resolve participants expressive or receptive speech and language impairments. Interventions
in this category might include, for example, emotional, psychological or creative interventions (such as art, dance or music) as delivered by other healthcare professionals (for example, art, physical
or music therapists). Other social stimulation interventions, such
as conversation or other informal, unstructured communicative
interactions are also included in this category.
We did not include pharmacological interventions for aphasia in
this review as they are addressed elsewhere (Greener 2001).

Search methods for identification of studies

Types of outcome measures

See the Specialized register section in the Cochrane Stroke Group


module.

Primary outcomes

Electronic searches

The primary outcome to indicate the effectiveness of an intervention that aims to improve communicative ability must be the
ability to communicate in real world settings, i.e. functional communication. Providing a definition for the concept of functional
communication is problematic and even more difficult to evaluate. The ability to functionally communicate relates to language
or communicational skills sufficient to permit the transmission of
a message via spoken, written or non-verbal modalities, or a combination of these channels. Success is typically and naturalistically
demonstrated through successful communication of the message
- the speaker communicates their message and the listener understands the message communicated. Attempts to measure this
communication success formally vary from analysis of discourse
interaction in real life to sampling of specific discourse tasks. Other
more formal tools might include the Communicative Abilities of

We searched the Cochrane Stroke Group Trials Register, which was


last searched by the Managing Editor on 7 April 2009. In addition,
we searched MEDLINE (January 1999 to April 2009) (Appendix
1) and CINAHL (January 1999 to April 2009) (Appendix 2)
using comprehensive search strategies. For the previous version of
the review searches of MEDLINE (1966 to 1998) and CINAHL
(1982 to 1998) were carried out using simple combinations of text
words describing aphasia and speech and language therapy.
Searching other resources
1. We handsearched the International Journal of Language and
Communication Disorders (formerly the International Journal of
Disorders of Communication, the European Journal of Disorders of
Communication and the British Journal of Disorders of

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Communication) from 1969 to December 2005. Since 2006 this


journal has been indexed in MEDLINE so our comprehensive
electronic search identified any relevant trials published in the
journal after that date.
2. We checked reference lists of all relevant articles to identify
other potentially relevant randomised studies.
3. We contacted all British universities and colleges where
SLTs are trained and all relevant Special Interest Groups in the
UK to enquire about any relevant published, unpublished or
ongoing studies.
4. We approached colleagues and authors of relevant
randomised trials to identify additional studies of relevance to
this review.
We did not impose any language restrictions.

Data collection and analysis

Selection of studies
Our selection criteria for inclusion in this review were:
1. the study participants included people with aphasia as a
result of stroke;
2. the SLT intervention was designed to have an impact on
communication; and
3. the methodological design was a randomised controlled
trial.
One review author (HK) screened references identified through
the search strategy described above and obtained hard copies
of all trials that fulfilled the listed inclusion criteria. In the
Characteristics of excluded studies table, we have listed studies
judged ineligible for inclusion together with reasons for their exclusion. Two review authors (HK and MB) independently made
the decision whether to include or exclude studies and they resolved any disagreements through discussion.

Data extraction and management


The data extraction form used in the original review was unavailable so we created and piloted another for use in this review update.
Two review authors (HK and MB) independently confirmed the
data for the trials as included in the original review and extracted
the data for the additional trials included in the update. Any disagreements were resolved through discussion and we extracted the
following data: number of sites; methods of randomisation; blinding; attrition from intervention; co-interventions; confounder details; number of participants; age; education; handedness; gender;
native language; severity of aphasia; time post-onset; frequency
and duration of therapy; details of intervention; outcome measures used and time points; evidence of an a priori sample size
calculation; intention-to-treat analysis; and summary data. We at-

tempted to contact investigators for any missing data (or data in


a suitable format) for inclusion in the review.
If we identified a cross-over trial design, we considered the suitability of the trial for inclusion in the review in view of a range
of factors including the intervention(s) used, the timing of the intervention(s), the impact of any treatment carry over and whether
data from relevant paired comparisons within the trial were available. Whenever possible, in such cases we sought individual patient data from the trialists.

Assessment of risk of bias in included studies


We assessed the trials for methodological quality, paying attention to whether there was protection from the following types of
bias: selection bias (i.e. true random sequencing and true concealment up to the time of allocation), performance bias (i.e. differences in other types of treatment (co-interventions) between the
groups), attrition bias (i.e. withdrawal after trial entry), and detection bias (i.e. unmasked assessment of outcome). We coded concealed allocation as adequate, unclear, or inadequate according
to the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions
(Higgins 2008). In addition, we extracted information on whether
power calculations and intention-to-treat analyses were employed.
In some cases, for example where all participants were accounted
for in the final results, this was not applicable.

Measures of treatment effect


We conducted the review using Review Manager (RevMan 2008)
for statistical analysis. We have recorded descriptive information
for each trial (characteristics of participants, interventions and
outcomes) in the Characteristics of included studies table and issues relating to the methodological quality of the trial in the Risk
of bias tables. Where trials made a similar comparison and were
judged sufficiently similar in respect of their descriptive information, we pooled the summary data (where available) using metaanalysis. We expressed continuous data as differences in means or
standardised difference in means and dichotomised data as odds
ratio (OR). We used 95% confidence intervals (CI) throughout
the review.
The results of the trials in this review reported measures based on
differences in final value scores (scores taken at the end of the intervention) and change-from-baseline scores (also known as change
scores). Although the mean differences based on change-frombaseline scores in randomised trials can generally be assumed to
address the same intervention effects as mean difference analysis
based on final value scores, change-from-baseline scores are given
higher weights in analysis than final value scores (Higgins 2008).
For this reason, we have used final value scores within the metaanalyses wherever possible. We will not report change-from-baseline scores unless they are the only available values used to report
trial results (Higgins 2008).

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Assessment of heterogeneity
We assessed heterogeneity using the I statistic with a value of
greater than 50% indicating substantial heterogeneity. Where
we observed substantial heterogeneity we used a random-effects
model.

Data synthesis
Where a single outcome measure was assessed and reported across
trials using different measurement tools, we presented these data
in a meta-analysis using a standardised mean differences summary
statistic. In cases where the direction of measurement differed it
was necessary to adjust the direction of some measures to ensure
that all the scales operated in the same direction. For example,
measures of comprehension ability generally increase with increasing ability, but in some cases (e.g. the Token Test) improving comprehension skills might be reflected by decreasing scores and so it
was necessary to multiply the mean values by -1 to ensure that all
the scales operated in the same direction. Standard deviation values were unaffected and we have presented these within the metaanalysis without the need for a directional change.
In cases where only partial summary data were reported, for example mean final value scores were available but standard deviations
were unavailable (Wertz 1981), we attempted to calculate these
values from available information. When this was not possible we
imputed the standard deviation to facilitate inclusion of the trial
within the review by using a standard deviation value from a similar participant group (Higgins 2008). We have reported details
of where the imputed standard deviation values have come from
within the text. Where there was a choice of possible standard
deviation values, we took the approach of imputing the highest
and lowest values to ensure that both methods provided a similar
overall conclusion and then used the highest value in the presentation of the trial within the forest plot.
Where results in a particular comparison were only available in a
mixture of final value and change from baseline scores, we presented these data graphically using standardised mean differences
but we were unable to pool these results in a meta-analysis.

Subgroup analysis and investigation of heterogeneity


We did not plan any subgroup analyses.

Sensitivity analysis
The original review did not include any planned sensitivity analyses. However, in this updated review we aimed to reflect developments in clinical practice including trials where SLT interventions
were delivered or facilitated by non-speech and language therapists. We planned to conduct sensitivity analyses to evaluate any
impact the inclusion of these groups of trials may have had on the
results of the review.

RESULTS

Description of studies
See: Characteristics of included studies; Characteristics of
excluded studies; Characteristics of studies awaiting classification;
Characteristics of ongoing studies.
The original review included 12 trials. We revisited the decision
taken in the original review to include Kinsey 1986 and Hartman
1987. Quasi-randomised trials such as Hartman 1987 have been
excluded from this review update while Kinsey 1986 reports a
comparison of methods of providing therapy materials rather than
a comparison of therapy interventions. Thus of the original 12
trials included in the review, 10 trials remain in this review update. In addition, we revised the decision to exclude one other
trial (Shewan 1984) from the original review following communication with the trialists who confirmed that it was a randomised
controlled trial.
Results of the search
In our substantially updated search we identified an additional
42 studies of potential relevance to the review (January 1999 to
April 2009). Eight of the 42 newly identified trials required translation; six Chinese (Gu 2003; Jufeng 2005; Liu 2006, Wang 2004;
Wu 2004; Zhang 2004), one Dutch (van Steenbrugge 1981) and
one German paper (Jungblut 2004) for which the translation was
provided by the author. Nine studies are ongoing (ACTNow;
IHCOP; Kukkonen 2007; Laska 2008; Maher 2008; RATS2;
SEATAS; SP-I-RiT; Varley 2005); these may be eligible for inclusion in the review at a later date. These studies are detailed in
the Characteristics of ongoing studies table. One study is awaiting translation (Liu 2006). In total we identified 20 new trials as
eligible for inclusion in this review update.
Included studies
We have included a total of 30 trials in this review (10 from
the original review and 20 identified for this update), which randomised a total of 1840 participants. Six trials randomised individuals across three or more groups (trial arms) but for the purposes
of meta-analyses we have presented and pooled the data within
paired comparisons. Thus in this review, we have presented the
data from these five trials in paired subcomparisons. For example, data from Jufeng 2005 were divided into three subcomparisons of (1) group SLT versus no SLT (Jufeng 2005i), (2) individual SLT versus no SLT (Jufeng 2005ii) and (3) group SLT versus individual SLT (Jufeng 2005iii). Other subcomparisons were
Katz 1997i; Katz 1997ii; Lincoln 1982i, Lincoln 1982ii, Lincoln
1982iii, Shewan 1984i; Shewan 1984ii; Shewan 1984iii; Smith
1981i; Smith 1981ii; Smith 1981iii; Wertz 1986i; Wertz 1986ii;
Wertz 1986iii. Further details can be found in the Characteristics

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

of included studies. As we used paired subcomparisons within this


review, there was a risk of including the same group of participants
(usually the control group) twice in a single meta-analysis. In such
cases we split the number of participants in the control group
across the two subcomparisons (Higgins 2008). In the case of continuous data the mean and standard deviation values remained the
same. In the case of dichotomous data both the number of events
and total number of patients would be split across the relevant
number of arms.
Four trials (eight subcomparisons) employed a cross-over design
(Elman 1999; Lincoln 1982i; Lincoln 1982ii; Lincoln 1982iii;
Lincoln 1984b; Wertz 1986i; Wertz 1986ii; Wertz 1986iii). We
carefully considered the suitability of each cross-over trial for inclusion within the review. We considered factors including the
suitability of the design, the intervention(s) used, the timing of
the intervention(s), the impact of any treatment carry over and
finally whether data from relevant paired comparisons from the
cross-over data were available. For five subcomparisons we only
extracted data up to the point of cross-over (Elman 1999; Lincoln
1982iii; Lincoln 1984b; Wertz 1986i; Wertz 1986ii). In some cases
though, the treatment that participants were allocated to receive
following cross-over was no SLT. In these cases, the no SLT
input after cross-over could be used as a follow-up period. In some
cases (e.g. Wertz 1986iii) it was also possible to make suitable
paired comparisons within the trial arms during this phase.
In contrast, Lincoln 1982 was also cross-over in design with participants randomly allocated to one of four groups with a sequence of
1. SLT versus no SLT

We included 14 subcomparisons in this section (Doesborgh


2004b; Jufeng 2005i; Jufeng 2005ii; Katz 1997i; Katz 1997ii;
Lincoln 1984a; Lyon 1997; MacKay 1988; Smania 2006; Smith
1981i; Smith 1981ii; Wertz 1986i; Wertz 1986ii; Wu 2004) involving 1064 randomised participants. The SLT intervention was
facilitated by a therapist-trained volunteer in two subcomparisons
(MacKay 1988; Wertz 1986ii) and by a doctor or nurse in three
subcomparisons conducted in China, where the speech and language therapy profession does not exist (Jufeng 2005i; Jufeng
2005ii; Wu 2004). Two additional trials (Prins 1989; Shewan
1984) also compared groups that did and did not receive SLT
but the participants were not randomly assigned to these no SLT
groups and were thus excluded from this review.
The subcomparisons in this section employed a range of SLT
interventions, namely conventional SLT (Jufeng 2005ii; Lincoln
1984a; Smania 2006; Smith 1981ii; Wertz 1986i; Wu 2004),
intensive SLT (Smith 1981i), group SLT (Jufeng 2005i), volunteer-facilitated (MacKay 1988; Wertz 1986ii), computer-mediated SLT (Doesborgh 2004b; Katz 1997i; Katz 1997ii) and
functionally-based SLT involving a communicative partner (Lyon
1997).
Most participants randomised to the no SLT groups received no

interventions that included one active treatment or placebo either


preceded by or followed by conventional SLT. We were very fortunate that the unpublished individual patient data were available
for this review update. This access to the data, the design, nature
and manner of SLT delivery within the trial and the clinical relevance of the comparisons made it possible to include two paired
comparisons of those groups within the review:
SLT + Operant Training versus SLT + Social Support
(Lincoln 1982i);
Operant Training + SLT versus Social Support + SLT
(Lincoln 1982ii).
In addition, by taking the individual data at the point of measurement prior to the cross-over it was also possible to extract and
compare the data from those that had received conventional SLT
and compare it to those participants that received a social support
and stimulation intervention (Lincoln 1982iii).
We have presented data from 41 subcomparisons as they relate to
the effectiveness of SLT for aphasia following stroke, within three
comparisons: 1. SLT versus no SLT; 2. SLT versus social support
and stimulation; and 3. SLT A versus SLT B. We have presented
details of data within each comparison below with further details
on each subcomparison available in the Characteristics of included
studies table. Further participant details can be found in Table 1,
an overview of the SLT interventions can be found in Appendix 3 ,
while details of the assessment tools used can be found in Appendix
4. A summary of all the findings of the results is available at the
end of the results section.
alternative treatment or support (Doesborgh 2004b; Jufeng 2005i;
Jufeng 2005ii; Katz 1997i; Lincoln 1984a; Lyon 1997; MacKay
1988; Wertz 1986i; Wertz 1986ii; Wu 2004). Only four control
arms described an intervention within these no SLT groups. In
three cases we considered the control interventions to be similar
to standard post-stroke care in the UK - participants were visited
at home by a health visitor (Smith 1981i; Smith 1981ii) or they
received limb apraxia therapy (Smania 2006). In addition, a fourth
control group received computer-based cognitive tasks (arcadestyle games) (Katz 1997ii) that had been specifically designed not
to target language rehabilitation. In all four cases we included these
groups as no SLT control groups in the review.
SLT interventions were delivered across a wide range of times after
the onset of aphasia with timings difficult to summarise because
of a lack of detailed reporting. Some trialists recruited participants
in the early stages after the onset of stroke - up to 10 weeks (
Lincoln 1984a) or up to six months (Wertz 1986i; Wertz 1986ii).
Other trials recruited participants longer after stroke, for example
between two months and three years after stroke (Smania 2006).
In some trials participants were recruited a year or more after their
stroke - up to 17 months (Doesborgh 2004b); two years (MacKay
1988) (61% of participants); 10 years (13 to124 months) (Lyon

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

1997); 19 years (Katz 1997i); or up to 22 years (Katz 1997ii)


after the onset of aphasia. Five subcomparisons failed to report
the timing of the SLT intervention in relation to the onset of
participants aphasia (Jufeng 2005i; Jufeng 2005ii; Smith 1981i;
Smith 1981ii; Wu 2004).
The frequency of SLT was reported as hours per week or number of times daily. SLT was provided weekly for up to two
hours (Doesborgh 2004b; Lincoln 1984a; Smith 1981ii), three
hours (Katz 1997i; Katz 1997ii; Smania 2006), four hours (Smith
1981i), six hours (MacKay 1988), eight hours (Lyon 1997) or 10
hours (Wertz 1986i; Wertz 1986ii). SLT was provided daily (duration unclear) within two subcomparisons (Jufeng 2005i; Jufeng
2005ii) while Wu 2004 did not report the frequency of the SLT
intervention. All SLT was delivered for at least a month, but in
some cases SLT was provided for up to three months (Doesborgh
2004b; Jufeng 2005i; Jufeng 2005ii; Smania 2006; Wertz 1986i;
Wertz 1986ii); between five and six months (Katz 1997i; Katz
1997ii; Lincoln 1984a; Lyon 1997; Wu 2004) or for up to a year
(MacKay 1988; Smith 1981i; Smith 1981ii).
The subcomparisons used a wide range of outcome measures including functional communication, receptive language, expressive language, severity of impairment, psychosocial impact and
economic outcomes. One of the 14 subcomparisons did not report outcome measures (Wu 2004). Nine subcomparisons carried
out follow-up assessments at two months (Smania 2006), three
months (Jufeng 2005i; Jufeng 2005ii; Wertz 1986i; Wertz 1986ii),
six months (MacKay 1988) and 12 months (MacKay 1988) after
SLT.

2. SLT versus social support and stimulation

We included six subcomparisons in this section (David 1982;


Elman 1999; Lincoln 1982iii; Rochon 2005; Shewan 1984ii;
Shewan 1984iii) with 279 randomised participants.
A range of SLT approaches were reported including conventional
SLT (David 1982; Lincoln 1982iii; Shewan 1984iii), group SLT
(Elman 1999), language-oriented SLT (Shewan 1984ii) and sentence mapping SLT (Rochon 2005). The social support and stimulation interventions were provided by volunteers not known to
the participants with aphasia (David 1982), nursing staff (Shewan
1984ii; Shewan 1984iii), speech and language therapists (Lincoln
1982iii; Rochon 2005) or through other social group activities
including movement classes, creative arts groups, church activities
or support groups (Elman 1999). David 1982 provided the volunteers with detailed information on their patients communication
problems and they were instructed to encourage their patient to
communicate as well as possible. Similarly, the nursing staff volunteers (Shewan 1984ii; Shewan 1984iii) were given some information about aphasia and instructed to stimulate communication
to the best of their ability. In all three subcomparisons the volunteers were given no guidance or instruction in SLT techniques.
Participants were recruited with aphasia of various duration -

up to four weeks (Shewan 1984ii; Shewan 1984iii); up to three


years (David 1982; Lincoln 1982iii), seven months to 28 years
(Elman 1999) or between two and nine years (Rochon 2005).
Interventions were provided weekly for up to two (David 1982;
Lincoln 1982iii), three (Shewan 1984ii; Shewan 1984iii) or five
hours (Elman 1999) over the course of one (Lincoln 1982iii), four
(Elman 1999), five (David 1982) or 12 months (Shewan 1984ii;
Shewan 1984iii).
Outcome measures used in this comparison included measures
of functional communication, receptive language, expressive language and levels of severity of impairment. Two subcomparisons
carried out follow-up measures at four weeks (Rochon 2005), three
months and six months (David 1982) after the treatment period.

3. SLT A versus SLT B

We included 21 subcomparisons (732 randomised participants)


in this section (Bakheit 2007; Denes 1996; Di Carlo 1980;
Doesborgh 2004a; Drummond 1981; Hinckley 2001; Jufeng
2005iii; Leal 1993; Lincoln 1982i; Lincoln 1982ii; Lincoln
1984b; Meikle 1979; Meinzer 2007; ORLA 2006; Prins
1989; Pulvermuller 2001; Shewan 1984i; Smith 1981iii; van
Steenbrugge 1981; Wertz 1981; Wertz 1986iii). Three subcomparisons (Bakheit 2007; Prins 1989; Shewan 1984) also reported additional groups but participants were not adequately randomised to these groups and so they have been excluded from this
review.
A wide range of SLT interventions were reported including functional SLT (Hinckley 2001), intensive SLT (Bakheit 2007; Denes
1996; ORLA 2006; Smith 1981iii), volunteer-facilitated SLT
(Meikle 1979; Meinzer 2007; Leal 1993; Wertz 1986iii), group
SLT (Jufeng 2005iii; Pulvermuller 2001; Wertz 1981) and taskspecific SLT (Drummond 1981; Prins 1989; Pulvermuller 2001;
van Steenbrugge 1981; Shewan 1984i) compared to conventional
SLT. Other trials compared a semantic approach to SLT with
a phonological approach (Doesborgh 2004a) and filmed programmed instructions with non-programmed activity (Di Carlo
1980).
The duration of participants aphasia ranged from up to a month
(Leal 1993; Shewan 1984i; Smith 1981iii; Wertz 1981), two
months (Bakheit 2007), six months (Denes 1996; Doesborgh
2004a; Wertz 1986iii), a year (Lincoln 1984b), two years (
Drummond 1981), three years (Lincoln 1982i), five years (van
Steenbrugge 1981; Meikle 1979), six years (Di Carlo 1980;
Meinzer 2007), eight years (Hinckley 2001), 16 years (Kinsey
1986), 17 years (Prins 1989) or 19 years (Pulvermuller 2001) after
the onset of aphasia. Jufeng 2005iii did not report the duration of
their participants aphasia.
Therapy was provided daily (Jufeng 2005iii) for up to three
hours (Meinzer 2007; Pulvermuller 2001 ), or weekly for up
to 30 minutes (Drummond 1981), an hour (Lincoln 1984b),
one-and-a half hours (Lincoln 1982i; Smith 1981iii), two hours

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

(Prins 1989; van Steenbrugge 1981), three hours (Di Carlo


1980; Doesborgh 2004a; Leal 1993; Shewan 1984i), four hours
(Meikle 1979; Smith 1981iii), five hours (Bakheit 2007; Denes
1996), eight hours (Wertz 1981), 10 hours (Wertz 1986iii)
or 20 hours (Hinckley 2001). Therapy was provided for two
weeks (Drummond 1981), four weeks (Jufeng 2005iii), five weeks
(Hinckley 2001; Pulvermuller 2001), eight weeks (Lincoln 1982i,
Lincoln 1984b), nine weeks (van Steenbrugge 1981), 12 weeks
(Bakheit 2007; Wertz 1986iii), 16 weeks (Lincoln 1984b), 30
weeks (Di Carlo 1980), five months (Prins 1989), up to six months
(Denes 1996; Leal 1993), nine months (Doesborgh 2004a), 10
months (Wertz 1981), a year (Shewan 1984i, Smith 1981iii) or
two years (Meikle 1979).
There was a wide range of outcome measures used in this comparison including measures of functional communication, receptive language, expressive language, severity of impairment and psychosocial impact. Follow-up assessments were carried out at six
weeks (Wertz 1986iii) and three months (Jufeng 2005iii; Bakheit
2007) following treatment.

Excluded studies
We excluded 13 studies (Cherney 2007; Cohen 1992; Cohen
1993; Gu 2003; Hartman 1987; Jungblut 2004; Kagan 2001;
Kinsey 1986; Meinzer 2005; Rudd 1997; Wang 2004; Wolfe
2000; Zhang 2004). Three additional studies had been excluded
from the original review (Kalra 1993; Stoicheff 1960; Wood
1984). Reasons for exclusion were primarily due to inadequate
randomisation and the unavailability of aphasia specific data (see
details in the Characteristics of excluded studies table).

Risk of bias in included studies


Two review authors independently reviewed the methodological
quality of the included studies and resolved disagreements through
discussion. Details can be found in the Risk of bias tables for each
of the subcomparisons in the Characteristics of included studies
table.
The number of participants randomised across subcomparisons included in the review ranged from five to 327 participants. Three comparisons randomised 10 participants or fewer
(Drummond 1981; Rochon 2005; van Steenbrugge 1981). Eleven
randomised between 11 and 20 participants (Denes 1996; Di
Carlo 1980; Doesborgh 2004b; Hinckley 2001; Lincoln 1982i;
Lincoln 1982ii; Lincoln 1982iii Lincoln 1984b; Meinzer 2007;
Pulvermuller 2001). Eleven randomised up to 50 participants
(Elman 1999; Katz 1997i; Katz 1997ii; Lyon 1997; Meikle 1979;
Prins 1989; Shewan 1984iii; Smania 2006; Smith 1981i; Smith
1981ii; Smith 1981iii). Thirteen randomised between 51 and
100 participants (Bakheit 2007; Doesborgh 2004a; Jufeng 2005i;
Jufeng 2005ii; Jufeng 2005iii; Leal 1993; MacKay 1988; Shewan
1984i; Shewan 1984ii; Wertz 1981; Wertz 1986i; Wertz 1986ii;

Wertz 1986iii) and three randomised more than 150 participants


(David 1982; Lincoln 1984a; Wu 2004) (see Table 1).
Of the 41 subcomparisons, only 17 listed both inclusion and exclusion criteria. Details of exclusion criteria were unavailable for
19 subcomparisons (Denes 1996; Di Carlo 1980; Hinckley 2001;
Jufeng 2005i; Jufeng 2005ii; Jufeng 2005iii; Katz 1997i; Katz
1997ii; Lincoln 1984b; Lyon 1997; MacKay 1988; Meikle 1979;
ORLA 2006; Prins 1989; Rochon 2005; van Steenbrugge 1981;
Wertz 1981; Wertz 1986i; Wertz 1986ii; Wertz 1986iii) and two
listed neither inclusion nor exclusion criteria (Drummond 1981;
Wu 2004). For details, see the Characteristics of included studies
table.
Suitable statistical data for communication outcomes were only
available for 28 of the 41 subcomparisons (Bakheit 2007; David
1982; Denes 1996; Di Carlo 1980; Doesborgh 2004a; Doesborgh
2004b; Hinckley 2001; Jufeng 2005i; Jufeng 2005ii; Jufeng
2005iii; Katz 1997i; Katz 1997ii; Lincoln 1982i; Lincoln 1982ii;
Lincoln 1982iii; Lincoln 1984a; Lincoln 1984b; Meikle 1979;
Meinzer 2007; ORLA 2006; Prins 1989; Rochon 2005; Smania
2006; van Steenbrugge 1981; Wertz 1981; Wertz 1986i; Wertz
1986ii; Wertz 1986iii). An additional nine subcomparisons contributed data on the trial drop-outs or withdrawals. Psychosocial
data were available for one subcomparison (Lincoln 1984a). Appropriate statistical data were not provided or could not be extracted in the remaining three subcomparisons (Drummond 1981;
Lyon 1997; Wu 2004).
There was a wide range of variation in the descriptions of the SLT
interventions. Most reported the use of a conventional SLT approach (Bakheit 2007; Denes 1996; Drummond 1981; Hinckley
2001; Leal 1993; Lincoln 1982iii; Lincoln 1984b; Prins 1989;
Pulvermuller 2001; Shewan 1984i; Shewan 1984iii; Smania 2006;
Smith 1981i; Smith 1981ii; Smith 1981iii; van Steenbrugge 1981;
Wertz 1981; Wertz 1986i; Wertz 1986iii; Wu 2004) or described
an intervention which reflects clinical practice where the therapist was responsible for design and delivery of the treatment
(David 1982; Jufeng 2005i; Jufeng 2005ii; Jufeng 2005iii; Lincoln
1984a; Meikle 1979). Other more prescriptive SLT interventions
were also evaluated (Denes 1996; Doesborgh 2004a; Doesborgh
2004b; Drummond 1981; Elman 1999; Hinckley 2001; Katz
1997i; Katz 1997ii; Kinsey 1986; Lincoln 1982i; Lincoln 1982ii;
Lincoln 1984a; Lyon 1997; MacKay 1988; Meinzer 2007; ORLA
2006; Prins 1989; Pulvermuller 2001; Rochon 2005; Wertz 1981;
Shewan 1984i; Shewan 1984ii; Smith 1981iii; van Steenbrugge
1981) and these will be detailed further in later sections.
Twenty-four subcomparisons reported similar groups at baseline
(Bakheit 2007; Denes 1996; Di Carlo 1980; Doesborgh 2004b;
Drummond 1981; Elman 1999; Hinckley 2001; Katz 1997i; Katz
1997ii; Leal 1993; Lincoln 1982i; Lincoln 1984a; Meikle 1979;
Rochon 2005; Shewan 1984i; Shewan 1984ii; Shewan 1984iii;
Smania 2006; Smith 1981iii; van Steenbrugge 1981; Wertz 1981;
Wertz 1986i; Wertz 1986ii; Wertz 1986iii). Comparison between the groups at baseline was unclear in seven subcompar-

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

10

isons (Jufeng 2005i; Jufeng 2005ii; Jufeng 2005iii; Lincoln 1984b;


Lyon 1997; MacKay 1988; Wu 2004). For seven subcomparisons
the two groups differed despite randomisation in relation to their
time post-onset (Pulvermuller 2001), the severity of their aphasia
(Smith 1981i; Smith 1981ii) and age (David 1982; Doesborgh
2004a; Meinzer 2007; Prins 1989). In Meikle 1979 the participants that were allocated to SLT received more weeks of the intervention than the volunteer-facilitated group (P = 0.01).
Allocation
Details of the method of generating the randomisation sequence
were only available in nine of the 41 subcomparisons. Six used random numbers tables (Bakheit 2007; David 1982; Katz 1997i; Katz
1997ii; Lincoln 1984a; Smania 2006) and three were computergenerated (Doesborgh 2004a; Doesborgh 2004b; Pulvermuller
2001). The remaining 32 subcomparisons stated that participants
were randomly allocated but did not report any further details.
Four subcomparisons described stratifying participants by type
and severity of aphasia (Leal 1993; Shewan 1984i; Shewan 1984ii;
Shewan 1984iii).
Details of the allocation concealment were available for six of the
41 subcomparisons. Five used sequentially numbered sealed envelopes and were considered to be adequately concealed (Bakheit
2007; David 1982; Doesborgh 2004a; Doesborgh 2004b; Lincoln
1984a) but one described a trialist-led allocation method which inadequately concealed participant allocation to the groups (Smania
2006). Data from two subcomparisons (Smith 1981i; Smith
1981ii) are subgroups of participants with aphasia from within a
larger trial examining models of stroke care. The main trial described the inclusion of 20 participants with mild dementia but it
is unclear whether any of these individuals were included in the
aphasia-specific data.
Blinding
Due to the nature of SLT it is difficult to blind either the patient
or person carrying out the intervention. However, blinding of
the outcome assessor is possible and should be in place to avert
detection bias. More than half of the included subcomparisons
(23/41) reported blinding of the outcome assessors (Bakheit 2007;
Denes 1996; Doesborgh 2004a; Jufeng 2005i; Jufeng 2005ii;
Jufeng 2005iii; Lincoln 1982i; Lincoln 1982ii; Lincoln 1982iii;
Lincoln 1984a; MacKay 1988; Meinzer 2007; Pulvermuller 2001;
Shewan 1984i; Shewan 1984ii; Shewan 1984iii; Smania 2006;
Smith 1981i; Smith 1981ii; Smith 1981iii; Wertz 1981; Wertz
1986i; Wertz 1986ii; Wertz 1986iii; Wu 2004). In addition, David
Selective reporting
Two subcomparisons reported using intention-to-treat (ITT) analysis but not all participants appeared to be included in the fi-

1982 described blinding of outcome assessors but they also report


that this was confounded to some extent by indications from the
participants being assessed as to which group they were attending.
This is likely to have occurred in more than one trial. In other
cases blinding was partially in place - for example the assessor for
one of several outcome measures was blinded in Lincoln 1984b
while Katz 1997i and Katz 1997ii ensured blinding of a second
assessor who checked 95% of assessment scores. Blinding however
was unclear for eight subcomparisons (Di Carlo 1980; Doesborgh
2004b; Drummond 1981; Hinckley 2001; Leal 1993; Prins 1989;
Rochon 2005; van Steenbrugge 1981). Outcome assessors were
not blinded for three subcomparisons (Elman 1999; Lyon 1997;
Meikle 1979).
Incomplete outcome data
Overall just over a quarter of the 1840 participants randomised
across the studies included in this review withdrew or were lost to
follow up (431 participants plus 51 at follow up).
Of the 1064 participants in the SLT versus no SLT comparison, a
fifth (224) of participants withdrew from the treatment phase of
the studies (111 from the SLT interventions and 113 from the no
SLT allocation). In addition, 19 participants were lost during the
follow-up assessment phase (10 withdrawing from the SLT groups
and nine from the no SLT groups) and five more participants
withdrew after randomisation but it is unclear which group they
were allocated to (Smith 1981i; Smith 1981ii; Smith 1981iii).
The trials that compared SLT to social support and stimulation
randomised a total of 279 participants (David 1982; Elman 1999;
Lincoln 1982iii; Rochon 2005; Shewan 1984ii; Shewan 1984iii).
However, over a quarter of participants (83 participants) were
lost during the treatment phase (32 from the SLT group and 51
from the social support groups). Twenty-five additional participants were not included in the follow up (David 1982; Elman
1999).
The final comparison of SLT A versus SLT B involved 732 randomised participants; however a fifth (154 participants) withdrew
from the trials during the treatment phase with an additional 10
withdrawing from the follow-up phase. In one trial five participants were reported to have withdrawn from the overall trial but it
is unclear which subcomparison group(s) those participants were
allocated to (Smith 1981i; Smith 1981ii; Smith 1981iii). Participants in one subcomparison (Meikle 1979) remained in the trial
until two successful estimations on an outcome measure showed
no appreciable improvement, participants requested withdrawal
or until the end of the trial, however no further details were given.
Where available, details of drop-outs are presented in Table 2.
nal analyses (Bakheit 2007; Doesborgh 2004a). None of the 23
subcomparisons with participants who had dropped out from
the intervention or control groups used ITT analysis (Bakheit
2007; David 1982; Doesborgh 2004a; Doesborgh 2004b; Elman

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

11

1999; Katz 1997i; Katz 1997ii; Leal 1993; Lincoln 1982i; Lincoln
1984a; MacKay 1988; Meikle 1979; Shewan 1984i; Shewan
1984ii; Shewan 1984iii; Smania 2006; Smith 1981i; Smith 1981ii;
Smith 1981iii; Wertz 1981; Wertz 1986i; Wertz 1986ii; Wertz
1986iii). All randomised participants were included in the final
analyses for the remaining 15 subcomparisons.
Recruitment and retention of stroke rehabilitation trial participants is known to be a challenge and the trials in this review were
no exception. However, seven trials only reported data (including
demographic data) from participants that remained in the trial at
the end of treatment or at follow up. David 1982 reported data
from 133 of 155 randomised participants, Doesborgh 2004b reported 18 of 19 randomised participants, Katz 1997i reported
36 of 42 randomised participants, Katz 1997ii reported 40 of 42
randomised participants, Lincoln 1984a reported 191 of 327 randomised participants, MacKay 1988 reported 95 of 96 randomised
participants and Smania 2006 reported 33 of 41 randomised participants. More recently, to minimise the possibility of bias, trialists have been encouraged to report data from all randomised
participants.

being compared, treatment review processes were in place to ensure any possible risk of overlap in therapy approach was minimised. Being part of a larger stroke trial, participants in the Smith
1981iii trial also received other intensive treatment which may
have affected their levels of fatigue and ability to fully participate
in SLT intervention.

Effects of interventions
The results of this review are presented below within the three
comparisons: 1. SLT versus no SLT, 2. SLT versus social support
and stimulation and 3. SLT A versus SLT B. Where possible results from meta-analyses are also reported. As described within
the Measures of treatment effect section, we extracted the final
value scores for subcomparisons for inclusion within this review
whenever possible. Final values scores were available for 23 of the
41 subcomparisons and these have been included within the review. Change-from-baseline data were available for an additional
three subcomparisons (Denes 1996; Doesborgh 2004a; Hinckley
2001). Where change-from-baseline data are used they are clearly
marked and the data are not pooled within the meta-analyses with
final value scores.

Other potential sources of bias


Co-interventions were reported by some trialists that compared
the effects of SLT with no SLT but the number and allocation of
the participants and details of the intervention were unclear. For
example, some participants in Doesborgh 2004b also received psychosocial group therapy. Some (or all) of the participants reported
in Smith 1981i may have benefited from other intensive treatment
as part of the larger multi-disciplinary stroke trial. Three subcomparisons reported that not all participants received the planned
number of treatment sessions (Lincoln 1984a; Smith 1981i; Smith
1981ii).
Similarly, five subcomparisons that compared two different approaches to SLT provision reported that not all participants received the planned number of treatment sessions (Bakheit 2007;
Lincoln 1982i; Lincoln 1982ii; Meikle 1979; Smith 1981iii). In
one case (Meikle 1979) it was reported that five of the 16 participants receiving conventional SLT missed up to half of their
possible treatment. Two trials comparing intensive SLT with conventional SLT also reported difficulties providing intensive SLT
interventions as planned. Bakheit 2007 reported that only 13 of
the 51 participants received 80% or more of the planned intensive intervention. Smith 1981iii reported that participants allocated to intensive therapy only received an average of 21 hours
of therapy compared to the planned minimum of 50 hours during the first three months. Such difficulties in maintaining a clear
distinction between the two treatment groups has significant implications when evaluating the results and considering the clinical
implications of such treatment regimens.
Though all the speech and language therapists in Hinckley 2001
were trained in the characteristics of the two treatment approaches

Comparison 1: SLT versus no SLT


A total of 1064 participants were randomised across 14 subcomparisons that contrasted SLT with no SLT (Doesborgh 2004b;
Jufeng 2005i; Jufeng 2005ii; Katz 1997i; Katz 1997ii; Lincoln
1984a; Lyon 1997; MacKay 1988; Smania 2006; Smith 1981i;
Smith 1981ii; Wertz 1986i; Wertz 1986ii; Wu 2004). Reporting
of age and other descriptions of the participants across trials varied,
making it difficult to give an overview of the participants involved
in this comparison. Only four trials reported age ranges, spanning 38 to 92 years of age (Lincoln 1984a; Lyon 1997; Smania
2006; Wu 2004), while others reported participants mean age or
age bands. Details can be found in Table 1. Nine subcomparisons
gave an indication of the length of time since participants had
experienced the onset of their aphasia: the widest post-onset time
spanning from two to 36 months (Smania 2006). The shortest
mean length of time since the onset of participants aphasia was
between 6.6 and 7.8 weeks (Wertz 1986i; Wertz 1986ii). Severity
of aphasia was only reported by five subcomparisons (Doesborgh
2004b; Smith 1981i; Smith 1981ii; Wertz 1986i; Wertz 1986ii),
although two additional subcomparisons did provide some guide
to severity of impairment (Lyon 1997; Smania 2006).
Amongst the SLT interventions compared to a no SLT group
were interventions described as conventional SLT (Jufeng 2005ii;
Smania 2006; Smith 1981ii; Wertz 1986i; Wu 2004), computermediated SLT (Doesborgh 2004b; Katz 1997i; Katz 1997ii);
group SLT (Jufeng 2005i); functional SLT (Lyon 1997); intensive
SLT (Smith 1981i); SLT plus operant training (Lincoln 1984a)
and volunteer-facilitated SLT (MacKay 1988; Wertz 1986ii). We
planned to conduct a sensitivity analysis on subcomparisons that

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

12

involved the provision of SLT by non-speech and language therapists (Jufeng 2005i; Jufeng 2005ii; MacKay 1988; Wertz 1986ii)
but because of the present availability of data within each outcome
it was not useful to undertake this analysis.
Appropriate summary data for communication outcomes (allowing inclusion in the meta-analyses) were available for only nine of
the 14 subcomparisons (Doesborgh 2004b; Jufeng 2005i; Jufeng
2005ii; Katz 1997i; Katz 1997ii; Lincoln 1984a; Smania 2006;
Wertz 1986i; Wertz 1986ii). In addition, Lincoln 1984a also reported statistical data for psychosocial outcomes. Suitable summary data were not reported (or available on request) for the remaining five subcomparisons (Lyon 1997; MacKay 1988; Smith
1981i; Smith 1981ii; Wu 2004). Where data for this comparison
were available they are presented below in relation to the following: 1. functional communication; 2. receptive language; 3. expressive language; 4. severity of impairment; 5. psychosocial; 6.
number of drop-outs; 7. compliance with allocated intervention;
8. economic outcomes.

1. Functional communication

Eight of the 14 subcomparisons (Doesborgh 2004b; Katz 1997i;


Katz 1997ii; Lincoln 1984a; Lyon 1997; MacKay 1988; Wertz
1986i; Wertz 1986ii) compared participants that received SLT
to those randomised to no SLT by measuring functional communication outcomes. Five had suitable statistical data available
allowing inclusion within the meta-analyses (Doesborgh 2004b;
Katz 1997i; Katz 1997ii; Wertz 1986i; Wertz 1986ii). Tools used
included the spontaneous speech subtest of the Western Aphasia
Battery (WAB) (Katz 1997i; Katz 1997ii) and the Amsterdam-Nijmegen Everyday Language Test (ANELT-A) (Doesborgh 2004b),
the Communication Activities of Daily Living (CADL) (Wertz
1986i; Wertz 1986ii) and the Functional Communication Profile
(FCP) (Lincoln 1984a; Wertz 1986i; Wertz 1986ii).

Spontaneous speech
Three subcomparisons evaluated the impact of SLT by contrasting
the spontaneous speech of participants who received computermediated SLT with those who did not (Doesborgh 2004b; Katz
1997i) or those who received computer-mediated non-linguistic
tasks (Katz 1997ii). Comparisons were made using a subtest of
the WAB (Katz 1997i; Katz 1997ii) or the ANELT-A (Doesborgh
2004b). There was no evidence of a significant difference in the
measures of participants spontaneous speech abilities on these
measures though Doesborgh 2004b may demonstrate a trend towards better spontaneous speech skills in those participants that
had access to SLT than those that did not (P = 0.08, SMD 0.88,
95% CI -0.10 to 1.87) (Analysis 1.1).

Communication Activities of Daily Living (CADL)

Four subcomparisons used the CADL to compare the functional


communication skills of participants that received SLT (conventional SLT (Wertz 1986i), volunteer-facilitated SLT (MacKay
1988; Wertz 1986ii) and functional SLT (Lyon 1997)), and those
that received no SLT intervention. Two subcomparisons provided
statistical data which allowed inclusion within a meta-analysis
(Wertz 1986i; Wertz 1986ii). There was no evidence of a difference between the groups provided with SLT and those that were
not (Analysis 1.1).
Functional Communication Profile (FCP)
Three subcomparisons (Lincoln 1984a; Wertz 1986i; Wertz
1986ii) compared the pragmatic provision of SLT (approach tailored to individual participants needs) to a deferred SLT intervention using the FCP. Appropriate summary data for Lincoln
1984a on this outcome measure were not available. There was no
evidence of a difference between the groups.
In pooling the results of functional communication measures
within the meta-analysis only one set of functional communication measures from Wertz 1986i and Wertz 1986ii could be included at a time. Neither pooling approach provided evidence of
a difference between the groups (by including the CADL data P
= 0.16, SMD 0.19, 95% CI -0.13 to 0.50; including FCP data
P = 0.08, SMD 0.28, 95% CI -0.03 to 0.59). We have chosen
to present the data from the FCP within the forest plot (Analysis
1.1).
2. Receptive language

Five of the 14 subcomparisons measured participants receptive


language skills (Katz 1997i; Katz 1997ii; Smania 2006; Wertz
1986i; Wertz 1986ii) and all reported statistical data which permitted inclusion in the meta-analyses. Auditory comprehension
was measured using the Token Test, a WAB subtest and the Porch
Index of Communicative Abilities (PICA) subtest. Reading comprehension was measured using the Reading Comprehension Battery for Aphasia and the reading subtest of the PICA. Gesture
comprehension was measured using an unnamed assessment.
Auditory comprehension
Two subcomparisons used a subtest of the WAB to measure participants auditory comprehension (Katz 1997i; Katz 1997ii). There
was no evidence of a difference between the groups that received
computer-mediated SLT and those that did not. The same participants auditory comprehension skills were also measured on
a subtest of the PICA. Three subcomparisons used the Token
Test to measure changes in participants auditory comprehension
(Smania 2006; Wertz 1986i; Wertz 1986ii). As above, both sets
of data from Katz 1997i and Katz 1997ii could not be included
in the same meta-analysis. On pooling the data within two separate meta-analyses, neither demonstrated a significant difference

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

13

between the groups (by including the WAB data P = 0.59, SMD
0.08, 95% CI -0.21 to 0.38; by including the PICA data P = 0.52,
SMD 0.10, 95% CI -0.20 to 0.39). We have chosen to present
the PICA data within the forest plot (Analysis 1.2).

Reading comprehension
Reading comprehension was measured by four subcomparisons
(Katz 1997i; Katz 1997ii; Wertz 1986i; Wertz 1986ii) that compared participants that received SLT and those that did not. Two
trials used the Reading Comprehension Battery for Aphasia to
compare participants that received volunteer-facilitated SLT with
those that received no SLT (Wertz 1986i; Wertz 1986ii). Similarly, two trials used the PICA reading subtest to compare participants that received computer-mediated SLT to those that received
no treatment (Katz 1997i) or computer-mediated non-linguistic
tasks (Katz 1997ii). On pooling of the data there was no evidence
of a difference between the groups (Analysis 1.3).

Other comprehension
The PICA gestural subtest was used by four subcomparisons (Katz
1997i; Katz 1997ii; Wertz 1986i; Wertz 1986ii) and measures, not
just gestural abilities, but also tests auditory and written comprehension skills. Following pooling, participants that received SLT
had achieved higher scores on measures of gesture use than the
groups that received no SLT (P = 0.02, MD 8.04, 95% CI 1.55
to 14.52) (Analysis 1.4).

Gesture comprehension
Smania 2006 used an unnamed gesture comprehension assessment
tool to compare a group that received conventional SLT and those
that received limb apraxia therapy at two time points: after intervention and again two months later. There was no evidence of a
difference between the two groups comprehension of gestures at
either time point (Analysis 1.5).

3. Expressive language

Five of the 14 subcomparisons (Doesborgh 2004b; Katz 1997i;


Katz 1997ii; Wertz 1986i; Wertz 1986ii) formally evaluated participants expressive language skills using single word picture naming (Boston Naming Test and WAB naming subtests), repetition (WAB repetition subtest) and other verbal expression (PICA)
skills. Written language expressive skills were measured using the
PICA copying and writing subtests while the ability to communicate using gesture was measured using the PICA gesture subtest.

Expressive language: naming


Participants spoken language abilities were measured by three
subcomparisons (Doesborgh 2004b; Katz 1997i; Katz 1997ii).
Doesborgh 2004b used the Boston Naming Test to compare a
group receiving computer-mediated SLT and a group that did not
receive SLT. Similarly, Katz 1997i and Katz 1997ii employed the
WAB naming subtest. On pooling there was no evidence of a difference between the groups (Analysis 1.6).
Expressive language: general
Four subcomparisons used the PICA verbal subtest to compare the
spoken language skills of groups that received SLT and those that
did not (Katz 1997i; Katz 1997ii; Wertz 1986i; Wertz 1986ii).
There was no evidence of a between-group difference (Analysis
1.7).
Expressive language: written
Two subcomparisons compared a group receiving computer-mediated SLT to a group receiving no SLT (Katz 1997i) or a group
receiving computer-mediated non-linguistic tasks (Katz 1997ii)
using the PICA copying and writing subtests. Wertz 1986i and
Wertz 1986ii used the PICA Graphic subtests. Following pooling
there was no evidence of a difference between the groups on any
of these measures (Analysis 1.8).
Repetition
Katz 1997i and Katz 1997ii compared participants that received
computer-mediated SLT with participants that received no SLT
(Katz 1997i) and participants that received computer-mediated
non-SLT tasks (Katz 1997ii) using the WAB repetition test. Following pooling there was no evidence of a difference in the participants repetition skills on these measures (Analysis 1.9).
4. Severity of impairment

Ten subcomparisons compared participants that received SLT with


those that did not by measuring the severity of their aphasia
impairment using an aphasia quotient and language assessment
batteries (Jufeng 2005i; Jufeng 2005ii; Katz 1997i; Katz 1997ii;
Lincoln 1984a; Lyon 1997; Smith 1981i; Smith 1981ii; Wertz
1986i; Wertz 1986ii). Language assessment batteries included the
PICA, Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination (BDAE) and the
Minnesota Test for Differential Diagnosis of Aphasia (MTDDA).
Details of the Chinese Rehabilitation Research Centre Aphasia
Examination (CRRCAE) assessment were not available to us. We
were able to obtain suitable statistical summary data from these
outcome measures for six subcomparisons (Jufeng 2005i; Jufeng
2005ii; Katz 1997i; Katz 1997ii; Wertz 1986i; Wertz 1986ii).
Data from two subcomparisons that used the CRRCAE to compare participants aphasia following group SLT (Jufeng 2005i) or

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

14

one-to-one SLT (Jufeng 2005ii) with a group that received no


SLT were available. Four additional subcomparisons compared
groups that received computer-mediated SLT (Katz 1997i; Katz
1997ii), conventional SLT (Wertz 1986i) or volunteer-facilitated
SLT (Wertz 1986ii) to groups that received no SLT (Katz 1997i;
Wertz 1986i; Wertz 1986ii) or a computer-mediated non-SLT intervention (Katz 1997ii) using the PICA. On pooling, there was
no evidence of a difference between the groups that received SLT
and those that did not in measures of severity of aphasia impairment using either the Katz 1997i and Katz 1997ii PICA data (P
= 0.19) or the WAB data (P = 0.28). We have chosen to present
the PICA data (Analysis 1.10).
Jufeng 2005i and Jufeng 2005ii also repeated the comparison of
participants who received group SLT and conventional SLT with
those who had not received any SLT on measures of aphasia severity at a three-month follow up. The group that received group SLT
scored significantly higher than those that received no SLT but on
pooling (using a random-effects model in the presence of significant statistical heterogeneity) there was no evidence of a difference
between the groups (Analysis 1.11).

5. Psychosocial

Four subcomparisons compared the benefits of SLT intervention


to no SLT by employing psychosocial measures including the
Multiple Affect Adjective Checklist (MAAC), the General Health
Questionnaire (GHQ), the Affect Balance Scale and the Psychological Wellbeing Index (Lincoln 1984a; Lyon 1997; Smith 1981i;
Smith 1981ii).
Lyon 1997 used the Affect Balance Scale and Psychological Wellbeing Index to compare a group of triads (person with aphasia,
caregiver and communication partner) that received functional
SLT that aimed to establish and maximise effective means of communication between communication partners and a group that
received no SLT. Suitable summary data for these measures were
unavailable and so they could not be included in this meta-analysis. Similarly, the GHQ was used to compare groups that received
either intensive SLT (Smith 1981i) or conventional SLT (Smith
1981ii) with a group that received no treatment but no summary
data were available for inclusion in this analysis. Lincoln 1984a
used the anxiety, depression and hostility scales of the MAAC to
compare the psychosocial wellbeing of a group that received SLT
(determined by the therapist) with a group that received no SLT.
Comparison of the groups failed to show any evidence of a difference in the participants anxiety, depression or hostility as measured on these scales (Analysis 1.12).

6. Number of drop-outs

Much of the information relating to the numbers of participant


drop-outs (where they occurred) was available for all 14 subcomparisons (Doesborgh 2004b; Jufeng 2005i; Jufeng 2005ii; Katz

1997i; Katz 1997ii; Lincoln 1984a; Lyon 1997; MacKay 1988;


Smith 1981i; Smith 1981ii; Smania 2006; Wertz 1986i; Wertz
1986ii; Wu 2004).
There was a range of reasons for the attrition of participants from
the trials (see Table 2 for details). One-fifth of participants randomised to this comparison withdrew during the treatment phase
(229 participants) and an additional 19 participants were lost at
the follow-up phase from across 11 subcomparisons (Doesborgh
2004b; Katz 1997i; Katz 1997ii; Lincoln 1984a; MacKay 1988;
Smith 1981i; Smith 1981ii; Smania 2006; Wertz 1986i; Wertz
1986ii; Wu 2004). An additional five participants withdrew from
Smith 1981i and Smith 1981ii (group allocation is unclear but
these withdrawals are included in the number above) and they
failed to report the number of withdrawals from the no SLT group.
On pooling of the available data relating to drop-outs there was
no evidence of a difference between the groups (Analysis 1.13).
7. Compliance with allocated intervention

Only two (Doesborgh 2004b; Smania 2006) of the 11 subcomparisons reporting participant drop-outs also described the reasons
for the 25 participants withdrawal. Of these, a total of 12 participants were described as withdrawing because they were uncooperative or they refused the allocated treatment (all from Smania
2006) with seven withdrawing from the conventional SLT group
and five withdrawing from the no SLT group. Details can be found
in Table 2.
8. Economic outcomes

Only one of the 14 subcomparisons described the measurement


of economic outcomes using structured questionnaires (MacKay
1988) but neither the questionnaire nor the results were available
for this review.
Comparison 2: SLT versus social support and
stimulation
Six subcomparisons compared the provision of SLT to the provision of informal social support and stimulation amongst a total
of 279 participants (David 1982; Elman 1999; Lincoln 1982iii;
Rochon 2005; Shewan 1984ii; Shewan 1984iii). As described
above, the description of participant groups within trials was variable and so it is difficult to give a precise overview of the participants included in this comparison. Four subcomparisons described the participants age range, which spanned 18 to 85 years
(Elman 1999; Lincoln 1982iii; Rochon 2005; Shewan 1984ii;
Shewan 1984iii). David 1982 reported participants in the SLT
and social support and stimulation groups had a mean age of 70
(8.7) and 65 (10.6) years respectively, indicating a significant difference between the groups (P = 0.003). Details can be found in
Table 1. All five subcomparisons detailed the length of time since
the onset of participants aphasia. Participants with the most acute

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

15

aphasia were recruited to Shewan 1984ii and Shewan 1984iii with


aphasia that was between two and four weeks since onset. In contrast, Lincoln 1982iii recruited participants between one and 36
months post-stroke while some of the other trials recruited participants much later following stroke with ranges from two to nine
years (Rochon 2005) or seven months to 28 years (Elman 1999).
Severity of aphasia was reported by all six subcomparisons in varying degrees of detail. Lincoln 1982iii recruited participants with
moderate degrees of aphasia. The remaining five subcomparisons
described the recruitment of participants with a range of mild to
severe aphasia impairments (David 1982; Elman 1999; Rochon
2005; Shewan 1984ii; Shewan 1984iii) (see Table 1 for details).
There were a number of approaches to the provision of SLT interventions in the trials: four provided conventional SLT (David
1982; Elman 1999; Lincoln 1982iii; Shewan 1984iii) and the
others provided sentence-mapping SLT (Rochon 2005) and language-orientated SLT (Shewan 1984ii). These SLT interventions
were then compared to the provision of social support and stimulation which also took a variety of formats. The unstructured
support and communicative stimulation was provided by nurses
(Shewan 1984ii; Shewan 1984iii), other volunteers (David 1982),
speech and language therapists (Lincoln 1982iii; Rochon 2005)
or through attendance at an externally organised support group or
class, for example dance classes or church groups (Elman 1999).
Volunteers had been given detailed information about their own
participants particular presentation of aphasia but were not given
any training in SLT techniques. Lincoln 1982iii had a specific
non-therapeutic intervention protocol for the therapists whose
role was to have semi-structured conversations with the participant on a series of predetermined topics. The participants in these
groups received this support for one hour (Rochon 2005), two
hours (David 1982; Lincoln 1982iii) or three hours (Elman 1999;
Shewan 1984ii; Shewan 1984iii), each week over a period of a
month (Lincoln 1982iii), two-and-a-half months (Rochon 2005),
four months (Elman 1999), five months (David 1982) or a year
(Shewan 1984ii; Shewan 1984iii).
Statistical data for communication outcomes were available for
half the included subcomparisons (David 1982; Lincoln 1982iii;
Rochon 2005). Suitable data allowing inclusion within the metaanalyses were unavailable for the remaining three subcomparisons
(Elman 1999; Shewan 1984ii; Shewan 1984iii). The comparisons
made (with meta-analysis where possible) are reported below as
they relate to measures of: 1. functional communication; 2. receptive language; 3. expressive language; 4. severity of impairment;
5. psychosocial; 6. number of drop-outs; 7. compliance with allocated intervention. Economic outcomes were not measured.

1. Functional communication

Two subcomparisons measured functional communication (David


1982; Elman 1999) using the Functional Communication Profile
(FCP), the Communication Abilities of Daily Living (CADL) and

the Communicative Effectiveness Index (CETI).

Functional Communication Profile


David 1982 used the FCP to compare a group who received conventional SLT with a group that received communication treatment by volunteers. There was no evidence of a difference between
the groups nor was there any evidence of a difference at three and
six-month follow up (Analysis 2.1).

Communication Abilities of Daily Living and the


Communicative Effectiveness Index
Elman 1999 used the CADL, the CETI and measures of connected speech to compare the functional communication skills of
participants that received conventional SLT and those that did not
but who attended social groups and activities instead. No suitable
summary data were provided and so the data could not be included
in the meta-analysis.

2. Receptive language

Four of the six subcomparisons that compared participants that


received SLT or a social support and stimulation intervention did
so by comparing the groups receptive language skills (Lincoln
1982iii; Rochon 2005; Shewan 1984ii; Shewan 1984iii). Measures
used included the Philadelphia Comprehension Battery (PCB),
the Auditory Comprehension Test for Sentences (ACTS), the Token Test and the PICA Gestural subtest.

Philadelphia Comprehension Battery


Rochon 2005 measured participants receptive language skills on
the PCB, which includes subtests for sentence comprehension and
picture comprehension. There was no evidence of a difference between the receptive language skills of the participants that received
sentence-mapping SLT and those that received unstructured social
support and stimulation (Analysis 2.2).

Auditory Comprehension Test for Sentences (ACTS)


Two additional subcomparisons also measured receptive language
skills of a group that received either language-oriented therapy
(Shewan 1984ii) or conventional SLT (Shewan 1984iii) and compared their language reception to participants that received an intervention that provided unstructured social support. Both subcomparisons used the ACTS to make this comparison but the
manner in which the data are reported prevented inclusion within
the meta-analysis (Analysis 2.2).

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

16

Token Test
Lincoln 1982iii measured participants receptive language skills
using the Token Test. There was no evidence of a difference between the groups (Analysis 2.2).

treated and untreated items but there was no evidence of a between-group difference on the treated or untreated items (Analysis
2.6).

Expressive language: general


Participants auditory and written comprehension skills were measured using the PICA Gestural subtest by Lincoln 1982iii and
those that had access to social support and stimulation performed
significantly better on these measures than those that had access
to SLT (P = 0.04, MD -0.87, 95% CI -1.70 to -0.04) (Analysis
2.3).

Lincoln 1982iii and Elman 1999 compared the groups performances on the PICA verbal subtest. Suitable statistical data were
unavailable from Elman 1999 and so it could not be included
in the meta-analysis. Participants that had received social support
and stimulation scored significantly better than those that received
SLT (P = 0.0007, MD -1.56, 95% CI -2.46 to -0.66) (Analysis
2.7).

3. Expressive language

Expressive language: written

Three of the six subcomparisons that compared participants that


received SLT or a social support and stimulation intervention did
so by comparing the groups expressive language skills (Elman
1999; Lincoln 1982iii; Rochon 2005). Measures used included
the Object Naming Test (ONT), Caplan and Hanna Sentence
Production Test (CHSPT), the Picture Description with Structured Modeling (PDSM) and the PICA.

Similarly, Lincoln 1982iii compared the groups performances on


the PICA graphic subtests and found participants that received
social support performed significantly better than those that had
received SLT (P = 0.01, MD -1.39, 95% CI -2.49 to -0.29) (
Analysis 2.8).

Receptive language: other comprehension

Expressive language: single words


Lincoln 1982iii measured participants naming skills on the ONT
and a word fluency test and found those participants that received
social support and stimulation performed significantly better on
these tests than those that had received conventional SLT (P =
0.003, MD -7.00, 95% CI -11.67 to -2.33, and P < 0.0001, MD
-14.00, 95% CI -20.35 to -7.65 respectively) (Analysis 2.4).

Expressive language: sentences


Rochon 2005 compared the participants who received sentencemapping SLT and a group receiving unstructured social support
and stimulation. Comparison of the two groups showed no evidence of a difference between the groups performance on the
Caplan and Hanna Sentence Production Test scores. Those that
had received SLT did perform significantly better on treated items
from the test (P = 0.01, MD 3, 95% CI 0.63 to 5.37) than the participants that received social support but there was no evidence of
a difference between the groups on the untreated items (Analysis
2.5).

4. Severity of impairment

Elman 1999, Lincoln 1982iii, Shewan 1984ii and Shewan 1984iii


compared groups that had access to SLT and those that received
social support and stimulation by measuring participants aphasia
severity. The assessments used included the PICA and the Western
Aphasia Battery-Aphasia Quotient (WAB AQ).

PICA
Two subcomparisons used the Shortened PICA to compare participants that had received group SLT and those that had attended
other social activities or groups that provided social support and
stimulation (Elman 1999; Lincoln 1982iii). Suitable statistical
data were unavailable from Elman 1999 and so it could not be
included in the meta-analysis. Lincoln 1982iii found that participants provided with social support and stimulation were less impaired as a result of aphasia (as measured on the PICA) than those
that received SLT (P = 0.005, OR 0.65, 95% CI 0.38 to 1.12).
Suitable summary data were not available from Elman 1999 to
allow inclusion within the meta-analysis (Analysis 2.9).

WAB
Expressive language: picture description
Two subcomparisons elicited samples of participants connected
speech using picture description tasks (Lincoln 1982iii; Rochon
2005). There was no evidence of a difference between the two
groups. Rochon 2005 also reported the two groups scores on the

Two additional subcomparisons (Shewan 1984ii; Shewan 1984iii)


also compared groups based on the severity of participants aphasia using the WAB. They compared participants who received
language-oriented SLT (Shewan 1984ii) or conventional SLT
(Shewan 1984iii) with a group who received psychological support and unstructured communication provided by trained nurses.

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

17

Suitable summary data were unavailable and so it could not be


included in the meta-analysis.
5. Psychosocial

Elman 1999 compared participants that had received SLT and


those that had received social support and stimulation using measures of psychosocial impact using the Affect Balance Scale but
appropriate summary values were unavailable and so it could not
be included in the review.
6. Number of drop-outs

Drop-outs were reported by five of the six subcomparisons in


this section (David 1982; Elman 1999; Lincoln 1982iii; Shewan
1984ii; Shewan 1984iii). In the main Lincoln 1982 trial (from
which the subcomparison Lincoln 1982iii has been extracted) 13
participants were excluded for failing to complete the full treatment intervention. It is unclear which intervention arms these
participants were randomised to and so these drop-outs cannot
be included in this meta-analysis. In the remaining subcomparisons, a total of 44 participants were lost to the groups allocated
to SLT (32 from treatment and 12 at follow up) while 64 were
lost to the social support and stimulation interventions (45 during the intervention and 11 at follow up). There was no evidence
of a significant difference in the drop-out rates between the two
groups although there seems to be a consistency in the direction of
drop-outs, with a suggestion of better retention of participants in
the groups given SLT but this did not reach significance (Analysis
2.10).
7. Compliance with allocated intervention

Four subcomparisons that experienced drop-outs from their trial


also described the reasons for the drop-outs so that those who had
voluntarily withdrawn from the allocated intervention can be identified. A total of five participants withdrew from the groups that
received SLT while eight participants withdrew from the groups
that were allocated to receive social support and stimulation interventions (David 1982; Shewan 1984ii; Shewan 1984iii). Elman
1999 reports one withdrawal from the SLT group and two withdrawals from the social support and stimulation group because of
time constraints. In addition, David 1982 also describes the withdrawal of four more participants from the social support group
because of volunteer problems. Details can be found in Table 2.
Comparisons: SLT A versus SLT B
A total of 719 participants were randomised across 21 subcomparisons that compared one SLT intervention (SLT A) with another SLT intervention (SLT B) (Bakheit 2007; Denes 1996;
Di Carlo 1980; Doesborgh 2004a; Drummond 1981; Hinckley
2001; Jufeng 2005iii; Leal 1993; Lincoln 1982i; Lincoln 1982ii;

Lincoln 1984b; Meikle 1979; Meinzer 2007; ORLA 2006; Prins


1989; Pulvermuller 2001; Shewan 1984i; Smith 1981iii; van
Steenbrugge 1981; Wertz 1981; Wertz 1986iii). As within other
sections of this review, descriptions of the participants age and
other characteristics across trials varied. Participants age ranges
spanning 17 to 92 years were available for 13 subcomparisons while
the remaining reported mean age (Denes 1996; Doesborgh 2004a;
Drummond 1981; Hinckley 2001; Leal 1993; Smith 1981iii;
Wertz 1986iii) or participants within age bands (Jufeng 2005iii).
See Table 1 for details. All but two subcomparisons (Jufeng 2005iii;
Smith 1981iii) reported the length of time since their participants had experienced the onset of aphasia, ranging from within a
month of stroke onset (Bakheit 2007; Leal 1993; Shewan 1984i;
Wertz 1981), within approximately six months of stroke (Denes
1996; Di Carlo 1980; Doesborgh 2004a; Lincoln 1982i; Lincoln
1982ii; Lincoln 1984b; Meinzer 2007; Wertz 1986iii), or up to
a year or more after stroke (Drummond 1981; Hinckley 2001;
Meinzer 2007; ORLA 2006; Pulvermuller 2001; Prins 1989; van
Steenbrugge 1981).
Similarly, almost all subcomparisons reported the severity of aphasia with only two failing to give an indication of how severe participants aphasia was (Drummond 1981; Jufeng 2005iii). In most
cases subcomparisons reported the range of participants aphasia
severity as measured on a suitable assessment tool but in some
cases this was reported in more general terms (details can be found
in Table 1). Some subcomparisons focused specifically on participants with severe aphasia (Denes 1996; Di Carlo 1980; Lincoln
1984b) while others focused on moderate to severe presentations
of aphasia (Lincoln 1982i; Leal 1993).
Most of the subcomparisons included in this section compared
an experimental approach to the delivery of SLT to a conventional SLT intervention. These included a comparison of functional SLT (Hinckley 2001), intensive SLT (Bakheit 2007; Denes
1996; Smith 1981iii), group SLT (Jufeng 2005iii; Wertz 1981),
task-specific approaches to SLT (Drummond 1981; Prins 1989;
Pulvermuller 2001; Shewan 1984i; van Steenbrugge 1981) and
volunteer-facilitated SLT (Leal 1993; Meikle 1979; Wertz 1986iii)
with conventional SLT.
Additional comparisons included in this section are SLT with operant training (Lincoln 1982i) or SLT with programmed instruction and operant training (Lincoln 1984b) compared to conventional SLT (with a placebo attention intervention), semantic approaches to SLT compared with phonological approaches to SLT
(Doesborgh 2004a) and filmed programmed instructions compared with non-programmed activities (Di Carlo 1980).
Only 14 of the 21 subcomparisons reported suitable summary
data that permitted inclusion in the meta-analyses (Bakheit 2007;
Denes 1996; Di Carlo 1980; Doesborgh 2004a; Hinckley 2001;
Jufeng 2005iii; Lincoln 1982i; Lincoln 1982ii; Lincoln 1984b;
Meinzer 2007; ORLA 2006; Prins 1989; van Steenbrugge 1981;
Wertz 1986iii). Where data for this comparison were available they
are presented below within the comparisons: 3. Functional SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

18

versus Conventional SLT; 4. Intensive SLT versus Conventional


SLT; 5. Volunteer-facilitated SLT versus Conventional SLT; 6.
Group SLT versus Conventional SLT; 7. Task-specific SLT versus
Conventional SLT; 8. Operant training SLT versus Conventional
SLT; 9. Semantic SLT versus Phonological SLT; 10. Programmed
instruction versus Non-programmed instruction. (Note: for consistency with the analyses this list starts at number 3).

required to complete the tasks with or without a concurrent task.


Participants that received the functional SLT performed significantly better on the spoken telephone order task (no concurrent
task P = 0.0001, MD 32.80, 95% CI 16.16 to 49.44; with concurrent task P = 0.03, MD 16.90, 95% CI 1.31 to 32.49) than
the participants that received the conventional SLT intervention.
There was no evidence of any difference between the groups performance on the written order tasks (Analysis 3.2).

3. Functional SLT (SLT A) versus conventional SLT (SLT B)

Hinckley 2001 was the only subcomparison identified that contrasted a group receiving functional SLT with a group who received conventional SLT in relation to participants (a) functional
communication and (b) expressive language. They did not address
participants receptive language skills, severity of impairment, psychosocial or economic outcomes. No participants were lost during
the interventions and so comparisons in relation to number of
drop-outs and compliance with allocated intervention could not
be made.

(a) Functional communication


Participants functional communication skills were measured on
the CADL and the CETI in order to compare the impact of a
functional SLT approach and a conventional SLT approach.

CADL
Hinckley 2001 only reported the participants change-from-baseline scores which demonstrated that participants in the conventional SLT group performed significantly better on the CADL
than those participants in the functional SLT group (P = 0.001,
MD -9.30, 95% CI -15.01 to -3.59) (Analysis 3.1).

CETI
The CETI was used by Hinckley 2001 to compare the groups
functional communication skills as perceived by their carer. Using
final value scores there was no evidence of a difference in the carers
ratings of the participants functional communication skills based
on whether they had access to functional SLT or a conventional
SLT intervention (Analysis 3.1).

Functional communication: catalogue ordering


Hinckley 2001 also developed a functional catalogue ordering task
to compare the two groups functional communication skills using change-from-baseline scores. Participants were required to order clothes from a catalogue by telephone (spoken modality) or
in writing (written modality). In each modality participants were

(b) Expressive language


Hinckley 2001 used the PALPA to compare the expressive language skills (oral and written) of participants that received either
functional SLT or conventional SLT. There was no evidence of a
difference between the groups oral naming change-from-baseline
scores (Analysis 3.3) or their written naming change-from-baseline scores (Analysis 3.4) on the PALPA measure

4. Intensive SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)

Four subcomparisons compared intensive SLT to conventional


SLT (Bakheit 2007; Denes 1996; ORLA 2006; Smith 1981ii).
The number of weekly hours in therapy for participants in the
Intensive SLT groups ranged from four hours (Smith 1981ii), five
hours (Bakheit 2007; Denes 1996), to 10 hours (ORLA 2006)
each week while the conventional SLT groups received 80 minutes
(Smith 1981iii), two hours (Bakheit 2007), three hours (Denes
1996) or four hours (ORLA 2006) each week. Statistical data for
communication outcomes were only available for three subcomparisons (Bakheit 2007; Denes 1996; ORLA 2006) and comparisons were made by measuring participants (a) receptive language,
(b) expressive language, (c) severity of impairment, (d) psychosocial impact, (e) number of drop-outs and (f ) compliance with allocated intervention. Functional communication and economic
outcome measures were not used.

(a) Receptive language


Measures of participants receptive language skills were only available for Denes 1996. Participants auditory comprehension was
measured using the Aachen Aphasia Test (AAT) Comprehension
subtest and the Token Test. Only change-from-baseline scores were
available. Comparison of the groups comprehension skills failed
to show a difference between those that had received intensive SLT
and those that had received conventional SLT on this measure, although the groups performance on the AAT Comprehension subtest indicated a trend towards better comprehension skills amongst
those participants that had received intensive SLT than those that
had received conventional SLT (P = 0.06) (Analysis 4.1).

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

19

(b) Expressive language

Expressive language: spoken


Denes 1996 measured expressive language skills using the AAT
Naming and Repetition subtests. Comparison of the groups
change from baseline scores showed no evidence of a difference
between those that received intensive SLT and those that received
conventional SLT on either of these measures (Analysis 4.2).

Expressive language: written


Denes 1996 used the AAT written subtest to compare changesfrom-baseline in participants written language (including reading
aloud and writing subtests). The group that was given intensive
SLT achieved significantly higher scores on this subtest than the
group that received conventional SLT intervention (P = 0.01, MD
8.9, 95% CI 1.81 to 15.99) (Analysis 4.3).
(c) Severity of impairment
Three subcomparisons (Bakheit 2007; Denes 1996; ORLA 2006)
compared participants overall level of aphasia severity following
intensive or conventional SLT by using the WAB and the Aachen
Aphasia Test (AAT). Smith 1981iii also measured aphasia severity
using the Minnesota Test for Differential Diagnosis of Aphasia
(MTDDA) but suitable statistical data allowing inclusion in the
meta-analysis were unavailable. There was no evidence of a difference between the groups that received intensive SLT and those
that received conventional SLT on these WAB final value scores
(Bakheit 2007; ORLA 2006) or AAT change-from-baseline scores
(Denes 1996) of severity either immediately post-treatment or at
three-month follow up (Bakheit 2007) (Analysis 4.4).
(d) Psychosocial
Smith 1981iii used the General Health Questionnaire to compare
groups receiving intensive SLT and conventional SLT. Appropriate
summary data for these groups were unavailable and so the results
could not be presented here.
(e) Number of drop-outs
Data relating to number of participants that dropped out of the
subcomparisons were available for Bakheit 2007, Denes 1996 and
ORLA 2006 and were partially available for Smith 1981iii. No
participants appear to have been lost from the treatment or followup time points in the Denes 1996 or ORLA 2006 studies. Five
additional participants were excluded from the final analysis in
Smith 1981iii (three were found not to have aphasia and two
died) but their group allocation was unclear. These data were not
included in this overview. Across the subcomparisons significantly
more participants (30 participants) were lost to the intensive SLT
groups in comparison to those lost to the conventional SLT groups
(17 participants) (P = 0.05, OR 2.10, 95% CI 0.99 to 4.46). Of
these, some were lost at follow up, with little difference between the
three participants lost from intensive SLT and the four participants

lost from the conventional SLT group in the Bakheit 2007 study
(Analysis 4.5).
(f ) Compliance with allocated intervention
Only Bakheit 2007 reported (in part) the reasons for loss of participants from within the study. Of these, one participant voluntarily withdrew from the intensive SLT group during the treatment
phase while none withdrew from the conventional SLT group.

5. Volunteer-facilitated SLT (SLTA) versus professional SLT


(SLTB)

Four subcomparisons compared participants that received volunteer-facilitated SLT and participants that received professional SLT
provided directly in a clinical setting by a professional therapist
(Leal 1993; Meikle 1979; Meinzer 2007; Wertz 1986iii). In most
cases professional SLT was delivered by a speech and language
therapist (Leal 1993; Meikle 1979; Wertz 1986iii) though delivery of the constraint-induced SLT intervention in Meinzer 2007
was delivered by a specialist psychologist. We believed that this
trial was suitable for inclusion in this comparison as it compared
interventions delivered by a professional clinician with delivery
facilitated by a trained volunteer.
Most volunteers were family members (Leal 1993; Meinzer 2007;
Wertz 1986iii) although some trialists also engaged friends (Wertz
1986iii) or recruited volunteers unknown to the participants
(Meikle 1979; Wertz 1986iii). Volunteer groups across the trials
all received SLT training, information on their patients communication impairment, access to working materials or equipment,
and ongoing support or supervision. Most studies indicated that
the professional therapist was accountable for, or informed the design and content of the volunteer-facilitated SLT (Meikle 1979;
Meinzer 2007; Wertz 1986iii).
The professional therapists were based in a formal or clinical setting (Leal 1993; Meikle 1979; Meinzer 2007; Wertz 1986iii). The
duration of the professional SLT interventions varied from three
hours daily for 10 consecutive days (Meinzer 2007) or up to three
hours (Leal 1993), four hours (Meikle 1979) or 10 hours weekly
for approximately three months (Wertz 1986iii), six months (Leal
1993) or an average of nine months (SD 22 weeks) (Meikle 1979).
The duration of volunteer-facilitated SLT and professionally-delivered SLT was the same for two subcomparisons (Meinzer 2007;
Wertz 1986iii). The volunteers in Meikle 1979 visited participants
four times weekly over a shorter period of time (average of five
months (SD 13.5 weeks)) while the duration of the volunteerfacilitated SLT in Leal 1993 is unclear. The four subcomparisons
used a range of measures to compare volunteer-facilitated SLT
with professional SLT delivery including (a) functional communication, (b) receptive language, (c) expressive language, (d) written
language, (e) severity of impairment, (f ) number of drop-outs and
(g) compliance with allocation.

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

20

(a) Functional communication


Only Wertz 1986iii formally measured the functional communication skills of the participants that received volunteer-facilitated
SLT or professional SLT using the CADL and the Functional
Communication Profile. There was no evidence of a difference
between the groups (Analysis 5.1).

(b) Receptive language

SLT and those that received professional SLT. There was no evidence of a difference between the groups (Analysis 5.5).

Expressive language: repetition


The group that received the volunteer-facilitated SLT intervention
scored significantly higher on the Repetition subtest (AAT) than
those that received SLT from a professional therapist (P = 0.05,
MD 13.50, 95% CI 0.19 to 26.81) (Meinzer 2007) (Analysis 5.6).

Receptive language: auditory comprehension


Three subcomparisons evaluated participants language comprehension abilities using the Token Test (Leal 1993; Meinzer 2007;
Wertz 1986iii) but suitable statistical data were unavailable for
Leal 1993. Meinzer 2007 also used the AAT to measure Auditory and Reading comprehension skills. Meinzer 2007 and Wertz
1986iii used the Token Test to measure differences in the auditory
comprehension of participants that received volunteer-facilitated
SLT and those that received professional therapy input. There was
no significant difference between the two groups auditory comprehension (Analysis 5.2). The comprehension subtest of the AAT
measures both auditory and reading comprehension and was used
by Meinzer 2007 to compare a group receiving volunteer-facilitated SLT or SLT delivered by experienced professionals. There
was no evidence of a difference between the groups comprehension on this measure (Analysis 5.2).

Receptive language: reading comprehension (RCBA)


Wertz 1986iii measured participants reading comprehension using the Reading Comprehension Battery for Aphasia. There was
no evidence of a difference between the groups (Analysis 5.3).

Expressive language: written


The written language subtest of the AAT measures reading aloud
and writing to dictation. Meinzer 2007 compared the groups that
received volunteer-facilitated SLT and those that received professionally delivered SLT using this measure. Similarly, Wertz 1986iii
used the PICA Graphic subtest to compare the groups but found
no evidence of a difference (Analysis 5.7).

(d) Severity of impairment


Four subcomparisons compared the two groups using measures
of overall severity of aphasia following either volunteer-facilitated
SLT or professional SLT using the PICA (Meikle 1979; Wertz
1986iii), an aphasia quotient (Leal 1993) and the AAT profile
(Meinzer 2007). Summary data from the groups performance was
unavailable for Leal 1993 preventing inclusion within the review.
There was no evidence of a difference between the two groups
following pooling of data from the PICA and AAT profile (Analysis
5.8).

(e) Number of drop-outs


Receptive language: other
Wertz 1986iii compared participants receptive language skills using the PICA Gestural subtest. There was no evidence of a difference between the groups (Analysis 5.4).

(c) Expressive language

Expressive language: spoken


Meinzer 2007 measured expressive language skills using the Naming subtests of the AAT while Wertz 1986iii used the PICA Verbal
Subtest to compare participants that received volunteer-facilitated

All four subcomparisons reported the number of participants that


were lost to the trial following randomisation. Across three subcomparisons a total of 30 participants were lost from the groups
that experienced volunteer-facilitated SLT while 22 participants
were lost from the groups that received professional SLT interventions (Leal 1993; Meikle 1979; Wertz 1986iii). Meinzer 2007
experienced no participant withdrawals. An additional participant
that had received volunteer-facilitated SLT and two participants
that had received professional SLT were lost at follow up (Wertz
1986iii). No participants were reported lost at follow up from
Leal 1993. Overall, there was no evidence of a difference in the
numbers of drop-outs between the groups that received volunteerfacilitated SLT and those that had professionally delivered SLT
(Analysis 5.9).

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

21

(f ) Compliance with allocated intervention


Only two of the three trials provided details for participant withdrawals (Leal 1993; Meikle 1979). Overall, five participants declined to continue participating in the volunteer-facilitated SLT
groups while four declined in the professional SLT groups.

pooling these data with the Token Test data from the Pulvermuller
2001 comparison, there was no evidence of a difference between
the groups auditory comprehension skills, nor was there any indication of a difference between the groups on the AAT comprehension subtest (Pulvermuller 2001) (Analysis 6.1).

6. Group SLT (SLTA) versus one-to-one SLT (SLT B)

Receptive language: other

Three subcomparisons compared group SLT to conventional oneto-one SLT (Jufeng 2005iii; Pulvermuller 2001; Wertz 1981).
Within the group SLT interventions, participants received SLT in
groups of three plus a therapist (Pulvermuller 2001), between three
to seven (Wertz 1981) or 10 patients (Jufeng 2005iii). Participants
allocated to group SLT in Pulvermuller 2001 received a constraintinduced approach to SLT (only verbal responses were allowed). In
contrast, the group SLT intervention in Wertz 1981 encouraged
group discussion and recreational activities with a therapist while
Jufeng 2005iii focused on collective language strengthening training. In all cases the patients in the one-to-one SLT intervention
received conventional SLT (stimulus-response treatment across all
modalities). Between-intervention comparisons were made on a
variety of measures: (a) functional communication, (b) receptive
language, (c) expressive language, (d) severity of impairment, (e)
number of drop-outs and (f ) compliance with allocated intervention. Psychosocial and economic measures were not compared.

Wertz 1981 used the PICA Gestural Subtest to compare participants that had received group SLT and those that had received
one-to-one SLT. Though the mean values were available to the
review the SD values were unavailable. A standard deviation value
(25.67) was identified and imputed from Wertz 1986 where the
highest of three possible values in this trial from relevant clinical
groups was chosen to facilitate inclusion of the study within the
review (Analysis 6.2).

(a) Functional communication


Two subcomparisons measured change in functional communication using the Communicative Activity Log (CAL) (Pulvermuller
2001), the Conversational Rating Scale (Wertz 1981) and the Informants Rating of Functional Language (adapted form of the
Functional Communication Profile) (Wertz 1981). However, suitable statistical data were unavailable and so could not be included
within the review.

(c) Expressive language

Expressive language: spoken


Pulvermuller 2001 and Wertz 1981 measured participants expressive language skills using the naming subtest of the AAT, measures
of word fluency, and the PICA verbal subtest. Using the AAT naming subtest Pulvermuller 2001 found no evidence of a difference
between the groups expressive language skills (Analysis 6.3). Wertz
1981 used the verbal subtest of the PICA to measure participants
language comprehension skills. The mean scores of participants
that received group SLT and those that received one-to-one SLT
were available but SD data were not. A standard deviation value
(20.01) was identified and imputed from Wertz 1986 where the
highest of three possible values in this trial from relevant clinical
groups was chosen to facilitate inclusion of the study within the
review (Analysis 6.3). There was no evidence of a difference between the groups.

(b) Receptive language


Expressive language: word fluency
Receptive language: auditory comprehension
Two subcomparisons measured participants receptive language
skills using the Token Test (Pulvermuller 2001; Wertz 1981) and
the language comprehension subtest of the AAT (Pulvermuller
2001). Mean values were reported for Wertz 1981 but the standard
deviation (SD) values were unavailable. To facilitate inclusion of
these data within the review, the standard deviation value (13.93)
has been imputed from the Lincoln 1982 Token Test summary
data. The reason for choosing this value was both Wertz 1981 and
Lincoln 1982 used the same form of the Token Test and used it
to measure the language skills of similar participant groups. On

Measures of word fluency were used by Wertz 1981 to compare


participants word finding skills. Mean values for the participants
receiving group SLT and those receiving one-to-one SLT were
reported but no SDs were available and so these data could not be
included in this review.

Expressive language: repetition


Participants repetition abilities were compared by Pulvermuller
2001 using the AAT repetition subtest and no evidence of a difference between the groups was found (Analysis 6.4).

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

22

Expressive language: written


Wertz 1981 used the Graphic Subtest of the PICA to compare
participants written language skills. Mean values for those participants that received group SLT and those that received one-to-one
SLT were reported but SDs were unavailable. As with the other
PICA data from Wertz 1981, a standard deviation value (21.74)
was identified and imputed from Wertz 1986 where the highest
of three possible values in this trial from relevant clinical groups
was chosen to facilitate inclusion of the study within the review
(Analysis 6.5). There was no evidence of a difference between the
groups.

(d) Severity of impairment


Three subcomparisons measured the severity of participants aphasia following interventions using the CRRCAE (Jufeng 2005iii),
the PICA (Wertz 1981) and the AAT (Pulvermuller 2001). Summary data from Jufeng 2005iii and Pulvermuller 2001 were available for inclusion within the meta-analysis. Though the mean values for Wertz 1981 trial were available the SD data were missing.
We imputed a SD value (24.64) from Wertz 1986 to facilitate
inclusion of the data within the review. There was no evidence
of a difference between the scores of participants that received
group SLT and those that received one-to-one SLT on this measure
(Analysis 6.6). On follow up at three months the participants that
had received group SLT performed significantly better on the CRRCAE than those that had received one-to-one SLT (P < 0.0001,
MD 33.41, 95% CI 16.76 to 50.06) (Analysis 6.7).

(e) Number of drop-outs


Information on the number of participants leaving during the trials were available for all three subcomparisons (Jufeng 2005iii;
Pulvermuller 2001; Wertz 1981). Two subcomparisons experienced no drop-outs (Jufeng 2005iii; Pulvermuller 2001). In contrast, almost half those randomised in Wertz 1981 failed to remain
in the study (33 drop-outs) but there was no evidence of a difference in the numbers lost to each intervention (Analysis 6.8).

(f ) Compliance with allocated intervention


Twenty-two participants were reported to have returned home or
declined to travel to receive the allocated treatment intervention
(see Table 2) but further details on the exact number of participants
declining the interventions or how these numbers are split across
groups was unavailable.

7. Task-specific SLT (SLT A) versus conventional SLT (SLT B)

Eight subcomparisons compared the use of a task-specific approach to SLT with a more generalist conventional SLT approach
(Drummond 1981; Lincoln 1984b; Lincoln 1982i; Lincoln

1982ii; Prins 1989; Pulvermuller 2001; Shewan 1984i; van


Steenbrugge 1981). The range of task-specific SLT A interventions included AMERIND signs used as cues for word-finding
impairment (Drummond 1981); operant training (Lincoln 1982i;
Lincoln 1982ii); operant training with programmed instruction
(Lincoln 1984b); Systematic Therapy for Auditory Comprehension Disorders in Aphasic Patients (STACDAP) (Prins 1989); constraint-induced therapy (Pulvermuller 2001); language-oriented
SLT (Shewan 1984i) and SLT for naming and constructing sentences (van Steenbrugge 1981).
Within this comparison we have included data from Lincoln
1982i, Lincoln 1982ii and Lincoln 1984b, which has been extracted from two cross-over trials (described earlier).
Lincoln 1982i and Lincoln 1982ii randomised participants across
four groups that compared SLT including an operant training
adjunct to SLT with a social support and stimulation adjunct. The
social support and stimulation component acted as a placebo for
the operant training adjunct in the comparison groups. In both
of these subcomparisons the means and SD have been extracted
from the unpublished individual patient data and is inclusive of
the treatment cross-over period. Given the complementary nature
of the cross-over intervention (SLT plus operant training) or (SLT
plus social support) and the clinically relevant nature of the crossover treatments we felt it was appropriate to include these data
within this section of the review. As recommended, we have also
analysed and presented the cross-over inclusive data from these
subcomparisons in separate meta-analyses for readers information
(Analysis 8.1; Analysis 8.2; Analysis 8.3; Analysis 8.4; Analysis
8.5).
All eight subcomparisons evaluating the impact of these specialised
SLT interventions did so by comparing them with conventional
SLT. However, in Lincoln 1984b the conventional SLT group also
had a non-verbal tasks (matching, copying and recall of designs
plus manual dexterity tasks) which acted as a control for the specialist intervention. Similarly, in Lincoln 1982i and Lincoln 1982ii
the participants in the conventional SLT group also had access to
additional structured social stimulation in the form of topic-led
conversations with the therapist.
A range of outcome measures were used by these subcomparisons:
(a) functional communication, (b) receptive language, (c) expressive language, (d) severity of impairment, (e) number of drop-outs
and (f ) compliance with allocated intervention.

(a) Functional communication


Two subcomparisons reported functional communication skills
of participants as measured on the Functional Expression Scale
(Prins 1989; van Steenbrugge 1981). Pulvermuller 2001 measured
functional skills using the Communication Activity Log but these
data were unavailable for the review. On pooling the available
data there was no evidence of a difference between the groups
functional communication skills (Analysis 7.1).

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

23

(b) Receptive language

Receptive language: reading comprehension


One subcomparison measured participants ability to comprehend
written words (Prins 1989). There was no evidence of a difference
between the groups (Analysis 7.5).

Receptive language: auditory comprehension of single words


Six subcomparisons considered participants auditory comprehension skills across a range of comprehension complexities (Lincoln
1984b; Lincoln 1982i; Lincoln 1982ii; Prins 1989; Pulvermuller
2001; Shewan 1984i). Two subcomparisons measured participants ability to understand single words using the Word Naming
BDAE subtest (Lincoln 1984b; Prins 1989), the Body part identification BDAE subtest (Prins 1989) and the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (Lincoln 1984b). Following pooling of the results,
where possible, there was no indication of a difference between
the groups that received task-specific SLT interventions and those
that had received conventional SLT (Analysis 7.2).

Receptive language: other auditory comprehension


Two subcomparisons measured participants ability to comprehend sentences using Miscellaneous Commands (Prins 1989) and
the Aphasia Comprehension Test for Sentences (Shewan 1984i).
Appropriate statistical data from Shewan 1984i were unavailable
and so could not be included in the meta-analysis. There was no
evidence of a difference between the groups sentence comprehension abilities (Analysis 7.3). Prins 1989 measured participants
comprehension skills across levels of complexity on the AmAT
Comprehension Subtest. There was no evidence of a difference between the groups comprehension skills on this measure (Analysis
7.3). Participants auditory comprehension skills on the AAT subtest were tested by Pulvermuller 2001 and there was no evidence
of a difference between the groups performance on this measure (Analysis 7.2). Five subcomparisons evaluated comprehension skills using the Token Test (Lincoln 1982i; Lincoln 1982ii;
Lincoln 1984b; Prins 1989; Pulvermuller 2001). On pooling of
the available data there was no evidence of a difference between
the groups comprehension skills (Analysis 7.3).

Receptive language: other


Three subcomparisons measured gestural skills on the PICA
subtests which incorporates measures of not just gesture abilities
but also subtests of auditory comprehension and reading abilities
(Lincoln 1982i; Lincoln 1982ii; Lincoln 1984b). Following pooling of these data there was no evidence of a difference in the gestural skills of participants that received SLT with operant training
and those that received SLT with a placebo adjunct (Analysis 7.6).
(c) Expressive language
Participants expressive language skills were considered by seven
subcomparisons (Drummond 1981; Lincoln 1982i; Lincoln
1982ii; Lincoln 1984b; Prins 1989; Pulvermuller 2001; van
Steenbrugge 1981) across a range of levels of complexity from object naming to sentence construction tasks.

Expressive language: spoken naming


Six subcomparisons asked participants to name a variety of nouns
using the Object Naming Test (Lincoln 1982i; Lincoln 1982ii),
the AmAT Naming Test (Prins 1989; van Steenbrugge 1981), 20
items from the Taylor Aphasia Therapy Kit (Drummond 1981)
and the AAT Naming Subtest (Pulvermuller 2001). We were unable to obtain suitable summary data from Drummond 1981 to
permit inclusion within the meta-analysis. There was no evidence
of a difference between the groups naming skills. Nor was there
any evidence of a naming difference between the groups in the
van Steenbrugge 1981 subcomparison at three-week follow up
(Analysis 7.7).

Expressive language: spoken sentence construction


Receptive language: auditory comprehension (treated items)
One subcomparison also reported separate results for components of word and sentence comprehension that had been targeted
within the STADCAP SLT treatment intervention. Participants
ability to comprehend three tests of word or sentence comprehension that depended on phoneme recognition, lexicon and morphological skills (Prins 1989) were measured and compared. There
was no evidence of a difference between the groups given taskspecific SLT or conventional SLT (Analysis 7.4).

Prins 1989 and van Steenbrugge 1981 also compared participants


ability to construct sentences but there was no evidence of a difference between the groups nor was any indication of a difference between the groups at three-week follow up (van Steenbrugge 1981)
(Analysis 7.8).

Expressive language: other spoken tasks


Lincoln 1982i and Lincoln 1982ii compared participants expressive language skills using word fluency tasks and on pooling found

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

24

those that received conventional SLT performed better than those


that had received task-specific SLT (P = 0.02, SMD -1.05, 95% CI
-1.93 to -0.17). The two subcomparisons also asked participants
to describe a picture and compared their abilities on this expressive
language task but there was no evidence of a difference between
the groups. The PICA Verbal Subtest was used by three subcomparisons to compare participants that received task-specific SLT
and conventional SLT but on pooling there was no evidence of
a difference between the groups (Lincoln 1982i; Lincoln 1982ii;
Lincoln 1984b) (Analysis 7.9).

Expressive language: spoken (treated items)


Participants expressive language skills on items that had been
treated within the specialist Naming and Sentence Construction
SLT intervention were compared to participants expressive abilities on these items following conventional SLT (van Steenbrugge
1981). There was some trend towards better naming of treated
items (P = 0.06) from those participants that had received conventional SLT as compared with those who had received task-specific
SLT, with a similar trend observed at three-week follow up. There
was no evidence of a difference between the groups sentence construction skills (Analysis 7.10).

Expressive language: repetition


Pulvermuller 2001 was the only subcomparison to compare participants repetition skills following constraint-induced therapy or
conventional SLT interventions and there was no evidence of a
difference between the groups (Analysis 7.11).

that received a task-specific SLT intervention that included operant training (P = 0.05, MD -0.74, 95% CI -1.50 to 0.01) (Analysis
8.5). There was no evidence of a difference between the groups on
the AAT measure (Pulvermuller 2001) (Analysis 7.13).
(e) Number of drop-outs
Only three subcomparisons reported a loss of participants from
their trials (Lincoln 1982i; Lincoln 1982ii; Shewan 1984i). No
participants were lost from the other five subcomparisons. Thirteen participants were lost across the four groups in Lincoln 1982i
and Lincoln 1982ii but it is unclear to which groups these participants had been randomised. In contrast, Shewan 1984i reported
that six participants dropped out from the language-orientated
SLT intervention while only one dropped out of the conventional
SLT group. There was no significant difference between the numbers of participants lost to each intervention (Analysis 7.14).
(f ) Compliance with allocated intervention
As described above, only one subcomparison provided details of
the participants that dropped out of their trial (Shewan 1984i) with
only two deciding to withdraw from the language-orientated SLT
intervention. None voluntarily withdrew from the conventional
SLT group.

8. Operant training SLT (SLT A) versus conventional SLT


(SLTB)

The subcomparisons taken from the cross-over trials are presented


separately within the data and analysis table for information purposes.

9. Semantic SLT (SLT A) versus phonological SLT (SLTB)

Expressive language: written

(d) Severity of impairment

Doesborgh 2004a randomised 58 participants to receive either semantic SLT or phonological SLT. The semantic SLT approach focused on improving semantic processing by employing semantic
decision tasks at word, sentence and text level while the phonological SLT approach focused on sound structure by targeting phonological input and output. Between group comparisons were made
on the basis of (a) functional communication, (b) receptive language, (c) expressive language, (d) number of drop-outs and (e)
compliance with allocated intervention. The psychosocial impact,
severity of impairment and economic outcomes were not measured.

Participants overall severity of aphasia impairment was considered


by five subcomparisons using the PICA (Lincoln 1982i; Lincoln
1982ii; Lincoln 1984b), the AAT (Pulvermuller 2001) and the
WAB (Shewan 1984i). Suitable data from Shewan 1984i were unavailable and so could not be included in this meta-analysis. Following pooling of the PICA data, conventional SLT participants
were found to experience significantly less impairment than those

Doesborgh 2004a used the ANELT-A to compare groups that


received semantic SLT to those that received phonological SLT.
There was no evidence of a difference between the two groups
functional communication skills (Analysis 9.1).

Three subcomparisons measured participants written language


expressive skills on the PICA Graphic subtest (Lincoln 1982i;
Lincoln 1982ii; Lincoln 1984b). On pooling, those participants
that had received conventional SLT were found to have significantly better written expressive language skills that those that had
received task-specific SLT (P = 0.05, MD -0.85, 95% CI -1.69 to
-0.1) (Analysis 7.12).

(a) Functional communication

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

25

(b) Receptive language

not report group comparisons on the basis of functional communication, expressive language, severity of impairment or economic
outcome measures. No participants withdrew from the study so
comparison based on the number of drop-outs was not possible.

Receptive language: auditory comprehension


Participants auditory comprehension skills were measured by
Doesborgh 2004a using the Semantic Association Test and the
Auditory Lexical Decision Subtests of the PALPA. Using changefrom-baseline values there was no evidence of a difference between
the groups on the Semantic Association Test but the group that
received the phonological SLT performed significantly better on
the Auditory Lexical Decision Subtest than those that received
semantic SLT (P = 0.01, MD -3.50, 95% CI -6.23 to -0.77)
(Analysis 9.2).

(a) Receptive language

Receptive language: auditory


The participants word comprehension skills were measured using
a vocabulary test constructed by Di Carlo 1980. There was no
evidence of a difference between the groups (Analysis 10.1).

Receptive language: reading

Receptive language: reading

Doesborgh 2004a also measured the two groups synonym judgements using a subtest of the PALPA. This test required both synonym judgement and reading comprehension abilities. There was
no evidence of a difference between the groups (Analysis 9.3).

Di Carlo 1980 compared the two groups reading comprehension


skills using the Reading Recognition and Reading Comprehension
Test. There was no evidence of a difference between the groups on
either of these measures (Analysis 10.2).

(c) Expressive language: repetition

Summary of results

The only measure of expressive skill used by Doesborgh 2004a


was that of the PALPA non-word repetition subtest. There was
no evidence of a difference between the two groups (Analysis 9.4).
(d) Number of drop-outs
Doesborgh 2004a reported the loss from follow up of a total of
12 participants. Equal numbers were lost from both the semantic
SLT and the phonological SLT groups (Analysis 9.5).
(e) Compliance with allocated intervention
Reasons for the loss of 12 participants from the treatment phase
were given by Doesborgh 2004a. Within the semantic SLT group
four participants received less than 40 hours of the planned treatment intervention while in the phonological SLT group two participants received less than 40 hours of treatment and two participants declined to complete the final assessment.

10. Programmed instruction (SLT A) versus nonprogramme instruction (SLTB)

One subcomparison (Di Carlo 1980) investigated the effectiveness of a filmed programmed instruction intervention compared
to non-programmed activity, described by the trialists as viewing
slides and bibliotherapy. Both groups had received conventional
SLT and continued to do so throughout the trial. Di Carlo 1980
compared the groups on measures of receptive language. They did

1. SLT versus no SLT (14 subcomparisons)

Functional communication: eight subcomparisons (data


from five); four measures; no evidence of a difference.
Receptive language: five subcomparisons (data from five);
seven measures; PICA gestural subtest favours SLT P = 0.02, no
other evidence of a difference.
Expressive language: five subcomparisons (data from five);
seven measures; no evidence of a difference.
Severity of impairment: 10 subcomparisons (data from six);
four measures; no evidence of a difference.
Psychosocial impact: four subcomparisons (data from one);
six measures; no evidence of a difference.
Drop-outs: 14 subcomparisons (data from 14); no evidence
of a difference.
Compliance: 11 subcomparisons (data from two); no
evidence of a difference.
Economic outcomes: one subcomparison (no data).
2. SLT versus social support and stimulation (six
subcomparisons)

Functional communication: two subcomparisons (data from


one); three measures; no evidence of a difference.
Receptive language: four subcomparisons (data from two);
five measures; PICA subtest favours social support and
stimulation group (P = 0.04); no other evidence of a difference.

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

26

Expressive language: three subcomparisons (data from two);


six measures; Object Naming Test and Word Fluency favours
social support and stimulation group (P = 0.003 and P <
0.0001); Caplan and Hanna Sentence Production Test (treated
items) favours SLT (P = 0.01); PICA Verbal and Graphic subtests
favour social support and stimulation group (P = 0.0007; P =
0.01).
Severity: four subcomparisons (data from one); two
measures; PICA favours social support and stimulation group (P
= 0.005).
Psychosocial impact: one subcomparison (no data).
Drop-outs: five subcomparisons (data from four); no
evidence of a difference.
Compliance: four subcomparisons (data from four); no
evidence of a difference.
Economic outcomes: not measured.
3. SLT A versus SLT B (21 subcomparisons)

Functional SLT versus conventional SLT (one


subcomparison)
Functional communication: one subcomparison (data from
one); six measures; CADL change-from-baseline favours
conventional SLT (P = 0.001); Telephone Ordering Task (with
and without concurrent task) favours functional SLT (P =
0.0001 and P = 0.03).
Receptive language: not measured.
Expressive language: one subcomparison (data from one);
two measures; no evidence of a difference.
Severity of impairment: not measured.
Psychosocial impact: not measured.
Drop-outs: one subcomparison (data from one); no dropouts.
Compliance: not applicable.
Economic outcomes: not measured.
Intensive versus conventional SLT (four subcomparisons)
Functional communication: not measured.
Receptive language: one subcomparison (data from one); two
measures; AAT Comprehension subtest approaching
significance, favouring intensive SLT (P = 0.06); no other
evidence of a difference.
Expressive language: one subcomparison (data from one);
three measures; AAT written subtest favours intensive SLT (P =
0.01); no other evidence of a difference.
Severity of impairment: four subcomparisons (data from
three); three measures; no evidence of a difference; AAT profile
change from baseline scores suggesting a trend towards favouring
intensive SLT (P = 0.08).
Psychosocial impact: one subcomparison (no data).

Drop-outs: four subcomparisons (data from two); favours


conventional SLT (P = 0.05).
Compliance: two subcomparisons (data from one); no
evidence of a difference.
Economic outcomes: not measured.

Volunteer-facilitated SLT versus professional SLT (four


subcomparisons)
Functional communication: one subcomparison (data from
one); two measures; no evidence of a difference.
Receptive language: three subcomparisons (data from two);
four measures; no evidence of a difference.
Expressive language: two subcomparisons (data from two);
five measures; AAT Repetition subtest favoured volunteerfacilitated SLT (P = 0.05); no other evidence of a difference.
Severity of impairment: four subcomparisons (data from
three); three measures; no evidence of a difference.
Psychosocial impact: not measured.
Drop-outs: four subcomparisons (data from four); no
evidence of a difference.
Compliance: three subcomparisons (data from two); no
evidence of a difference.
Economic outcomes: not measured.

Group SLT versus conventional SLT (three subcomparisons)


Functional communication: two subcomparisons (no data);
three measures.
Receptive language: two subcomparisons (data from two);
three measures; no evidence of a difference.
Expressive language: two subcomparisons (data from two);
five measures; no evidence of a difference.
Severity of impairment: three subcomparisons (data from
three); three measures; CRRCAE favoured group SLT at threemonth follow up (P < 0.0001); no other evidence of a difference.
Psychosocial impact: not measured.
Drop-outs: three subcomparisons (data from three); no
evidence of a difference.
Compliance: one subcomparison (no data).
Economic outcomes: not measured.

Task-specific SLT versus conventional SLT (eight


subcomparisons)
Functional communication: three subcomparisons (data
from two); two measures; no evidence of a difference.
Receptive language: six subcomparisons (data from five); 12
measures; no evidence of a difference.
Expressive language: seven subcomparisons (data from six);
12 measures; word fluency and written language favoured
conventional SLT (P = 0.02 and P = 0.05); naming (treated

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

27

items) and at three-week follow up there was a trend towards


favouring task-specific SLT (P = 0.06).
Severity of impairment: five subcomparisons (data from
four); three measures; PICA favours conventional SLT (P = 0.05).
Psychosocial impact: not measured.
Drop-outs: eight subcomparisons (data from six); no
evidence of a difference.
Compliance: one subcomparison (data from one); no
evidence of a difference.
Economic outcomes: not measured.
Operant training versus conventional SLT (three
subcomparisons)
Functional communication: not measured
Receptive language: three subcomparisons (data from three);
four measures; no evidence of a difference.
Expressive language: three subcomparisons (data from three);
five measures; word fluency and PICA Graphic subtest favoured
conventional SLT (P = 0.02 and P = 0.05); no other evidence of
a difference.
Severity of impairment: three subcomparisons (data from
three); one measure; PICA overall favoured conventional SLT (P
= 0.05).
Psychosocial impact: not measured.
Drop-outs: two subcomparisons (no data).
Compliance: two subcomparisons (no data).
Economic outcomes: not measured.
Semantic SLT versus phonological SLT (one subcomparison)
Functional communication: one subcomparison (data from
one); one measure; no evidence of a difference.
Receptive language: one subcomparison (data from one);
three measures; Auditory Lexical Decision favoured phonological
SLT (P = 0.01); no other evidence of a difference.
Expressive language: one subcomparison (data from one);
one measure; no evidence of a difference.
Severity of impairment: not measured.
Psychosocial impact: not measured.
Drop-outs: one subcomparison (data from one); no evidence
of a difference.
Compliance: one subcomparison (data from one); no
evidence of a difference.
Economic outcomes: not measured.
Programmed instruction versus non-programmed
instruction (one subcomparison)
Functional communication: not measured.
Receptive language: one subcomparison (data from one);
three measures; no evidence of a difference.
Expressive language: not measured.

Severity of impairment: not measured.


Psychosocial impact: not measured.
Drop-outs: not measured.
Compliance: not measured.
Economic outcomes: not measured.

DISCUSSION
We updated this complex review of the effectiveness of speech and
language therapy interventions for people with aphasia following
stroke to reflect new evidence and developments in clinical practice. We assessed whether (1) SLT is more effective than no SLT,
(2) SLT is more effective than social support and stimulation and
(3) one SLT intervention is more effective than another. The data
from 20 additional trials were identified, synthesised and presented
together with data from 10 trials included in the original review.

Summary of main results


A total of 1840 participants were randomised across 41 subcomparisons. Fourteen compared participants who received SLT with
those who did not. Significant differences between the groups
scores were few but there was some indication of a consistency in
the direction of the results which favoured the provision of SLT.
More data are required to further inform this comparison.
Six subcomparisons compared groups who received SLT with
groups who received social support and stimulation. Most of the
data derived from one small trial prior to cross-over which provided some evidence of a difference in receptive and expressive
language skills and severity which favoured the social support and
stimulation group. This finding is heavily reliant on the data from
a single trial and additional data are required to confirm whether
social support and stimulation provides benefits to some aspects of
participants language skills and on measures of severity of aphasia
impairment.
Twenty-one subcomparisons compared two different types of SLT.
In general, comparisons were based on a small number of subcomparisons involving few participants. Based on the findings of
one trial, functional SLT was found to improve functional tasks
but in more general measures of communicative activities of daily
living the conventional SLT approach was better. Additional data
are required to further inform the comparison.
Intensive SLT was compared to conventional SLT by four subcomparisons and the intensive approach was found to result in benefits in participants written language skills with some indications
of improvements in their receptive language and severity of impairment measures as compared to a conventional SLT approach.
However, the number of participants dropping out from the intensive SLT groups was significantly higher than the conventional

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

28

SLT group suggesting that such an intensive approach to therapy


is not suited to all patients.
Volunteer-facilitated SLT did not seem to differ remarkably from
professional SLT. This is unsurprising as the volunteers providing
the SLT interventions were trained by the professional therapists,
had been given access to the relevant therapy materials and the
plan for therapeutic interventions was developed by (or under
the direction of ) the professional therapist. The only betweengroup difference identified in this comparison (derived from a
single trial) indicated that the volunteer-facilitated group scored
better on measures of spoken repetition than the conventional SLT
group.
There were very limited data and little evidence of any difference
between group SLT and one-to-one SLT.
Comparisons of task-specific SLT approaches to the provision of
generalist conventional SLT approaches found little evidence of a
between-group difference. Only on the PICA measure of severity
of impairment were the groups found to differ, favouring the conventional SLT approach rather than an operant training SLT approach. This was further supported on closer examination of the
operant training SLT in comparison to conventional SLT when,
in addition to less severity, the conventional SLT groups were also
found to have significantly better scores on measures of word fluency and writing than the operant training SLT groups. While
task-specific approaches may provide more benefits in the retrieval
of language items targeted in therapy than conventional SLT approaches, this raises the question of how functionally relevant such
improvements are.

Overall completeness and applicability of


evidence
We identified a great number of trials (and subcomparisons) of
relevance to our review question with most eligible for inclusion
within the review. However, across the trials included in the review there was a lack of comprehensive data collection, a wide
range of outcome tools employed and disappointingly inadequate
reporting of outcome measures. Within the review, approximately
half of the subcomparisons described measuring receptive (N =
23) and expressive language skills (N = 23) with only two-thirds
reporting suitable data in published format, which permitted inclusion within this review. Thanks to several trialists contributions of unpublished data we were able to fully include approximately 70% to 80% of the receptive measures (N = 19) and expressive measures (N = 21) within the review. The severity of participants aphasia impairment was evaluated by 30 subcomparisons
but unfortunately we were only able to included suitable data from
20 subcomparisons. Similarly, while one subcomparison reported
measuring economic outcomes, no data were available. Few subcomparisons measured participants functional and psychosocial
outcomes, measures that are probably most closely aligned to the
patients sense of recovery and return to normal. From the total

of 41 subcomparisons, less than half (N = 17) described measuring changes in functional communication and of these only half
(N = 11) reported data that could be included within the review.
Even fewer measured psychosocial outcomes (N = 5) and only one
reported data suitable for inclusion within the review.
The degree to which the models of conventional SLT employed
within the trials are reflective of therapists current practice should
be carefully considered across individual treatments in terms of
the frequency, duration and the extent of therapeutic intervention. Participants came from across a wide age range and were experiencing a range of aphasia impairments. However, the length
of time since participants stroke raises questions of how clinically relevant some recruitment parameters were to a SLT clinical
population. Only a quarter of the included subcomparisons (N
= 10) recruited participants within the first few weeks following
their stroke (a participant group of high clinical relevance) while
almost half the subcomparisons (N = 18) recruited participants
six months or more (in some cases many years) following their
stroke: a group that are highly unlikely to be seen in a clinical
setting for rehabilitation purposes. Such recruitment procedures
and the involvement of some participants up to 28 years after the
onset of their aphasia is of limited application to either a clinical
or treatment evaluation setting.

Quality of the evidence


This update adds a significant amount of data to the original review
and so, together with newly improved systematic review methodologies, we are in a better position to draw conclusions regarding
the effectiveness of SLT for aphasia following stroke. In this review we have included a total of 30 trials (which consisted of 41
subcomparisons) involving data from 1840 patients. Methods of
random sequence generation and concealment of allocation were
considered adequate in 13 and six subcomparisons respectively.
The randomisation methodology for most of the remaining subcomparisons had been inadequately described and so it was not
possible to judge the quality of randomisation. The lack of description and detail does not necessarily mean inadequate procedures
were in place but rather a lack of reporting of this detail (Soares
2004). The prevalence of good methodology in relation to blinding of outcome assessors may support this interpretation. Blinding of the outcome assessors was much more widely reported with
more than half of the subcomparisons within the review, 23 of
them, describing adequate blinding procedures. Only eight were
considered not to have adequately blinded assessors with 10 providing too little detail to make a judgement regarding the quality
of blinding.
Almost three-quarters of the subcomparisons in this review (N =
30) were published before the CONSORT statement (Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials) (Altman 2001; Moher 2001).
Disappointingly, of the 11 subcomparisons published in the last
five years (and after the implementation of the CONSORT state-

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

29

ment) only four reported the method of generating the randomisation sequence and the methods of concealing allocation. This is
however an improvement on the 30 earlier subcomparisons where
the method of random sequence generation and concealment of allocation was only available for one-fifth of subcomparisons. Thus,
there is some indication of improvements in the quality of the trial
methodologies or of their reporting.
Only two trials reported an a priori power size calculation, which
is reflected in the small numbers of randomised participants across
the subcomparisons: three randomised 10 or fewer participants,
22 randomised up to 50 participants, 13 between 50 and 100
participants, and only three subcomparisons randomised over 100
participants. The randomisation of such small numbers of participants reduces the power of the statistical analyses, raises questions
of the reliability of findings and (given the complexity of various
aphasia impairments) will cause difficulties in ensuring the comparability of the groups at baseline. In this review, we found onefifth of the included subcomparisons had groups that significantly
differed at baseline and group comparability was unclear for another fifth.
Despite these reporting and methodological limitations we have
synthesised a large number of trials that address the effectiveness
of SLT for aphasia following stroke across a number of outcome
measures. Across these measures there is some indication of a consistency in the direction of results when looking at SLT versus
no SLT which appears to favour SLT. With at least nine additional trials of relevance to this review currently ongoing or about
to report, the picture based on the current evidence for SLT for
aphasia following stroke will develop further over time. With a
clear consistency in the direction of results to date in many of the
measures we can be hopeful that with the availability of additional
data the evidence will become more conclusive in relation to the
effectiveness of SLT, social support and different approaches to
SLT provision.
Sixteen of the 41 subcomparisons in this review included all randomised participants in their final analyses. The remaining 25 subcomparisons lost participants during the treatment or follow-up
phases but none employed an intention-to-treat analysis. In some
cases large proportions of participants withdrew from some interventions and in some this appeared to be linked to the intervention itself, with significantly more participants withdrawing from
intensive SLT than conventional SLT. There was a similar suggestion (and a consistency in direction) of higher withdrawals from
groups that were receiving social support and stimulation than
SLT interventions but this did not reach significance (P = 0.09).
Unfortunately few trials gave detailed reasons for withdrawals and
so it was not possible to explore these findings further.

Potential biases in the review process


Within this review we refined the original search strategy and conducted a comprehensive search for high quality trials that evalu-

ated the effectiveness of SLT for aphasia following stroke. While


we are confident we have identified most published trials of relevance to the review it is possible, despite our efforts, that we may be
unaware of additional unpublished work. Our search strategy and
study selection criteria were agreed in advance and applied to all
identified trials. Our data extraction processes were completed independently and then compared. Whenever possible we extracted
all relevant data and sought missing data directly from the trialists
for inclusion within the review. We considered it appropriate to
include cross-over data within our review given the nature of the
comparisons, the points at which the data were extracted and, in
some cases, the availability of individual patient data.
This review has been informed by the availability of individual
patient data (N = 305). In three subcomparisons the individual
data were presented within the associated publications, while for
the remaining nine subcomparisons we are very grateful to the
trialists for the unpublished data thus allowing inclusion within
the review. In addition, other trialists generously contributed the
relevant summary values thus permitting (for the first time) the
full inclusion of important trials from this field (Wertz 1986i;
Wertz 1986ii; Wertz 1986iii) within a systematic review. However, there still remain a number of other trials that could not
be fully included. In some cases the outcome measure summary
data were reported in a format that made it unsuitable for full
inclusion within the reviews meta-analyses. For three additional
trials, outcome measure data were not reported at all (nor were
they available from the authors).

Agreements and disagreements with other


studies or reviews
One of the first reviews in this area was Robey 1994 who reviewed
21 published studies (restricted to English language but not to
randomised controlled trials). They identified at least 19 more
studies that they were unable to include because of the manner
in which the data had been reported. They concluded that the
provision of SLT in the acute stages of aphasia following stroke was
twice as effective as natural recovery patterns. Therapy started after
that acute period had less of an impact but was still evident. They
called for better reporting of data and the use of large sample sizes.
This team later updated this review (Robey 1998a), employing the
same methodologies and included 55 studies looking specifically
at the amount and type of SLT intervention and the impact of
the severity and type of aphasia. Again, they concluded that SLT
was effective, particularly SLT in the acute stages following stroke
and if two or more hours of therapy were provided each week.
However, they again did not have access to all the relevant data
and some key trials such as Wertz 1986 were excluded.
Bhogal 2003 reviewed 10 English language publications of controlled trials from a MEDLINE search (1975 to 2002) and associated references. They found that intensive SLT delivered significant treatment effects (when at least nine hours per week were

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

30

delivered) but that studies that failed to demonstrate a treatment


effect had only provided about two hours of SLT per week. The
total duration of SLT provision was also negatively correlated with
language outcomes. Cherney 2008 also reviewed 10 English language publications (1990 to 2006; 15 electronic databases; not all
RCTs) and found modest evidence for intensive SLT and benefits
of constraint-inducted language therapy.
In contrast, Moss 2006 reviewed 23 single patient reports involving the provision by a therapist on a one-to-one basis of SLT which
targeted spoken output or auditory comprehension in 57 participants identified following a systematic search (1985 to 2003) of
published or indexed work. They concluded that time since stroke
(and aphasia onset) is not linked to the response to SLT though
they indicate (based on their data) that response to SLT may decline eight years after stroke. However, the highly selected nature
of participants in single cases studies means that reviews based on
such a population group are of questionable relevance to a general
clinical population. Individuals (and their carers) within such reports are likely to be highly motivated, educated, dedicated and
reliable participants (Moss 2006).

AUTHORS CONCLUSIONS
Implications for practice
The evidence presented within this review shows some indication
of the effectiveness of SLT for people with aphasia following stroke,
especially in relation to functional communication, expressive language and the severity of aphasia.
We also observed a consistency in the direction of results which
favours intensive SLT over conventional SLT, though significantly
more people withdrew from intensive SLT treatment than conventional SLT.
SLT facilitated by a trained volunteer under the direction of a
therapist appears to be as effective as the provision of SLT from
a professional therapist. This is probably unsurprising as the volunteers receive specialist training, have access to therapy materials
and in many cases are delivering therapy interventions designed
and overseen by a professional therapist. This is a model of treatment often used in therapy in the UK. There was insufficient evidence to draw any conclusions in relation to the effectiveness of
group SLT as compared to conventional one-to-one SLT.
There is some very limited evidence that social support and stimulation may be beneficial to patients receptive and expressive language skills based on a single cross-over trial.
There was insufficient evidence within this review to establish the
effectiveness of one SLT approach over another.

Implications for research


This review presents a synthesis of SLT trial data which shows
some indications of the effectiveness of SLT for people with aphasia following stroke. Results from those additional trials recently
completed and currently ongoing will further contribute to this evidence base. The prevalence of outcome assessor blinding amongst
the trials included in this review is evidence of the quality of research undertaken to date. Future trials in this area should build
upon this by ensuring the use of standardised objective outcome
measures by assessors blinded to the participants background and
intervention. Digitally recording conversational interactions or
test performance for rating by independent assessors is one model
of outcome assessment that should be encouraged.
Some of the limitations of the review findings reflect limitations
in the availability of suitable data for inclusion within the review.
Researchers, funders, reviewers and editors should ensure that the
findings from future trials are fully published. The recommendations of the CONSORT statement (Altman 2001; Moher 2001)
should be adhered to, thus ensuring the quality of the trial is fully
demonstrated. Similarly, trialists should provide full descriptions
of the relevant statistical summary data (means and standard deviations of final value scores) thus allowing inclusion of their data
within relevant meta-analyses.
Future work might consider the more detailed examination of
the effectiveness of SLT as it applies across a range of patients
differing in aphasia profile, the length of time since their stroke
and other factors. It is possible that some SLT approaches may be
more effective for some patient groups (and aphasia profiles) than
others.
We saw some suggestion within the review of the effectiveness
of intensive approaches to SLT when compared to conventional
SLT. We need more data on volunteer-facilitated SLT, group SLT
and functional SLT approaches before we can be confident about
drawing conclusions in relation to their effectiveness. We still need
to establish what is the optimum approach, frequency, duration of
allocation and format of SLT provision for specific patient groups.
Future investigations should also consider contributing to the evidence base as it relates to the effectiveness of a social support and
stimulation intervention in comparison to a SLT intervention.
A priori sample size calculations should be employed thus ensuring SLT trials are adequately powered to demonstrate differences.
The challenge for SLT researchers and clinicians will be to design,
develop, conduct and support larger trials. It is essential for the
success of these trials that the work is undertaken in a collaborative
manner between patients, clinicians and researchers. Standardised
outcome measures should be employed to evaluate the impact of
SLT on participants functional communication, expressive and
receptive language skills and the severity of their aphasia.

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

31

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We would like to acknowledge Jenny Greener and Renata Whurr,
authors of the original review, and the significant contribution the
original review made to the field.
We would like to thank Hazel Fraser for her comments and suggestions for this review, and for providing us with relevant trials from
the Cochrane Stroke Groups Trials Register and Brenda Thomas
for her help with developing the search strategy.

We thank the Cochrane Stroke Group Editors and all those who
commented on the draft review, in particular Peter Langhorne,
Audrey Bowen, Nadina Lincoln, Cameron Sellars and Catherine
Mackenzie.
We are grateful to the Chinese Cochrane Centre, Mrs Christine
Versluis and Dr Audrey Morrison for translations.
We would like to thank all the trialists who responded to our
queries, provided translations and contributed unpublished data
and additional information to this review.

REFERENCES

References to studies included in this review


Bakheit 2007 {published and unpublished data}
Bakheit M, Shaw S, Barrett L, Wood J, Carrington S,
Griffiths S, et al.A prospective, randomized, parallel group,
controlled study of the effect of intensity of speech and
language therapy on early recovery from poststroke aphasia.
Clinical Rehabilitation 2007;21(10):88594.
David 1982 {published and unpublished data}
David R, Enderby P, Bainton D. Response to Huber W,
Poeck K, Springer L, Willmes K. Journal of Neurology,
Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 1983;46:6923.
David R, Enderby P, Bainton D. Response to Marshall RC,
Golper LA. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry
1983;46:68991.
David R, Enderby P, Bainton D. Response to TR Pring.
British Journal of Disorders of Communication 1983;18(2):
737.

David R, Enderby P, Bainton D. Treatment of acquired


aphasia: speech therapists and volunteers compared. Journal
of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 1982;45:95761.
David RM. A comparison of speech therapists and volunteers
in the treatment of acquired aphasia. Unpublished thesis,
University of London 1982.
David RM, Enderby P, Bainton D. Progress report on an
evaluation of speech therapy for aphasia. British Journal of
Disorders of Communication 1979;14(2):858.
Enderby P. Proposed evaluation of speech therapy
for acquired aphasia. British Journal of Disorders of
Communication 1976;11(2):1448.
Denes 1996 {published and unpublished data}
Denes G, Perazzolo C, Piani A, Piccione F. Intensive versus
regular speech therapy in global aphasia: a controlled study.
Aphasiology 1996;10(4):38594.
Di Carlo 1980 {published data only}
Di Carlo L. Language recovery in aphasia: effect of
systematic filmed programed instruction. Archives of
Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 1980;61:414.
Doesborgh 2004a {published and unpublished data}
Doesborgh SJC, van de Sandt-Koenderman MWME,
Dippel DWJ, Koudstaal PJ, Visch-Brink EG. Effects of

semantic treatment on verbal communication and linguistic


processing in aphasia after stroke: a randomized controlled
trial. Stroke 2004;35:1416.
Doesborgh 2004b {published and unpublished data}
Doesborgh SJC, van de Sandt-Koenderman MWME,
Dippel DWJ, van harskamp F, Koudstaal PJ, Visch-Brink
EG. Cues on request: the efficacy of Multicue, a computer
program for wordfinding therapy. Aphasiology 2004;18(3):
21322.
Drummond 1981 {published data only}
Drummond SS, Rentschler GJ. The efficacy of gestural
cueing in dysphasic word-retrieval responses. Journal of
Communication Disorders 1981;14(4):28798.
Elman 1999 {published data only (unpublished sought but not used)}
Elman RJ, Bernstein-Ellis E. The efficacy of group
communication treatment in adults with chronic aphasia.
Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 1999;42
(2):4119.
Hinckley 2001 {published data only}
Hinckley JJ, Patterson JP, Carr TH. Differential effects of
context- and skill-based treatment approaches: preliminary
findings. Aphasiology 2001;15(5):46376.
Jufeng 2005i {published data only}
Jufeng Y, Yuan X, Feng L. Clinical application research on
collective language strengthened training in rehabilitation
nursing of cerebral apoplexy patients with aphasia. Chinese
Nursing Research 2005;19(3B):4824.
Jufeng 2005ii {published data only}

Jufeng Y, Yuan X, Feng L. Clinical application research on


collective language strengthened training in rehabilitation
nursing of cerebral apoplexy patients with aphasia. Chinese
Nursing Research 2005;19(3B):4824.
Jufeng 2005iii {published data only}

Jufeng Y, Yuan X, Feng L. Clinical application research on


collective language strengthened training in rehabilitation
nursing of cerebral apoplexy patients with aphasia. Chinese
Nursing Research 2005;19(3B):4824.

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

32

Katz 1997i {published and unpublished data}


Katz RC, Wertz RT. Computerized hierarchical reading
treatment in aphasia. Aphasiology 1992;6(2):16577.

Katz RC, Wertz RT. The efficacy of computer-provided


reading treatment of chronic aphasic adults. Journal
of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 1997;40(3):
493507.
Katz RC, Wertz RT, Lewis SM, Esparza C, Goldojarb MA.
A comparison of computerized reading treatment, computer
stimulation, and no treatment for aphasia. In: Prescott TE
editor(s). Clinical Aphasiology. Vol. 19, Austin, Texas: ProEd, 1991:24354.
Katz 1997ii {published and unpublished data}
Katz RC, Wertz RT. Computerized hierarchical reading
treatment in aphasia. Aphasiology 1992;6(2):16577.

Katz RC, Wertz RT. The efficacy of computer-provided


reading treatment of chronic aphasic adults. Journal
of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 1997;40(3):
493507.
Katz RC, Wertz RT, Lewis SM, Esparza C, Goldojarb MA.
A comparison of computerized reading treatment, computer
stimulation, and no treatment for aphasia. In: Prescott TE
editor(s). Clinical Aphasiology. Vol. 19, Austin, Texas: ProEd, 1991:24354.
Leal 1993 {published and unpublished data}
Ferro JM, Leal G, Farrajota L, Fonseca J, Guerreiro M,
Castro-Caldas A. Speech therapy or home training for stroke
aphasics?. Journal of Neurology 1992;239 Suppl 3:20.
Leal MG, Farrajota L, Fonseca J, Guerriero M, CastroCaldas A. The influence of speech therapy on the evolution
of stroke aphasia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Neuropsychology 1993;15(3):399.

Leal MG, Farrajota L, Fonseca J, Santos ME, Guerriero


M, Ferro JM, et al.The influence of speech therapy on the
evolution of stroke aphasia. Unpublished report. Language
Research Laboratory, Lisbon, Portugal, 1994.
Lincoln 1982i {published and unpublished data}
Lincoln NB. An investigation of the effectiveness of
language retraining methods with aphasic stroke patients.
PhD thesis 1980.

Lincoln NB, Pickersgill MJ, Hankey AI, Hilton CR. An


evaluation of operant training and speech therapy in the
language rehabilitation of moderate aphasics. Behavioural
Psychotherapy 1982;10(2):16278.
Lincoln 1982ii {published and unpublished data}
Lincoln NB. An investigation of the effectiveness of
language retraining methods with aphasic stroke patients.
PhD thesis 1980.

Lincoln NB, Pickersgill MJ, Hankey AI, Hilton CR. An


evaluation of operant training and speech therapy in the
language rehabilitation of moderate aphasics. Behavioural
Psychotherapy 1982;10(2):16278.
Lincoln 1982iii {published data only}
Lincoln NB. An investigation of the effectiveness of
language retraining methods with aphasic stroke patients.

PhD thesis 1980.

Lincoln NB, Pickersgill MJ, Hankey AI, Hilton CR. An


evaluation of operant training and speech therapy in the
language rehabilitation of moderate aphasics. Behavioural
Psychotherapy 1982;10(2):16278.
Lincoln 1984a {published and unpublished data}
Berman A, Rowntree P, Smith L, Chambers C, Russell R,
Chipperfield E, et al.Speech therapy for the stroke patient.
Lancet 1984; Vol. July 14:104.
Howard D. Speech therapy for aphasic stroke patients.
Lancet 1984; Vol. June 23:14134.
Lendrem W, Lincoln NB. Spontaneous recovery of language
in patients with aphasia between 4 and 34 weeks after
stroke. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry
1985;48:7438.
Lendrem W, McGuirk E, Lincoln N. Factors affecting
language recovery in aphasic stroke patients receiving speech
therapy. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry
1988; Vol. 51:110310.

Lincoln N, Mulley GP, Jones AC, McGuirk E, Lendrem


W, Mitchell JRA. Effectiveness of speech therapy for aphasic
stroke patients. Lancet 1984;1(8388):1197200.
Lincoln NB. Psychological effects of speech therapy.
International Journal of Rehabilitation Research 1985; Vol.
8 Suppl 4:22.
Lincoln NB, Jones AC, Mulley GP. Psychological effects of
speech therapy. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 1985;29
(5):46774.
Lincoln NB, McGuirk E. Speech therapy for the stroke
patient. Lancet 1984; Vol. July 14:104.
Williams J, Wenden F, Jenkins DG. Speech therapy for
aphasic stroke patients. Lancet 1984; Vol. June 23:1413.
Lincoln 1984b {published and unpublished data}
Lincoln NB. An investigation of the effectiveness of
language retraining methods with aphasic stroke patients.
PhD thesis 1980.

Lincoln NB, Pickersgill MJ. The effectiveness of


programmed instruction with operant training in the
language rehabilitation of severely aphasic patients.
Behavioural Psychotherapy 1984;12:23748.
Lyon 1997 {published and unpublished data}
Lyon JG, Cariski D, Keisler L, Rosenbek J, Levine R,
Kumpula J, et al.Communication partners: enhancing
participation in life and communication for adults with
aphasia in natural settings. Aphasiology 1997;11(7):
693708.
MacKay 1988 {published data only}
Mackay S, Holmes DW, Gersumky AT. Methods to assess
aphasic stroke patients. Geriatric Nursing 1988;May/June:
1779.
Meikle 1979 {published data only}
Meikle M, Wechsler E, Tupper A, Benenson M, Butler
J, Mulhall D, et al.Comparative trial of volunteer and
professional treatments of dysphasia after stroke. British
Medical Journal 1979;2(6182):879.

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

33

Meinzer 2007 {published and unpublished data}


Meinzer M, Streiftau S, Rockstroh B. Intensive language
training in the rehabilitation of chronic aphasia - effective
training by laypersons. Journal of the International
Neuropsychological Society 2007;13:84653.
ORLA 2006 {published and unpublished data}
Cherney L. Is more better? Preliminary results from a
computer treatment study for aphasia. In progress.

Cherney LR, Babbitt EM, Cole R, van Vuuren S, Hurwitz


R, Ngampatipatpong M. Computer treatment for aphasia:
efficacy and treatment intensity. Poster presentation 2006
ACRMASNR Joint Educational Conference.
Cole R, Cherney L. ORLA (Oral Reading for Language in
Aphasia with Virtual Therapist). www.bltek.com/virtualteachers/orla.html.
Prins 1989 {published and unpublished data}
Prins RS. Aphasia: classification, treatment and recovery
[Afasie: classificatie, behandeling en herstelverloop].
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Amsterdam
1987.

Prins RS, Schoonen R, Vermuelen J. Efficacy of two


different types of speech therapy for aphasic patients.
Applied Psycholinguistics 1989;10:85123.
Pulvermuller 2001 {published and unpublished data}
Pulvermuller F, Neininger B, Elbert T, Mohr B, Rockstroh
B, Koebbel P. Constraint-induced therapy of chronic
aphasia after stroke. Stroke 2001;32:16216.
Rochon 2005 {published and unpublished data}
Rochon E, Laird L, Bose A, Scofield J. Mapping therapy
for sentence production impairments in nonfluent aphasia.
Neuropsychological Rehabilitation 2005;15(1):136.
Shewan 1984i {published and unpublished data}
Shewan, CM, Bandur, DL. Treatment of Aphasia: A
Language-oriented Approach. San Diego: College-Hill Press,
1986.

Shewan CM, Kertesz A. Effects of speech and language


treatment on recovery from aphasia. Brain and Language
1984;23:27299.
Shewan 1984ii {published and unpublished data}
Shewan CM, Bandur DL. Treatment of Aphasia: A Languageoriented Approach. San Diego: College-Hill Press, 1986.

Shewan CM, Kertesz A. Effects of speech and language


treatment on recovery from aphasia. Brain and Language
1984;23:27299.
Shewan 1984iii {published and unpublished data}
Shewan, CM, Bandur, DL. Treatment of Aphasia: A
Language-oriented Approach. San Diego: College-Hill Press,
1986.

Shewan CM, Kertesz A. Effects of speech and language


treatment on recovery from aphasia. Brain and Language
1984;23:27299.
Smania 2006 {published and unpublished data}
Smania N, Aglioti SM, Girardi F, Tinazzi M, Fiaschi A,
Cosentino A, et al.Rehabilitation of limb apraxia improves
daily life activities in patients with stroke. Neurology 2006;
67:20502.

Smith 1981i {published and unpublished data}

Duffy FR. Speech therapy after stroke: a randomised


controlled trial - an interim report. Demonstration Centres
in Rehabilitation Newsletter, Volume 28, 1982.
Smith DS, Goldenberg E, Ashburn A, Kinsella G, Sheikh
K, Brennan PJ, et al.Remedial therapy after stroke: a
randomised controlled trial. British Medical Journal 1981;
282:51720.
Smith 1981ii {published and unpublished data}

Duffy FR. Speech therapy after stroke: a randomised


controlled trial - an interim report. Demonstration Centres
in Rehabilitation Newsletter, Volume 28, 1982.
Smith DS, Goldenberg E, Ashburn A, Kinsella G, Sheikh
K, Brennan PJ, et al.Remedial therapy after stroke: a
randomised controlled trial. BMJ 1981;282:51720.
Smith 1981iii {published and unpublished data}

Duffy FR. Speech therapy after stroke: a randomised


controlled trial - an interim report. Demonstration Centres
in Rehabilitation Newsletter, Volume 28, 1982.
Smith DS, Goldenberg E, Ashburn A, Kinsella G, Sheikh
K, Brennan PJ, et al.Remedial therapy after stroke: a
randomised controlled trial. BMJ 1981;282:51720.
van Steenbrugge 1981 {published and unpublished data}
van Steenbrugge WJ, Prins RS. Word finding difficulties
and efficacy of systematic language therapy in aphasic
patients. Logopedie en Foniatrie 1981;53:62237.
Wertz 1981 {published and unpublished data}
Wertz R, Collins MJ, Weiss D, Kurtzke JF, Friden T,
Brookshire RH, et al.Veterans administration cooperative
study on aphasia: a comparison of individual and group
treatment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 1981;24:
58094.
Wertz 1986i {published and unpublished data}
Kurtzke JF, Wertz RT, Weiss DG, Garcia-Bunuel L, Aten
JL, Brookshire RH, et al.Comparison of improvement in
neurologic severity and language in treated and untreated
aphasic patients. Neurology 1985;35 Suppl 1:122.

Marshall RC, Wertz RT, Weiss DG, Aten J, Brookshire


RH, Garcia-Bunuel L, et al.Home treatment for aphasic
patients by trained nonprofessionals. Journal of Speech and
Hearing Disorders 1989;54:46270.
Wertz R, Weiss WG, Aten JL, Brookshire RH, GarciaBunuel L, Holland AL, et al.Comparison of clinic, home
and deferred language treatment. Archives of Neurology
1986;43:6538.
Wertz 1986ii {published and unpublished data}
Kurtzke JF, Wertz RT, Weiss DG, Garcia-Bunuel L, Aten
JL, Brookshire RH, et al.Comparison of improvement in
neurologic severity and language in treated and untreated
aphasic patients. Neurology 1985;35 Suppl 1:122.

Marshall RC, Wertz RT, Weiss DG, Aten J, Brookshire


RH, Garcia-Bunuel L, et al.Home treatment for aphasic
patients by trained nonprofessionals. Journal of Speech and
Hearing Disorders 1989;54:46270.
Wertz R, Weiss WG, Aten JL, Brookshire RH, GarciaBunuel L, Holland AL, et al.Comparison of clinic, home

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

34

and deferred language treatment. Archives of Neurology


1986;43:6538.
Wertz 1986iii {published and unpublished data}
Kurtzke JF, Wertz RT, Weiss DG, Garcia-Bunuel L, Aten
JL, Brookshire RH, et al.Comparison of Improvement in
neurologic severity and language in treated and untreated
aphasic patients. Neurology 1985;35 Suppl 1:122.

Marshall RC, Wertz RT, Weiss DG, Aten J, Brookshire


RH, Garcia-Bunuel L, et al.Home treatment for aphasic
patients by trained nonprofessionals. Journal of Speech and
Hearing Disorders 1989;54:46270.
Wertz R, Weiss WG, Aten JL, Brookshire RH, GarciaBunuel L, Holland AL, et al.Comparison of clinic, home
and deferred language treatment. Archives of Neurology
1986;43:6538.
Wu 2004 {published data only}
Wu X. Analysis of the effect of two-step method on aphasia
in patients with acute cerebrovascular disease. Chinese
Journal of Clinical Rehabilitation 2004;8(22):44223.

References to studies excluded from this review

Kalra 1993 {published data only}


Kalra L, Dale P, Crome P. Improving stroke rehabilitation: a
controlled study. Stroke 1993;24(10):14627.
Kinsey 1986 {published data only (unpublished sought but not used)}
Kinsey C. Microcomputer speech therapy for dysphasic
adults: a comparison with two conventionally administered
tasks. British Journal of Disorders of Communication 1986;
21:12533.
Meinzer 2005 {published and unpublished data}
Meinzer M, Djundja D, Barthel G, Elbert T, Rockstroh
B. Long-term stability of improved language functions in
chronic aphasia after constraint-induced aphasia therapy.
Stroke 2005;36:14626.
Rudd 1997 {published data only}
Rudd AG, Wolfe CDA, Tilling K, Beech R. Randomised
controlled trial to evaluate early discharge scheme for
patients with stroke. BMJ 1997;315:103944.
Stoicheff 1960 {published data only}
Stoicheff M. Motivating instructions and language
performance of dysphasic subjects. Journal of Speech and
Hearing Research 1960;3(1):7585.

Cherney 2007 {unpublished data only}


Cherney LR, Small SL. Intensive language therapy for
nonfluent aphasia with and without surgical implantation of
an investigational cortical stimulation device: preliminary
language and imaging results. Clinical Aphasiology
Conference. May 2007.

Wang 2004 {published data only}


Wang D, Lu Y, Xie R, Yao J. Effect of different intensities
of rehabilitation therapy on the prognosis of patients with
stroke. Chinese Journal of Clinical Rehabilitation 2004;8
(22):44101.

Cohen 1992 {published data only}


Cohen NS. The effect of singing instruction on the speech
production of neurologically impaired persons. Journal of
Music Therapy 1992;XXIX(2):87102.

Wolfe 2000 {published data only}


Wolfe CDA, Tilling K, Rudd AG. The effectiveness
of community-based rehabilitation for stroke patients
who remain at home: a pilot randomized trial. Clinical
Rehabilitation 2000;14:5639.

Cohen 1993 {published data only}


Cohen NS, Masse R. The application of singing and
rhythmic instruction as a therapeutic intervention for
persons with neurogenic communication disorders. Journal
of Music Therapy 1993;XXX(2):8199.

Wood 1984 {published data only}


Wood-Dauphinee S, Shapiro S, Bass E, Fletcher C, Georges
P, Hensby V, et al.A randomized trial of team care following
stroke. Stroke 1984;15:86472.

Gu 2003 {published data only}


Gu Y, Wang S, Li S. The method and therapy effect of
the early speech therapy on aphasia. Zhongguo Linchuang
Kangfu 2003;7(3):3823.
Hartman 1987 {published data only}
Hartman J, Landau W. Comparison of formal language
therapy with supportive counselling for aphasia due to acute
vascular accident. Archives of Neurology 1987;44:6469.
Jungblut 2004 {published and unpublished data}
Jungblut M, Aldridge D. Effects of a specific music therapy
approach in the treatment of patients suffering from chronic
nonfluent aphasia. Neurologie und Rehabilitation 2004;10
(2):6978.
Kagan 2001 {published and unpublished data}
Kagan A, Black SE, Duchan JF, Simmons-Mackie N,
Square P. Training volunteers as conversation partners using
supported conversation for adults with aphasia (SCA): a
controlled trial. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing
Research 2001;44(3):62438.

Zhang 2004 {published data only}


Zhang T, Li LL, Bi S, Mei YW, Xie RM, Luo ZM,
et al.Effects of three-stage rehabilitation treatment on
acute cerebrovascular diseases: a prospective randomized
controlled multicenter study. Chinese Medical Journal 2004;
84(23):194854.

References to studies awaiting assessment


Liu 2006 {published data only (unpublished sought but not used)}
Liu, X, Dai, R, Cheng, L. Correlation between the design
of aphasia rehabilitative program and the diseased sites of
cerebrum. Chinese Journal of Clinical Rehabilitation 2006;
10(14):79.

References to ongoing studies


ACTNow {published and unpublished data}
Bowen A. Assessing the effectiveness of communication
therapy in the North West - ACTNoW study. http://
www.controlled-trials.com/mrct/ukctrsearch.html.

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

35

IHCOP {unpublished data only}


Woolf C. The effects of phoneme discrimination and
semantic therapies for speech perception deficits in aphasia.
National Research Register.
Kukkonen 2007 {published and unpublished data}
Kukkonen T, Korpijaakko-Huuhka AM. How much is
enough and when is the right time? What do we know about
the good practice and timing of aphasia rehabilitation?
. British Aphasiology Society Biennial Conference. 1012
September 2007, Edinburgh, UK.

Kukkonen T, Molnr G, Korpijaakko-Huuhka A-M. How


much is enough and when is the right time? Developing
strategies for assessing aphasia rehabilitation. 27th World
Congress of the International Association of Logopedics
and Phoniatrics, Copenhagen, Denmark. 59 August 2007.
Laska 2008 {published and unpublished data}
Laska AC, Kahan T, Hellblom A, Murray V, von Arbin
M. Design and methods of a randomised controlled trial
on early speech and language therapy in patients with acute
stroke and aphasia. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation 2008;15
(3):25661.
Maher 2008 {unpublished data only}
Gonzalez-Rothi LJ, Wu S. An investigation of constraint
induced language therapy for treatment of aphasia.
Unpublished.
RATS2 {unpublished data only}
Visch-Brink E. Rotterdam Aphasia Therapy Study-2. In
progress.
RATS3 {unpublished data only}
RATS-3. http://www.eso-stroke.org/pdf/dtd2009/
Rotterdam%20University%20Hospital.pdf.
SEATAS {published and unpublished data}
Godecke E, Hird K, Lalor E. Aphasia therapy in the acute
hospital setting: is it justified?. Internal Medicine Journal
2008;38 Suppl 4:A88.
SP-I-RiT {published and unpublished data}
Lauterbach M, Leal G, Aguiar M, Fonseca I, Farrajota L,
Fonseca J, et al.Intensive vs conventional speech therapy in
aphasia due to ischaemic stroke: a randomized controlled
trial. Proceedings of the British Aphasiology Society 2007
Biennial Conference. 1012 September 2007, Edinburgh,
UK:678.
Varley 2005 {published and unpublished data}
Varley R, Windsor F, Whiteside S. Whole word therapy
for acquired apraxia of speech. 35th Clinical Aphasiology
Conference, Sanibel Island, FL. 2005.

Additional references
Altman 2001
Altman DG, Schulz KF, Moher D, Egger M, Davidoff F,
Elbourne D, et al.The revised CONSORT statement for
reporting randomized trials: explanation and elaboration.
Annals of Internal Medicine 2001;134(8):66394.
Bastiaanse 1995
Bastiaanse R, Bosje M, Visch-Brink EG. Dutch adaptation
of Kay J, Lesser R, Coltheart M. Psycholinguistic Assessment

of Language Processing in Aphasia. Hove, UK: Lawrence


Erlbaum Associates Ltd, 1995.
Benson 1996
Benson DF, Ardila A. Aphasia: a clinical perspective. New
York: Oxford University Press, 1996.
Bhogal 2003
Bhogal SK, Teasell R, Speechley M. Intensity of aphasia
therapy, impact on recovery. Stroke 2003;34:98793.
Blomert 1994
Blomert L, Kean ML, Koster C, Schokker J. AmsterdamNijmegen Everyday Language Test: construction, reliability
and validity. Aphasiology 1994;8:381407.
Borkowski 1967
Borkowski JG, Benton AL, Spreen O. Word fluency and
brain damage. Neuropsychologia 1967;5:13540.
Bradburn 1969
Bradburn NM. The Structure of Psychological Well-being.
Chicago IL: Aldine, 1969.
Caplan 1998
Caplan D, Hanna JE. Sentence production by aphasic
patients in a constrained task. Brain and Language 1998;63:
184218.
Castro-Caldas 1979
Castro-Caldas A. Diagnostico e Evolucao das Afasias de Causa
Vascular. Lisboa: Faculdade de Medicina de Lisboa, 1979.
Cherney 2008
Cherney LR, Patterson JP, Raymer A, Frymark T, Schooling
T. Evidence-based systematic review: effects of intensity
of treatment and constraint-induced language therapy for
individuals with stroke-induced aphasia. Journal of Speech,
Language and Hearing Research 2008;51:128299.
Code 2003
Code C, Herrmann M. The relevance of emotional
and psychological factors in aphasia to rehabilitation.
Neuropsychological Rehabilitation 2003;13(1/2):10932.
DeRenzi 1962
DeRenzi E, Vignolo LA. The Token Test: a sensitive test to
detect receptive disturbances in aphasics. Brain 1962;85:
66578.
Engelter 2006
Engelter ST, Gostynski M, M, Papa A, Frei M, Born C,
Ajdacic-Gross V, Gutzwiller F, Lyrer PA. Epidemiology of
Aphasia Attributable to First Ischemic Stroke: Incidence,
Severity, Fluency, Etiology, and Thrombolysis. Stroke 2006;
37:13791384.
Fink 1994
Fink RB, Schwartz MF, Rochon E, Myers JL, Socolof
GS, Bluestone R, et al.Picture description with structure
modelling (PDSM): a procedure for measuring syntactic
generalisation. Poster presented at Academy of Aphasia,
Boston 1994.
Goldberg 1972
Goldberg D. The Detection of Psychiatric Illness by
Questionnaire Mandsley Monograph No 21. London: Oxford
University Press, 1972.

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

36

Goodglass 1972
Goodglass H, Kaplan E. Boston Diagnostic Aphasia
Examination. Philadelphia: Lea and Febiger, 1972.
Goodglass 1983
Goodglass H, Kaplan E. The assessment of aphasia and related
disorders. Philadelphia, PA: Lea and Febiger, 1983.
Greener 2001
Greener J, Enderby P, Whurr R. Pharmacological treatment
for aphasia following stroke. Cochrane Database of Systematic
Reviews 2001, Issue 4. [Art. No.: CD000424. DOI:
10.1002/14651858.CD000424]
Higgins 2008
Higgins JPT, Green S (editors). Cochrane Handbook for
Systematic Reviews of Interventions Version 5.0.0 [updated
February 2008]. The Cochrane Collaboration, 2008.
Available from www.cochrane-handbook.org.
Holland 1980
Holland A. Communicative abilities in daily living.
Baltimore: University Park Press, 1980.
Holland 1998
Holland A, Frattali C, Fromm D. Communication activities
of daily living. 2nd Edition. Austin Texas: Pro-Ed, 1998.
Huber 1984
Huber E, Poeck K, Wilmes K. The Aachen Aphasia Test.
In: Rose FC editor(s). Progress in Aphasiology. New York:
Ravens Press, 1984:291303.
Jufeng 2005
Jufeng Y, Yuan X, Feng L. Clinical application research on
collective language strengthened training in rehabilitation
nursing of cerebral apoplexy patients with aphasia. Chinese
Nursing Research 2005;19(3B):4824.
Kaplan 1983
Kaplan E, Goodglass H, Weintraub S. Boston Naming Test.
Philadelphia, PA: Lea and Febiger, 1983.
Kay 1992
Kay J, Lesser R, Coltheart M. Psycholinguistic Assessments of
Language Processing in Aphasia. Hove: Psychology Press,
1992.
Kertesz 1982
Kertesz A. Western Aphasia Battery. New York: Grune and
Stratton, 1982.
LaPointe 1979
LaPointe LL, Horner J. Reading Comprehension Battery
for Aphasia. USA, 1979. Tigard OR: CC Publications.
Laska 2001
Laska AC, Hellblom A, Murray V, Kahan T, von Arbin M.
Aphasia in acute stroke and relation to outcome. Journal of
Internal Medicine 2001;249:413422.
Lincoln 1979
Lincoln N. An Investigation of the Effectiveness of
Language Retraining Methods with Aphasic Stroke Patients.
University of London, 1979.
Lincoln 1982
Lincoln NB, Pickersgill MJ, Hankey AI, Hilton CR. An
evaluation of operant training and speech therapy in the

language rehabilitation of moderate aphasics. Behavioural


Psychotherapy 1982;10(2):16278.
Lomas 1989
Lomas J, Pickard L, Bester S, Elbard H, Finlayson A,
Zoghaib C. The communicative effectiveness index:
development and psychometric evaluation of a functional
communication measure for adults. Journal of Speech and
Hearing Disorders 1989;54:11324.
Moher 2001
Moher D, Schulz KF, Altman DG. The CONSORT
statement: revised recommendations for improving the
quality of reports of parallel-group randomised trials. Lancet
2001;357:11914.
Moss 2006
Moss A, Nicholas M. Language rehabilitation in chronic
aphasia and time post onset: a review of single-subject data.
Stroke 2006;37:304351.
Oldfield 1965
Oldfield RC, Wingfield A. A series of pictures for use in
object naming. Psycholinguistics research unit, Special
report No. PLU/65/19, Oxford 1965.
Parr 1997
Parr S, Byng S, Gilpin S, Ireland C. Talking about Aphasia:
Living with Loss of Language after Stroke. Buckingham:
OUP, 1997.
Porch 1967
Porch B. Porch Index of Communicative Ability. Austin, TX:
Pro-Ed, 1967.
Porch 1971
Porch BE. Porch Index of Communicative Ability. 2nd
Edition. Palo Alto, California: Consulting Psychologists
Press, 1971.
Porch 1981
Porch BE. Porch Index of Communicative Ability. 3rd
Edition. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press,
1981.
Prins 1980
Prins RS. Psycholinguistic aspects of aphasia diagnosis
and therapy [Psycho Linguistische aspekten van afasie
diagnostiek en therapie]. Gerontologie 1980;11(1):228.
RevMan 2008
The Nordic Cochrane Centre. The Cochrane Collaboration.
Review Manager (RevMan). 5.0. Copenhagen: The Nordic
Cochrane Centre. The Cochrane Collaboration, 2008.
Robey 1994
Robey R. The efficacy of treatment for aphasic persons: a
meta-analysis. Brain and Language 1994;47:582608.
Robey 1998a
Robey R. A meta-analysis of clinical outcomes in the
treatment of aphasia. Journal of Speech, Language and
Hearing Research 1998;41(1):17287.
Saffran 1988
Saffran EM, Schwartz MF, Linebarger M, Martin N,
Bochetto P. The Philadelphia comprehension battery for
aphasia. Unpublished manuscript 1988.

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

37

Sarno 1969
Sarno MT. The Functional Communication Profile:
Manual of Directions. Vol. 42, New York Institution of
Rehabilitation Medicine, 1969.
Schuell 1965
Schuell H. Minnesota Test for Differential Diagnosis of
Aphasia. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press,
1965.
Shewan 1979
Shewan CM. The Auditory Comprehension Test for Sentences
(ACTS). Chicago II: Biolinguistics, 1979.
Shewan 1984
Shewan CM, Kertesz A. Effects of speech and language
treatment on recovery from aphasia. Brain and Language
1984;23:27299.
Soares 2004
Soares HP, Daniels S, Kumar A, Clarke M, Scott C,
Swann S, et al.Bad reporting does not mean bad methods
for randomised trials: observational study of randomised
controlled trials performed by the Radiation Therapy
Oncology Group. BMJ 2004;328:224.
Spreen 1969
Spreen O, Benton A. Neurosensory Center Comprehensive
Examination for Aphasia. Victoria BC: Neuropsychology
Laboratory, University of Victoria, 1969.

Taylor 1959
Taylor M, Marks M. Aphasia Rehabilitation Manual and
Therapy Kit. Aphasia Rehabilitation Manual and Therapy
Kit. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1959.
Vermeulen 1979
Vermeulen J. Psychometrische eigenschappen van de AAT.
Aphasia Center, St. Lucas Ziekenhuis, 1979.
Visch-Brink 1996
Visch-Brink EG, Denes G, Stronks D. Visual and verbal
semantic processing in aphasia. Brain and Language 1996;
55:1302.
Wertz 1986
Wertz R, Weiss WG, Aten JL, Brookshire RH, GarciaBunuel L, Holland AL, et al.Comparison of clinic, home
and deferred language treatment. Archives of Neurology
1986;43:6538.
Zuckerman 1965
Zuckerman M, Lubin B. Manual for the Multiple Affect
Adjective Checklist. San Diego, California: Educational and
Industrial Testing Service, 1965.

References to other published versions of this review


Greener 1999
Greener J, Enderby P, Whurr R. Speech and language
therapy for aphasia following stroke. Cochrane Database of
Systematic Reviews 1999, Issue 4. [Art. No.: CD000425.
DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD000425]

Indicates the major publication for the study

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

38

CHARACTERISTICS OF STUDIES

Characteristics of included studies [ordered by study ID]


Bakheit 2007
Methods

RCT

Participants

Inclusion criteria: first stroke, below normal on WAB, native English speaker, medically
stable, fit for participation
Exclusion criteria: depression, Parkinsons disease, unlikely to survive, severe dysarthria,
more than 15 miles from hospital
Group 1: 51 participants
Group 2: 46 participants
Groups comparable at baseline

Interventions

1. Intensive SLT (1 hour therapy 5 times weekly for 12 weeks)


2. Conventional SLT (1 hour therapy 2 sessions weekly for 12 weeks)
Intensive SLT and conventional SLT: tasks included picture-object selection, object naming, recognition and associations; expression of feelings and opinions; improving conversational skills; gestural and non-verbal communication (including communication aids
and equipment)

Outcomes

Western Aphasia Battery


Assessed at baseline and weeks 4, 8, 12 and 24

Notes

UK
A further NHS Group was not randomised (first 6 consecutive participants allocated
to this group) and were therefore excluded from this review
Drop-outs: 31 participants (Intensive 20; Conventional 11)

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Yes

Random numbers table

Allocation concealment?

Yes

Sequentially numbered sealed envelopes

Blinding?
All outcomes

Yes

Outcome assessors blinded

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

No

ITT analysis not used

Free of selective reporting?

Yes

Statistical data included in the review

Free of other bias?

Unclear

Sample size calculation not reported


Only 13/51 participants in intensive SLT group received 80%
or more of prescribed treatment

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

39

David 1982
Methods

Parallel group RCT

Participants

Inclusion criteria: aphasia, less than 85% on Functional Communication Profile (x 2),
English speaking, at least 3 weeks after stroke
Exclusion criteria: previous SLT, deafness, blindness or confusion preventing participation
Group 1: 65 participants
Group 2: 68 participants
Baseline between group difference: the conventional SLT group were older

Interventions

1. Conventional SLT (30 hours therapy for up to 20 weeks)


2. Social support and stimulation (30 hours contact for up to 20 weeks)
Conventional SLT: therapist directed SLT
Social support and stimulation: untrained volunteers received details about participants
aphasia, general support and within-treatment assessment scores. They were not given
instruction in SLT techniques

Outcomes

Functional Communication Profile, Schuell Assessment


Assessed twice at baseline and at 2, 4, 8, 12 weeks and post-treatment (3 and 6-month
follow ups)

Notes

UK
Randomisation details provided through personal communication with authors of original review
Drop-outs: 82 participants (conventional SLT 34; social support 48)

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Yes

Random numbers table

Allocation concealment?

Yes

Adequate

Blinding?
All outcomes

Yes

Outcome assessor not treating therapist

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

No

ITT analysis was not used

Free of selective reporting?

Unclear

Statistical data included in the review

Free of other bias?

Unclear

Sample size calculation not reported


Participants in the social support and stimulation group were
younger (mean age 65 years; SD 10.6) than those in the conventional SLT group (mean age 70 years; SD 8.7)

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

40

Denes 1996
Methods

Parallel group RCT

Participants

Inclusion criteria: global aphasia, left CVA, within first year after stroke, right-handed,
native Italian speakers, literate
Exclusion criteria: none listed
Group 1: 8 participants
Group 2: 9 participants
Groups comparable at baseline

Interventions

1. Intensive SLT (45 to 60-minute session approximately 5 times weekly for 6 months)
2. Conventional SLT (45 to 60-minute session approximately 3 times weekly for 6
months)
Intensive SLT: conversational approach more focus on comprehension (e.g. picturematching to understanding complex scenes, short stories, engaging patient in conversation, retelling personally relevant stories)
Conventional SLT: based on stimulation approach

Outcomes

Aachen Aphasia Test


Assessed at baseline and 6 months

Notes

Italy
Data from an additional 4 non-randomised participants with global aphasia were also
reported. They received no SLT intervention but were assessed at 6-monthly intervals
and their scores were used to account for spontaneous recovery. They were not included
in this review

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Unclear

Allocation concealment?

Unclear

Blinding?
All outcomes

Yes

Outcome assessors blinded

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

Yes

All randomised participants included in analysis

Free of selective reporting?

Yes

Statistical data included in the review

Free of other bias?

Unclear

Sample size calculation not reported


Groups comparable at baseline

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

41

Di Carlo 1980
Methods

Parallel group RCT

Participants

Inclusion criteria: right-handed, left MCA stroke


Exclusion criteria: none listed
Group 1: 7 participants
Group 2: 7 participants
Groups comparable at baseline

Interventions

1. Conventional SLT with filmed programmed instruction (programme lasted at least


80 hours for between 5 to 22 months)
2. Conventional SLT with non-programmed activity (lasted at least 80 hours for between
6 to 9 months)
Filmed programmed instruction: perceptual, thinking and language training films (designed for population with hearing impairment) based on linguistic learning theory;
passing criterion of 80%, then progression to the next film
Non-programme activity: viewing slides, bibliotherapy

Outcomes

Reading recognition, reading comprehension, visual closure, visual learning, vocabulary


learning
Assessed at baseline, mid-test and at end of treatment

Notes

USA

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Unclear

Allocation concealment?

Unclear

Blinding?
All outcomes

Unclear

Outcome assessor blinding not described

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

Yes

All randomised participants included in analysis

Free of selective reporting?

Yes

Individual patient data reported across all measures

Free of other bias?

Unclear

Sample size calculation not reported


Groups comparable at baseline

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

42

Doesborgh 2004a
Methods

Parallel group RCT

Participants

Inclusion criteria: > 3 months after stroke, experiencing both semantic and phonological
deficits, moderate/severe aphasia
Exclusion criteria: illiterate, non-native speaker, dysarthria, global aphasia, developmental/severe acquired dyslexia, visual perceptual deficit, recovered/no aphasia
Group 1: 29 participants
Group 2: 29 participants
Group 1 older than Group 2

Interventions

1. Semantic treatment SLT (1.5 to 3 hours in 2 to 3 sessions weekly for up to 40 weeks)


2. Phonological treatment SLT (1.5 to 3 hours in 2 to 3 sessions weekly for up to 40
weeks)
Semantic treatment SLT: aimed to enhance semantic processing (multiple choice, right/
wrong format), several levels of difficulty
Phonological treatment SLT: sound structure targeting phonological input and output
routes, e.g. rhyming consonant clusters, stress patterns, compiling words, syllabification,
phonetic similarity

Outcomes

Amsterdam Nijmegen Everyday Language Test Scale A (ANELT-A), Semantic Association Test (SAT), PALPA synonym judgement, PALPA repetition of non-words, PALPA
auditory lexical decision
Assessed at baseline and end of treatment

Notes

The Netherlands
Co-morbidity: memory and executive function impairment
Drop-outs: 12 participants (semantic SLT 6; phonological SLT 6)
A priori sample size calculated

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Yes

Computer-generated

Allocation concealment?

Yes

Sequentially numbered sealed envelopes

Blinding?
All outcomes

Yes

Outcome assessors blinded

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

No

Trialists reported ITT


3 participants not included (ANELT scores missing)
On-treatment analysis used

Free of selective reporting?

Yes

Statistical data included in the review

Free of other bias?

No

Semantic SLT group older than phonological SLT group


Sample size calculation not reported

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

43

Doesborgh 2004b
Methods

RCT

Participants

Inclusion criteria: age 20 to 86 years, native Dutch speaker, minimum 11 months after
stroke with moderate to severe naming deficits
Exclusion criteria: illiterate, global or rest aphasia, developmental dyslexia
Group 1: 9 participants
Group 2: 10 participants
Groups similar at baseline

Interventions

1. Computer-mediated SLT (30 to 45 minutes 2 to 3 sessions weekly for 2 months)


2. No SLT (6 to 8 weeks)
Computer-mediated SLT: improve naming using computer cueing programme

Outcomes

Assessed at baseline and end of treatment


Boston Naming Test, ANELT-A

Notes

The Netherlands
Co-intervention: psychosocial group therapy aimed at coping with consequences of
aphasia, unclear if all participated
Patient confounder: executive function deficits
Drop-outs: 1 participant (computer-mediated SLT 1; no SLT 0)
A priori sample size calculated

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Yes

Computer-generated sequence

Allocation concealment?

Yes

Concealment in sequentially numbered opaque sealed envelopes

Blinding?
All outcomes

No

Trialists were the outcome assessors

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

No

ITT analysis was not used

Free of selective reporting?

Yes

Statistical data included in the review

Free of other bias?

Yes

A priori sample size calculated


Groups similar at baseline

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

44

Drummond 1981
Methods

Parallel group RCT

Participants

Inclusion criteria: none listed


Exclusion criteria: none listed
Group 1: 4 participants
Group 2: 4 participants
Groups similar at baseline

Interventions

1. Gesture Cueing SLT: 15 to 30 minutes daily for 2 weeks


2. Conventional SLT: 15 to 30 minutes daily for 2 weeks
Gestural cueing (AMERIND): signs to facilitate word finding
Conventional SLT: initial syllable and sentence completion cues to facilitate word finding

Outcomes

Picture naming test (20/30 items from the Aphasia Therapy Kit Taylor 1959), response
times
Assessed at baseline and at end of treatment

Notes

USA

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Unclear

Allocation concealment?

Unclear

Blinding?
All outcomes

Unclear

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

Yes

All randomised participants included in analysis

Free of selective reporting?

Unclear

Suitable statistical data permitting inclusion within the review


unavailable

Free of other bias?

Unclear

Inclusion criteria not listed


Groups similar at baseline
Sample size calculation not reported

Elman 1999
Methods

Cross-over group RCT (only data collected prior to cross-over treatment included in this
review)

Participants

Inclusion criteria: > 6 months after stroke, completed SLT available via insurance, single
left hemisphere stroke, 80 years or younger, premorbidly literate in English, no medical
complications or history of alcoholism, 10th to 90th overall percentile on SPICA on
entry, attend more than 80% of therapy

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

45

Elman 1999

(Continued)

Exclusion criteria: multiple brain lesions, diagnosed alcoholism


Group 1: 12 participants
Group 2: 12 participants
Groups comparable at baseline (age, education level, aphasia severity)
Interventions

1. Conventional SLT: 2.5 hour session twice weekly for 4 months


2. Social support and stimulation: at least 3 hours weekly for 4 months)
Conventional SLT: improve ability to convey message using any verbal/non-verbal methods in group format, social breaks for communication practice, performance artist (1
hour weekly) to facilitate physical exercises, creative expression
Social support and stimulation: participants attended social group activities of their
choice, e.g. church groups

Outcomes

Shortened Porch Index of Communicative Ability, Western Aphasia Battery AQ, Communicative Activities in Daily Living
Assessed at baseline, 2 and 4 months and 4 to 6 weeks from end of treatment

Notes

USA
Drop-outs: 7 participants (conventional SLT 3; social support and stimulation 4)

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Unclear

Allocation concealment?

Unclear

Blinding?
All outcomes

No

Outcome assessor inadequately blinded

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

No

ITT analysis was not used

Free of selective reporting?

Unclear

Statistical data reported unsuitable for inclusion within the review

Free of other bias?

Unclear

Groups comparable at baseline (age, education level, aphasia severity)


Sample size calculation not reported

Hinckley 2001
Methods

Parallel group RCT

Participants

Inclusion criteria: single left hemisphere stroke, native English speaker, minimum 3
months after stroke, hearing and vision corrected to normal, minimum high school
education, chronic non-fluent aphasia

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

46

Hinckley 2001

(Continued)

Exclusion criteria: none listed


Group 1: 6 participants
Group 2: 6 participants
Groups comparable at baseline (age, time post-onset, aphasia severity, education, occupation)
Interventions

1. Functional SLT: 20 hours weekly for 5 weeks


2. Conventional SLT: 20 hours weekly for 5 weeks
Functional SLT: disability based, context trained, role plays of functional tasks, establish compensatory strategies (practise ordering by telephone, self-generate individualised
strategies)
Conventional SLT: impairment based, skill trained, aimed at remediating deficit areas
using cueing hierarchies

Outcomes

CADL-2, CETI (completed by primary carer), phone and written functional task developed for project (catalogue ordering quiet and tone), PALPA oral and written picture
naming
Assessed at baseline and end of treatment

Notes

USA
5 additional participants were non-randomly assigned to a baseline group (both functional SLT and conventional SLT) but they were excluded from this review
In the functional SLT group, therapy was discontinued when performance on training
probes (50% trained items) reached a minimum of 90% accuracy for 3 consecutive
sessions
All SLTs were trained in 2 treatment approaches

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Unclear

Allocation concealment?

Unclear

Blinding?
All outcomes

Unclear

Outcome assessor not reported

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

Yes

All randomised participants included in analyses

Free of selective reporting?

Yes

Statistical data included within the review

Free of other bias?

Unclear

Groups comparable at baseline (age, time post-onset, aphasia


severity, education, occupation)
Sample size calculation not reported

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

47

Jufeng 2005i
Methods

Parallel group RCT

Participants

Inclusion criteria: post-stroke aphasia


Exclusion criteria: none listed
Group 1: 30 participants
Group 2: 30 participants
Comparability of groups at baseline unclear

Interventions

1. Group SLT: daily for 28 days


2. No SLT
Group SLT: participants talk with a doctor/nurse in small groups (10 participants)
Participants encouraged to communicate with each other

Outcomes

Chinese Rehabilitation Research Centre Aphasia Examination (CRRCAE)


Assessed at baseline, 28 days and 3-month follow up

Notes

China
Translated by Chinese Cochrane Centre

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Unclear

Allocation concealment?

Unclear

Blinding?
All outcomes

Yes

Outcome assessor blinded

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

Yes

All randomised participants included in analyses

Free of selective reporting?

Yes

Statistical data included within the review

Free of other bias?

Unclear

Comparability of groups at baseline unclear


Limited inclusion criteria listed and no exclusion criteria
Sample size calculation not reported

Jufeng 2005ii
Methods

Parallel group RCT

Participants

Inclusion criteria: post-stroke aphasia


Exclusion criteria: none listed
Group 1: 24 participants
Group 2: 30 participants
Comparability of groups at baseline unclear

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

48

Jufeng 2005ii

(Continued)

Interventions

1. Conventional SLT: daily for 28 days


2. No SLT
Conventional SLT: 1-to-1 rehabilitative training, i.e. 1 nurse talked with 1 participant

Outcomes

Chinese Rehabilitation Research Centre Aphasia Examination (CRRCAE)


Assessed at baseline, 28 days and 3-month follow up

Notes

China
Translated by Chinese Cochrane Centre

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Unclear

Allocation concealment?

Unclear

B - Unclear

Blinding?
All outcomes

Yes

Outcome assessor blinded

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

Yes

All randomised participants included in analyses

Free of selective reporting?

Yes

Statistical data included within the review

Free of other bias?

Unclear

Comparability of groups at baseline unclear


Limited inclusion criteria listed and no exclusion criteria
Sample size calculation not reported

Jufeng 2005iii
Methods

Parallel group RCT

Participants

Inclusion criteria: aphasia following stroke


Exclusion criteria: none listed
Group 1: 30 participants
Group 2: 24 participants
Comparability of groups at baseline unclear

Interventions

1. Group SLT: daily for 28 days


2. Conventional SLT: daily for 28 days
Group SLT: participants talk with a doctor/nurse in small groups (10 participants)
Participants encouraged to communicate with each other
Conventional SLT: 1-to-1 rehabilitative training, i.e. 1 nurse talked with 1 participant

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

49

Jufeng 2005iii

(Continued)

Outcomes

Chinese Rehabilitation Research Centre Aphasia Examination (CRRCAE)


Assessed at baseline, 28 days and 3-month follow up

Notes

China
Translated by Chinese Cochrane Centre

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Unclear

Allocation concealment?

Unclear

Blinding?
All outcomes

Yes

Outcome assessor blinded

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

Yes

All randomised participants included in analyses

Free of selective reporting?

Yes

Statistical data included within the review

Free of other bias?

Unclear

Comparability of groups at baseline unclear


Limited inclusion criteria listed and no exclusion criteria
Sample size calculation not reported

Katz 1997i
Methods

Parallel group RCT

Participants

Inclusion criteria: single left hemisphere stroke, maximum 85 years, minimum 1 year
after stroke, PICA overall between 15th to 90th percentile, premorbidly right handed,
minimum education 8th grade, premorbidly literate in English, vision no worse than
20/100 corrected in better eye, hearing no worse than 40 dB unaided in better ear, no
language treatment 3 months before entry to study, non-institutionalised living environment
Exclusion criteria: premorbid psychiatric, reading or writing problems
Group 1: 21 participants
Group 2: 21 participants
Groups were comparable at baseline

Interventions

1. Computer-mediated SLT: 3 hours weekly for 26 weeks


2. No SLT
Computer-mediated SLT: computerised language tasks using visual matching and reading comprehension software
No SLT: no computer-based reading intervention or stimulation

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

50

Katz 1997i

(Continued)

Outcomes

Porch Index of Communicative Ability, Western Aphasia Battery Aphasia Quotient


Assessed at baseline, 13 and 26 weeks

Notes

USA
Drop-outs: 6 participants (computer-mediated SLT 0, no SLT 6)
Across 6 hospitals, 2 community stroke groups across 5 cities

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Yes

Random numbers table

Allocation concealment?

Unclear

B - Unclear

Blinding?
All outcomes

Yes

Outcomes measured by 1 of 4 SLTs, 95% checked by second


SLT with no knowledge of group allocation

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

No

ITT analysis not used

Free of selective reporting?

Yes

Statistical data included within the review

Free of other bias?

Unclear

Groups were comparable at baseline


Sample size calculation not reported

Katz 1997ii
Methods

Parallel group RCT

Participants

Inclusion criteria: single left hemisphere stroke, maximum 85 years, minimum 1 year
after stroke, PICA overall between 15th to 90th percentile, premorbidly right handed,
minimum education 8th grade, premorbidly literate in English, vision no worse than 20/
100 corrected, hearing no worse than 40 dB unaided, no language treatment 3 months
before entry to study, non-institutionalised living environment
Exclusion criteria: premorbid psychiatric, reading or writing problems
Group 1: 21 participants
Group 2: 21 participants
Groups were comparable at baseline

Interventions

1. Computer-mediated SLT: 3 hours weekly for 26 weeks


2. Computer-based placebo: 3 hours weekly for 26 weeks
Computer-mediated SLT: computerised language tasks using visual matching and reading comprehension software
Computer-based placebo: computerised cognitive rehabilitation software and arcadestyle games, no language stimulation

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

51

Katz 1997ii

(Continued)

Outcomes

Porch Index of Communicative Ability, Western Aphasia Battery Aphasia Quotient


Assessed at baseline, 13 and 26 weeks

Notes

USA
Drop-outs: 2 participants (computer-mediated SLT 0; no SLT/computer-based placebo
2)
Across 6 hospitals, 2 community stroke groups across 5 cities

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Yes

Random numbers table

Allocation concealment?

Unclear

B - Unclear

Blinding?
All outcomes

Yes

Outcomes measured by 1 of 4 SLTs, 95% checked by 2nd SLT


with no knowledge of group allocation

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

No

ITT analysis not used

Free of selective reporting?

Yes

Statistical data included within the review

Free of other bias?

Unclear

Groups were comparable at baseline


Sample size calculation not reported

Leal 1993
Methods

Parallel group RCT (stratified by aphasia type)

Participants

Inclusion criteria: no history of neurologic or psychiatric disease, first left stroke (single)
, first month after stroke, moderate-severe aphasia, good health, maximum 70 years,
residing near hospital with flexible transport
Exclusion criteria: mild aphasia (i.e. Aphasia Quotient above 80% on Test Battery for
Aphasia)
Group 1: 59 participants
Group 2: 35 participants

Interventions

1. Conventional SLT: 3 sessions weekly for 6 months


2. Volunteer-facilitated SLT: unclear
Conventional SLT: conventional hospital-based SLT rehabilitation programme
Volunteer-facilitated SLT: speech and language therapist provided relatives with information and working material; they were encouraged to stimulate the patient as much as
possible; monitored monthly by therapist

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

52

Leal 1993

(Continued)

Outcomes

Test Battery for Aphasia created by trialists (reported to have good correlation with
Western Aphasia Battery)
Assessed at baseline and 6 months post stroke

Notes

Portugal
Drop-outs: 34 participants (conventional SLT 21; volunteer-facilitated SLT 13)

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Unclear

Allocation concealment?

Unclear

Blinding?
All outcomes

Yes

Outcome assessor not therapist

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

No

ITT analysis not used

Free of selective reporting?

Unclear

Statistical data reported in a manner unsuitable for inclusion within the review

Free of other bias?

Unclear

Groups were comparable at baseline. Sample size calculation not reported

Lincoln 1982i
Methods

Cross-over RCT (data extracted after completion of cross-over treatment)

Participants

Inclusion criteria: moderate aphasia after stroke, no previous history of brain damage,
to attend for a minimum of eight weeks, PICA overall between 35th to 65th percentile
Exclusion criteria: severely or mildly aphasic
Group 1: 6 participants
Group 2: 6 participants

Interventions

1. Conventional SLT followed by operant training SLT (30-minute session 4 times


weekly for 4 weeks followed by another 4 weeks with cross-over intervention
2. Conventional SLT followed by social support and stimulation (30-minute session 4
times weekly for 4 weeks followed by another 4 weeks with cross-over intervention
Social support and stimulation: pre-determined topics of conversation, participant initiates as able, direct questioning/verbal encouragement given, no attempts to correct
responses
Conventional SLT: automatic and serial speech, picture-word/sentence matching, reading, writing, verbal encouragement
Operant training: verbal conditioning procedure (reinforcement, tokens for correct responses, incorrect responses ignored)

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

53

Lincoln 1982i

(Continued)

Outcomes

Porch Index of Communicative Ability, Token Test (shortened), object naming test,
word fluency naming tasks, picture description, self-rating abilities
Assessed at baseline and end of treatment

Notes

UK
Some participants unable to complete full number of sessions (leaving slightly early,
insufficient therapist time, holidays occurring during trial)
Drop-outs: 13 participants (group allocation unclear)

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Yes

Random numbers table

Allocation concealment?

No

Partial: participants recruited by speech


and language therapists then assigned to intervention by trialist

Blinding?
All outcomes

Yes

Outcome assessors blinded

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

No

ITT analysis not used

Free of selective reporting?

Yes

Statistical data included within the review

Free of other bias?

Yes

Groups were comparable at baseline


Sample size calculation not reported

Lincoln 1982ii
Methods

Cross-over RCT (data extracted after completion of cross-over treatment)

Participants

Inclusion criteria: moderate aphasia after stroke, no previous history of brain damage,
to attend for a minimum of eight weeks, PICA overall between 35th to 65th percentile
Exclusion criteria: severely or mildly aphasic
Group 1: 6 participants
Group 2: 6 participants

Interventions

1. Operant training SLT followed by conventional SLT: 30-minute session 4 times weekly
for 4 weeks followed by another 4 weeks with cross-over intervention
2. Social support and stimulation followed by conventional SLT: 30-minute session 4
times weekly for 4 weeks followed by another 4 weeks with cross-over intervention
Social support and stimulation: pre-determined topics of conversation, participant initiates as able, direct questioning/verbal encouragement given, no attempts to correct
responses
Conventional SLT: automatic and serial speech, picture-word/sentence matching, read-

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

54

Lincoln 1982ii

(Continued)

ing, writing, verbal encouragement


Operant training: verbal conditioning procedure (reinforcement, tokens for correct responses, incorrect responses ignored)
Outcomes

Porch Index of Communicative Ability, Token Test (shortened), object naming test,
word fluency naming tasks, picture description, self-rating abilities
Assessed at baseline and end of treatment

Notes

UK
Some participants unable to complete full number of sessions (leaving slightly early,
insufficient therapist time, holidays occurring during trial)
Drop-outs: 13 participants (group allocation unclear)

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Yes

Random numbers table

Allocation concealment?

No

Partial: participants recruited by speech


and language therapists then assigned to intervention by trialist

Blinding?
All outcomes

Yes

Outcome assessors blinded

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

Unclear

ITT analysis not used

Free of selective reporting?

Yes

Statistical data included within the review

Free of other bias?

Unclear

Groups were comparable at baseline


Sample size calculation not reported

Lincoln 1982iii
Methods

Cross-over RCT (data extracted up to point of cross-over)

Participants

Inclusion criteria: moderate aphasia after stroke, no previous history of brain damage,
to attend for a minimum of 8 weeks, PICA overall between 35th to 65th percentile
Exclusion criteria: severely or mildly aphasic
Group 1: 12 participants
Group 2: 6 participants

Interventions

1. Conventional SLT: 30-minute session 4 times weekly for 4 weeks (before cross-over)
2. Social support and stimulation: 30-minute session 4 times weekly for 4 weeks (before
cross-over)
Social support and stimulation: pre-determined topics of conversation, participant ini-

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

55

Lincoln 1982iii

(Continued)

tiates as able, direct questioning/verbal encouragement given, no attempts to correct


responses
Conventional SLT: automatic and serial speech, picture-word/sentence matching, reading, writing, verbal encouragement
Outcomes

Porch Index of Communicative Ability, Token Test (shortened), object naming test,
word fluency naming tasks, picture description, self-rating abilities
Assessed at baseline and end of treatment

Notes

UK
Some participants unable to complete full number of sessions (leaving slightly early,
insufficient therapist time, holidays occurring during trial)
Drop-outs: 13 participants (group allocation unclear)

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Yes

Random numbers table

Allocation concealment?

No

Partial: participants recruited by speech


and language therapists then assigned to intervention by trialist

Blinding?
All outcomes

Yes

Outcome assessors blinded

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

Unclear

ITT analysis not used

Free of selective reporting?

Yes

Statistical data included within the review

Free of other bias?

Unclear

Groups were comparable at baseline


Sample size calculation not reported

Lincoln 1984a
Methods

Parallel group RCT

Participants

Inclusion criteria: acute stroke, admitted to Nottingham hospital


Exclusion criteria: unable to tolerate full language testing at 10 weeks, very mild aphasia,
severe dysarthria
Group 1: 163 participants
Group 2: 164 participants
Data reported: 191 participants
Groups comparable at baseline (age, gender, aphasia types)

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

56

Lincoln 1984a

(Continued)

Interventions

1. Conventional SLT: 1-hour session 2 times weekly for 24 weeks


2. No SLT (deferred SLT)
Conventional SLT: as chosen by each SLT

Outcomes

Porch Index of Communicative Ability, Functional Communication Profile


Secondary outcome: Multiple Adjective Affect Checklist
Assessed at baseline, 12 weeks and at end of treatment at 24 weeks

Notes

UK
Method of randomisation and concealed allocation provided through personal communication with authors of original review
Other hospital treatment given as normal
Not all patients received planned number of sessions mainly due to recovery or withdrawal from treatment
Drop-outs: 166 participants (conventional SLT 76; no SLT 90)

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Yes

Random numbers table

Allocation concealment?

Yes

Sequentially numbered sealed envelopes

Blinding?
All outcomes

Yes

Outcome assessors blinded

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

No

ITT analysis not used

Free of selective reporting?

Yes

Statistical data reported unsuitable for inclusion within the review

Free of other bias?

Unclear

Groups were comparable at baseline


Sample size calculation not reported

Lincoln 1984b
Methods

Cross-over RCT (only data collected prior to cross-over treatment included in this review)

Participants

Inclusion criteria: < 35th percentile of Porch Index of Communicative Ability, severe
aphasia following stroke, spontaneous speech (few single words), writing limited to
copying, poor auditory comprehension, < average non-verbal intellectual functioning
Exclusion criteria: none listed
Group 1: 6 participants
Group 2: 6 participants

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

57

Lincoln 1984b

(Continued)

Interventions

1. Programmed instruction with operant training plus conventional SLT: 30 minute


session twice weekly for 4 weeks, followed by cross-over
2. Attention placebo plus conventional SLT: 30-minute session twice weekly for 4 weeks,
followed by cross-over
Programmed instruction with operant training: electric board graded language tasks,
board lights in response to correct answer plus therapist provides verbal praise; for incorrect answers, there is no light response, the therapist shakes head and provides verbal
feedback - no
Attention placebo: non-verbal tasks (matching, copying, recall of designs, performance
scale of WAIS, manual dexterity tasks)
Conventional SLT: as provided by qualified speech and language therapist

Outcomes

Porch Index of Communicative Ability, Token Test, Peabody PCT, object naming test
Assessed at baseline, 4 weeks then 8 weeks following cross-over

Notes

UK
The same therapist provided conventional SLT to both groups
Manner of reporting prevents inclusion of data within the meta-analyses
Comparisons between group 1 and group 2 showed group 2 performed significantly
better on PICA test (reading cards) and copying shapes than group 1

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Yes

Random numbers table

Allocation concealment?

No

Partial: participants recruited by speech


and language therapists then assigned to intervention by trialist

Blinding?
All outcomes

Unclear

Outcome assessor blinded for one measure


only (PICA)

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

Yes

All randomised participants included in


analyses

Free of selective reporting?

Yes

Statistical data included within the review

Free of other bias?

Unclear

Groups comparable at baseline


Sample size calculation not reported

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

58

Lyon 1997
Methods

Parallel group RCT

Participants

Inclusion criteria (patient): minimum 1 year after stroke, no bilateral brain damage,
ability to ambulate short distances, function independently in primary ADL, English
primary language, normal range of cognition, hearing and vision, weekly contact with
primary caregiver, history free of psychosis
Inclusion criteria (caregiver): normal cognitive, hearing and vision, no history of psychiatric problems
Exclusion criteria: none reported
Group 1: 18 participants (7 triads)
Group 2: 9 participants (3 triads)
Each triad comprised 1 person with aphasia, 1 caregiver, 1 communication partner
Comparability of groups at baseline unclear

Interventions

1. Functional SLT: Phase A: 1 to 1.5 hours twice weekly for 6 weeks; Phase B: 1 to 2hour session (clinic) plus 2 to 4-hour session (community) once weekly for 14 weeks
2. No SLT intervention
Functional SLT: Phase A: clinic-based, establishing effective means of communication
between person with aphasia and communication partner, maximise pairs communication strategies; Phase B: home or community-based, activities chosen by person with
aphasia

Outcomes

Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination, Communicative Abilities of Daily Living, Affect Balance Scale, Psychological Wellbeing Index, Communication Readiness and Use
Index, informal subjective measures
Assessed at baseline and post-treatment

Notes

USA

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Unclear

Allocation concealment?

Unclear

Blinding?
All outcomes

No

Outcome assessors inadequately blinded

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

Unclear

All randomised participants appear to have been included in


analyses but it is unclear

Free of selective reporting?

No

Statistical data reported unsuitable for inclusion within the review

Free of other bias?

Unclear

Comparability of groups at baseline unclear. Sample size calculation not reported

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

59

MacKay 1988
Methods

Parallel group RCT

Participants

Inclusion criteria: minimum age 30 years, post-stroke aphasia, minimum 6 months postonset, living within 50 mile radius of hospital/specified geographical area
Exclusion criteria: none listed
96 participants in total: division between groups unclear
Unclear whether groups were comparable at baseline

Interventions

1. Volunteer-facilitated SLT: 3 to 6 hours once weekly for 1 year


2. No SLT
Volunteer-facilitated SLT: language and social stimulation

Outcomes

Communicative Abilities of Daily Living, trialist assessment measuring social/interpersonal skills, structured questionnaires assessing economic, medical and demographic factors (completed by carers/family members)
Assessed at baseline, 6, 12, 18 and 24 months

Notes

USA
Participants continued individual medical/nursing care
Drop-outs: 1 (no SLT group 1)

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Unclear

Allocation concealment?

Unclear

Blinding?
All outcomes

Yes

Outcome assessors blinded

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

No

ITT analysis was not used

Free of selective reporting?

No

Data reported unsuitable for inclusion within the review

Free of other bias?

Unclear

Comparability of groups at baseline unclear


Sample size calculation not reported

Meikle 1979
Methods

Parallel group RCT

Participants

Inclusion criteria: aphasia after stroke, minimum 3 weeks after stroke


Exclusion criteria: none listed
Group 1: 15 participants
Group 2: 16 participants

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

60

Meikle 1979

(Continued)

Group that received conventional SLT had more weeks in the trial than the volunteerfacilitated SLT group
Interventions

1. Volunteer-facilitated SLT: 4 home visits weekly plus group sessions for a mean of 20.8
(13.5) (range 2 to 46) weeks
2. Conventional SLT: 45-minute session 3 to 5 times weekly plus group sessions for a
mean of 37.13 (21.89) (range 7 to 84) weeks
Volunteer-facilitated SLT: volunteers given basic background to aphasia, standard items
of SLT equipment, initial and ongoing support and advice, encouraged to use initiative
and ingenuity in developing therapeutic techniques
Conventional SLT: chosen by SLT (no details)

Outcomes

Porch Index of Communicative Ability


Assessed at baseline and at 6-week intervals until end of trial
Wolfson Test (unpublished) (comprehension, verbal expression, writing, spelling)
Assessed at baseline, after 3 months and at end of treatment

Notes

UK
In the conventional SLT group 5 participants missed up to half their possible treatments
(illness, holidays, transport difficulties)
Unclear whether volunteer supervisor was a speech and language therapist
Participants remained in trial until 2 successful estimations on PICA showed no appreciable improvement, they requested withdrawal or until end of trial in December 1978
Participants who plateaued exited trial and counted as successes
Drop-outs: 2 (conventional SLT 0; volunteer-facilitated SLT 2)

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Unclear

Allocation concealment?

Unclear

Blinding?
All outcomes

No

Outcome assessor not blinded

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

No

ITT analysis was not used

Free of selective reporting?

Yes

Statistical data included within the review

Free of other bias?

Unclear

Group that received conventional SLT had more weeks in the


trial than the volunteer-facilitated SLT group
In the conventional SLT group 5 participants missed up to half
their possible treatments (illness, holidays, transport difficulties)
Sample size calculation not reported

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

61

Meinzer 2007
Methods

Parallel group RCT

Participants

Inclusion criteria: 1 or more participating relative, single left hemisphere stroke, aphasia,
minimum 6 months post-onset, globally aphasic if residual expressive language, i.e. repeat
short phrases
Exclusion criteria: none listed
Group 1: 10 participants (4 subgroups)
Group 2: 10 participants (4 subgroups)
Participants receiving constraint-induced SLT were younger than those in the volunteerfacilitated group

Interventions

1. Constraint-induced SLT: 3 hours daily for 10 consecutive working days


2. Volunteer-facilitated constraint-induced SLT: 3 hours daily for 10 consecutive working
days
Constraint-induced SLT: communicative language games, pairs of cards depicting objects, everyday situations or words; screens between the participants prevents seeing each
others cards; participant must choose a card from their own set and ask for the identical card from another participant; can be adjusted to target different levels of language
complexity
Volunteer-facilitated constraint-induced SLT: relatives volunteered to receive 2-hour introduction to constraint-induced SLT; they were supervised during first 2 of 10 sessions
by experienced therapist; following 8 sessions experts were available, further group training sessions at end of each daily training session; where 2 or more relatives were available
they alternated each day

Outcomes

Aachen Aphasia Test (Token Test, repetition, written language, naming, comprehension)
Assessed at baseline and immediately post-treatment

Notes

Germany
One participant in each group had mild apraxia of speech

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Unclear

Allocation concealment?

Unclear

Blinding?
All outcomes

Yes

Outcome assessor blinded

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

Yes

All randomised participants included in analyses

Free of selective reporting?

Yes

Statistical data included within the review

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

62

Meinzer 2007

(Continued)

Free of other bias?

Unclear

Participants receiving constraint-induced SLT were younger


than those in the trained volunteers group
Sample size calculation not reported

ORLA 2006
Methods

RCT

Participants

Inclusion criteria: right-handed, non-fluent aphasia, single left ischaemic stroke at least
6 months post-onset
Exclusion criteria: none listed
Group 1: 6 participants
Group 2: 7 participants
Groups seem to be comparable

Interventions

1. Intensive SLT: 10 hours weekly for 6 weeks


2. Conventional SLT: 4 hours weekly for 6 weeks
In both interventions patients used a computer programme which allows patient to
practise reading sentences aloud together with a virtual therapist
A non-randomised third group that acted as a control group was also included in the
study report but was excluded from this review

Outcomes

Western Aphasia Battery Aphasia Quotient

Notes
Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Unclear

Allocation concealment?

Unclear

Blinding?
All outcomes

Unclear

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

Yes

All randomised participants included in analyses

Free of selective reporting?

Yes

Statistical data included within the review

Free of other bias?

Unclear

Groups seem to be comparable at baseline


Sample size calculation not reported

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

63

Prins 1989
Methods

Parallel group RCT

Participants

Inclusion criteria: unilateral left CVA, minimum 3 months post-onset, < 80% on auditory
comprehension test, good prognosis for auditory comprehension per SLT, motivated and
fit for participation
Exclusion criteria: none listed
Group 1: 10 participants
Group 2: 11 participants

Interventions

1. STACDAP SLT: 2 sessions weekly for 5 months


2. Conventional SLT: 2 sessions weekly for 5 months
STACDAP SLT: a series of 28 tasks; non-verbal, phonology, lexical-semantics and morphosyntax of increasing complexity
Conventional SLT: conventional stimulation therapy

Outcomes

Word discrimination, body-part identification, Token Test, miscellaneous commands,


reading comprehension, naming, sentence construction, spontaneous speech, STACDAP phonology, lexicon and morphosyntax
Assessed at baseline and at the end of treatment

Notes

The Netherlands
Participants in additional no treatment group were not randomly allocated but matched
to other groups, and were therefore excluded from the review

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Unclear

Allocation concealment?

Unclear

Blinding?
All outcomes

Unclear

Outcome assessor blinding not reported

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

Yes

All randomised participants included in analyses

Free of selective reporting?

Yes

Statistical data included within the review

Free of other bias?

Unclear

STACDAP SLT group were older than the conventional SLT


group at baseline
Sample size calculation not reported

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

64

Pulvermuller 2001
Methods

Parallel group RCT

Participants

Inclusion criteria: single left MCA stroke, monolingual, competent German speakers
Exclusion criteria: severe cognitive or perceptual difficulties affecting participation, left
handed, additional neurological diseases, depression
Group 1: 10 participants
Group 2: 7 participants
Constraint-induced SLT group were longer since stroke (mean 98.2 (74.2) months) than
conventional SLT group (mean 24 (20.6) months)

Interventions

1. Constraint-induced SLT: 3 to 4 hours daily for 10 days


2. Conventional SLT: 2 to 3 hours daily for approximately 4 weeks
Constraint-induced SLT: small groups (2 to 3 participants) with speech and language
therapist involving barrier therapeutic games; all communication verbal, pointing or
gestures not permitted
Conventional SLT: syndrome-specific intervention for example naming, repetition, sentence completion, following instructions, conversation topics of participants own choice

Outcomes

Aachen Aphasia Test (Token Test, comprehension, repetition, naming), Communicative


Activity Log
Assessed at baseline and at end of treatment

Notes

Germany

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Yes

Computer-generated

Allocation concealment?

Yes

Blinding?
All outcomes

Yes

Outcome assessor blinded

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

Yes

All randomised participants included in analyses

Free of selective reporting?

Yes

Statistical data included within the review

Free of other bias?

No

Constraint-induced SLT group were longer after stroke (mean


98.2 (74.2) months) than conventional SLT Group (mean 24
(20.6) months) at baseline
Sample size calculation not reported

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

65

Rochon 2005
Methods

Parallel group RCT

Participants

Inclusion criteria: chronic Brocas aphasia (BDAE), produce sufficient speech for analyses,
single left hemisphere stroke, native English speaker, normal hearing on screening
Exclusion criteria: none listed
Group 1: 3 participants
Group 2: 2 participants
Groups comparable at baseline

Interventions

1. Sentence mapping SLT: 1 hour session twice weekly for approximately 2.5 months
2. Social support and stimulation: 1 hour session twice weekly for approximately 2.5
months
Sentence mapping SLT: 4 levels of treatment: active, subject cleft, passive, object cleft
sentences
Social support and stimulation: unstructured conversation about current events; participants were given a narrative retelling task on alternate sessions

Outcomes

Trained sentence structures: (1) active, (2) subject cleft, (3) passive, (4) object cleft; Caplan and Hanna Sentence Production Test; Picture Description and Structure Modeling
Test; narrative task: (1) mean length of utterance, (2) percentage words in sentences, (3)
percentage well formed words, (4) sentence elaboration index; Philadelphia Comprehension Battery (reversible sentences); Picture Comprehension Test
Assessed at baseline, end of treatment and 4-week follow up
Social support and stimulation group also participated in between level probes

Notes

Canada
Only 1 group 1 participant entered all 4 levels; 1 only entered levels 1 and 2 (did not
need levels 3 to 4); 1 participant entered levels 1, 2 and 4

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Unclear

Allocation concealment?

Unclear

Blinding?
All outcomes

No

Outcome assessor blinding inadequate


Primary examiner scored all outcome measures
A fifth of measures were also scored by independent assessor
Point-to-point agreement was 98%

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

Yes

All randomised participants included in analyses

Free of selective reporting?

Unclear

Statistical data included within the review

Free of other bias?

Unclear

Sample size calculation not reported

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

66

Shewan 1984i
Methods

Parallel group RCT (stratified for type and severity of aphasia)

Participants

Inclusion criteria: unilateral first CVA, Global, Brocas, Wernickes, anomic, conduction
per WAB, occlusive/stable intracerebral haemorrhagic stroke, functional English speakers
Exclusion criteria: non-stroke, symptoms lasting fewer than 5 days, language recovery
within 2 to 4 weeks post-onset, unstable illness, arteriovenous malfunction, aneurysm
rupture, subarachnoid haemorrhage, hearing or visual impairment, WAB aphasia quotient at or above 93.8
Group 1: 28 participants
Group 2: 24 participants
Groups comparable at baseline

Interventions

1. Language-orientated SLT: 1 hour session 3 times weekly* for 1 year


2. Conventional SLT: 1 hour session 3 times weekly* for 1 year
*(or 1.5 hours twice weekly)
Language-orientated SLT: based on psycholinguistic principles provided by speech and
language therapists
Conventional SLT: stimulation-facilitation therapy based on Schuell and Wepmans approaches provided by speech and language therapists

Outcomes

Western Aphasia Battery, Auditory Comprehension Test for Sentences


Assessed at baseline, 3, 6 and 12 months

Notes

Canada
Participants refusing or unable to participate were allocated to a third no-treatment
group. This group were not included in this review
Drop-outs: 7 participants (language-orientated SLT 6; conventional SLT 1)

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Unclear

Allocation concealment?

Unclear

Blinding?
All outcomes

Unclear

Outcome assessor blinding unclear

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

No

ITT analysis not used

Free of selective reporting?

Unclear

Data reported unsuitable for inclusion


within the review

Free of other bias?

Unclear

Sample size calculation not reported


Groups comparable at baseline

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

67

Shewan 1984ii
Methods

Parallel group RCT (stratified for type and severity of aphasia)

Participants

Inclusion criteria: unilateral first CVA, Global, Brocas, Wernickes, anomic, conduction
per WAB, occlusive/stable intracerebral haemorrhagic stroke, functional English speakers
Exclusion criteria: non-stroke, symptoms lasting fewer than 5 days, language recovery
within 2 to 4 weeks post-onset, unstable illness
Group 1: 28 participants
Group 2: 25 participants
Groups comparable at baseline

Interventions

1. Language-orientated SLT: 1-hour session 3 times weekly* for 1 year


2. Social stimulation and support: 1-hour session 3 times weekly* for 1 year
*(or 1.5 hours twice weekly)
Language-orientated SLT: based on psycholinguistic principles provided by speech and
language therapists
Social stimulation and support: based on stimulation orientation, providing psychological support, communication in unstructured settings carried out by nurses

Outcomes

Western Aphasia Battery, Auditory Comprehension Test for Sentences


Assessed at baseline, 3, 6 and 12 months

Notes

Canada
Participants refusing or unable to participate were allocated to a third no-treatment group
but were not included in this review
Drop-outs: 12 participants (language-orientated SLT 6; social stimulation and support
6)

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Unclear

Allocation concealment?

Unclear

Blinding?
All outcomes

Unclear

Outcome assessor blinding unclear

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

No

ITT analysis not used

Free of selective reporting?

Unclear

Data reported unsuitable for inclusion


within the review

Free of other bias?

Unclear

Sample size calculation not reported


Groups comparable at baseline

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

68

Shewan 1984iii
Methods

Parallel group RCT (stratified for type and severity of aphasia)

Participants

Inclusion criteria: unilateral first stroke, Global, Brocas, Wernickes, anomic, conduction
as per WAB, occlusive or stable intracerebral haemorrhagic stroke, functional English
speakers
Exclusion criteria: non-stroke, symptoms lasting fewer than 5 days, language recovery
within 2 to 4 weeks after stroke, unstable illness
Group 1: 24 participants
Group 2: 25 participants
Groups comparable at baseline

Interventions

1. Conventional SLT: 1 hour 3 times weekly for 1 year (or 1.5 hours twice weekly)
2. Social stimulation and support: 1 hour 3 times weekly for 1 year (or 1.5 hours twice
weekly)
Conventional SLT: stimulation-facilitation therapy based on Schuell and Wepmans approaches provided by speech and language therapists
Social stimulation and support: based on stimulation orientation, providing psychological support, communication in unstructured settings carried out by nurses

Outcomes

Western Aphasia Battery, Auditory Comprehension Test for Sentences


Assessed at baseline, 3, 6 and 12 months

Notes

Canada
Participants refusing or unable to participate were allocated to a third no-treatment group
but were not included in this review
Drop-outs: 7 participants (conventional SLT 1; social stimulation and support 6)

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Unclear

Allocation concealment?

Unclear

Blinding?
All outcomes

Unclear

Outcome assessor blinding unclear

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

No

ITT analysis not used

Free of selective reporting?

Unclear

Data reported unsuitable for inclusion


within the review

Free of other bias?

Unclear

Sample size calculation not reported.


Groups comparable at baseline

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

69

Smania 2006
Methods

Parallel group RCT

Participants

Inclusion criteria: left unilateral CVA, limb apraxia lasting a minimum of 2 months,
aphasia
Exclusion criteria: previous CVA or other neurological disorders, > 80 years of age, uncooperative, orthopedic or other disabling disorders
Group 1: 20 participants
Group 2: 21 participants
Groups comparable at baseline

Interventions

1. Conventional SLT: 50 minutes 3 times weekly for 10 weeks


2. No SLT: limb apraxia therapy over 10 weeks
Conventional SLT: based on Basso et al 1979 approach
No SLT: limb apraxia therapy only

Outcomes

Token Test, Gestural comprehension (not described)


Assessed at baseline, end of treatment and 2-month follow up

Notes

Italy
All participants had apraxia
Drop-outs: 24 participants (conventional SLT 12; no SLT 12)

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Yes

Random numbers table

Allocation concealment?

No

Co-ordinating trialist allocated participants to groups

Blinding?
All outcomes

Yes

Outcome assessor blinded

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

No

ITT was not used

Free of selective reporting?

Unclear

Statistical data included within the review

Free of other bias?

Unclear

Sample size calculation not reported


Groups comparable at baseline

Smith 1981i
Methods

Parallel group RCT (subgroup within larger trial)

Participants

Inclusion criteria: hospital catchment area, measurable residual neurological deficit, no


life threatening concurrent illness, fit for intensive therapy, independent prior to stroke,
inpatient for not more than 2 months after stroke

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

70

Smith 1981i

(Continued)

Exclusion criteria: too old or frail to travel to hospital, some non-described reasons
Group 1: 16 participants
Group 2: 17 participants
Group 1 (intensive SLT) had higher mean percentage error scores on MTDDA than
group 2 (no SLT)
Interventions

1. Intensive SLT: 1 hour 4 times weekly for up to 12 months


2. No SLT
Intensive SLT: not described
No SLT: participants were visited at home by health visitor but frequency is unclear

Outcomes

Minnesota Test for the Differential Diagnosis of Aphasia, General Health Questionnaire
Assessed at baseline, 3, 6 and 12 months after trial admission

Notes

UK
Difficult to maintain intensive SLT input after first 3 months
Participants were also receiving physiotherapy and occupational therapy
No restrictions on other treatments prescribed by hospital staff or GP
Drop-outs: 10 plus ? (5 participants withdrawn prior to final analyses (3 with dysarthria
but no aphasia; 2 died before first re-assessment but grouping not advised) plus intensive
SLT 10; no SLT: none reported

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Unclear

Allocation concealment?

Unclear

Blinding?
All outcomes

No

Outcome assessors not blinded

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

No

ITT analysis not used

Free of selective reporting?

Unclear

Statistical data reported unsuitable for inclusion within the review

Free of other bias?

Unclear

20 patients in main trial had mild dementia, unclear whether any were participants
with aphasia
Group 1 (intensive SLT) had lower mean
percentage error scores on MTDDA than
group 2 (no SLT); it is unclear whether this
was a significant difference
Sample size calculation not reported

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

71

Smith 1981ii
Methods

Parallel group RCT (subgroup within larger trial)

Participants

Inclusion criteria: lives in hospital catchment area, measurable residual neurological


deficit, no life-threatening concurrent illness, fit for intensive therapy if assigned, independent prior to stroke, inpatient for not more than 2 months post-onset
Exclusion criteria: too old or frail to travel to hospital, some non-described reasons
Group 1: 14 participants
Group 2: 17 participants
Group 1 (conventional SLT) had higher mean percentage error scores on MTDDA than
group 2 (no SLT)

Interventions

1. Conventional SLT: 40 minutes twice weekly for up to 12 months


2. No SLT
Conventional SLT: not described
No SLT: participants were visited at home by health visitor but frequency is unclear

Outcomes

Minnesota Test for the Differential Diagnosis of Aphasia, General Health Questionnaire
Assessed at baseline, 3, 6 and 12 months after trial admission

Notes

UK
Participants also receiving physiotherapy and occupational therapy
No restrictions of other treatments prescribed by the hospital or GP
Drop-outs: 5 participants withdrawn prior to final analyses (3 with dysarthria but no
aphasia; 2 died before first re-assessment but grouping not advised) plus 6 participants
(conventional SLT 6; no SLT: none reported)

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Unclear

Allocation concealment?

Unclear

Blinding?
All outcomes

No

Outcome assessors not blinded

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

No

ITT analysis not used

Free of selective reporting?

Unclear

Statistical data reported unsuitable for inclusion within the review

Free of other bias?

Unclear

20 patients in main trial had mild dementia, unclear whether any were participants
with aphasia
Group 1 (conventional SLT) had higher
mean percentage error scores on MTDDA
than group 2 (no SLT)

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

72

Smith 1981ii

(Continued)

Sample size calculation not reported

Smith 1981iii
Methods

Parallel group RCT (subgroup within larger trial)

Participants

Inclusion criteria: lives in hospital catchment area, measurable residual neurological


deficit, no life-threatening concurrent illness, fit for intensive therapy if assigned, independent prior to stroke, inpatient for not more than 2 months post-onset
Exclusion criteria: too old or frail to travel to hospital, some non-described reasons
Group 1: 16 participants
Group 2: 14 participants
Groups comparable at baseline

Interventions

1. Intensive SLT: 1 hour 4 times weekly for up to 12 months


2. Conventional SLT: 40 minutes twice weekly for up to 12 months
Intensive SLT: not described
Conventional SLT: not described

Outcomes

Minnesota Test for the Differential Diagnosis of Aphasia, General Health Questionnaire
Assessed at baseline, 3, 6 and 12 months after trial admission

Notes

UK
Distinction between intensive and conventional became impossible to maintain after
first 3 months as individual patterns of therapy attendance emerged; in first 3 months
mean 21/50 hours intended
Conventional SLT group received additional group treatment; also received physiotherapy and occupational therapy
No restrictions of other treatments prescribed by the hospital or GP
Drop-outs: 5 participants withdrawn prior to final analyses (3 with dysarthria but no
aphasia; 2 died before first re-assessment but grouping not advised) plus 16 participants
(intensive SLT 10; conventional SLT 6)

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Unclear

Allocation concealment?

Unclear

Blinding?
All outcomes

No

Outcome assessors not blinded

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

No

ITT analysis not used

Free of selective reporting?

Unclear

Statistical data reported unsuitable for inclusion within the review

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

73

Smith 1981iii

(Continued)

Free of other bias?

Unclear

20 patients in main trial had mild dementia, unclear whether any were participants
with aphasia
Sample size calculation not reported

van Steenbrugge 1981


Methods

Parallel group RCT

Participants

Inclusion criteria: neurologically stable, > 3 months after stroke, aphasia, motivated, clear
but not too severe naming difficulties
Exclusion criteria: none listed
Group 1: 5 participants
Group 2: 5 participants
Groups comparable at baseline

Interventions

1. Task-specific SLT: 1 hour twice weekly for 6 weeks (followed by 3 weeks free therapy
from patients own therapists)
2. Conventional SLT: unclear but continued for 9 weeks
Task-specific SLT: for naming and constructing sentences: Phase 1 delivered by research
speech and language therapists, Phase 2 delivered by participants own therapist
Conventional SLT: expressive tasks (no details)

Outcomes

FE-Scale (expression), naming (test not specified), sentence construction (not described)
Assessed at baseline, 6 and follow up at 9 weeks

Notes

The Netherlands
Translated by Mrs Christine Versluis (Netherlands)

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Unclear

Allocation concealment?

Unclear

Blinding?
All outcomes

Unclear

Outcome assessor blinding unclear

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

Yes

All randomised participants included in analyses

Free of selective reporting?

Unclear

Statistical data included within the review

Free of other bias?

Unclear

Groups comparable at baseline (age, time post-stroke)


Sample size calculation not reported

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

74

Wertz 1981
Methods

Parallel group RCT

Participants

Inclusion criteria: male veteran, aged 40 to 80 years old, premorbidly literate in English,
first thromboembolic left CVA, no co-existing major medical complications, hearing no
worse than 40 dB in poorer ear, corrected vision no worse than 20/100 in poorer eye,
adequate sensory/motor ability in 1 hand to write/gesture, 4 weeks post-onset, language
severity 15th to 75th overall percentile on PICA
Exclusion criteria: none listed
Group 1: 32 participants
Group 2: 35 participants
Groups comparable at baseline

Interventions

1. Group SLT: 4 hours in group with therapist plus 4 hours of group activities weekly
for up to 44 weeks
2. Conventional SLT: 4 hours with therapist plus 4 hours machine-assisted treatment
and SLT drills weekly for up to 44 weeks
Group SLT: each week, 4 hours direct SLT contact in groups of 3 to 7 participants
designed to stimulate language through social interaction; no direct manipulation of
deficits; encouraged group discussion on current events and topics; no direct attempts
to improve or correct incorrect responses; in addition, 4 hours of group recreational
activities weekly
Conventional SLT: direct, stimulus-response manipulation of speech and language
deficits plus 4 hours of machine-assisted treatment and SLT drill

Outcomes

Porch Index of Communicative Ability, Token Test, word fluency measure, Conversational Rating, Informants ratings of functional language use
Assessed at baseline and every 11 weeks until end of 44-week treatment or withdrawal
of participant

Notes

USA over 5 sites


Drop-outs: 33 participants (group SLT 16; conventional SLT 17)

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Unclear

Allocation concealment?

Unclear

Blinding?
All outcomes

Yes

Outcome assessors blinded

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

No

ITT analysis not used

Free of selective reporting?

Unclear

Some statistical data included within the review

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

75

Wertz 1981

(Continued)

Free of other bias?

Unclear

Groups comparable at baseline


Sample size calculation not reported

Wertz 1986i
Methods

Cross-over group RCT (only data collected prior to cross-over treatment included in this
review)

Participants

Inclusion criteria: male veteran, maximum 75 years old, 2 to 24 weeks post-onset, single
left thromboembolic CVA, no previous or co-existing neurologic, serious medical or
psychological disorder, no worse than 20/100 corrected vision in better eye, hearing no
worse than 40 dB unaided in better ear, sensory/motor ability in 1 upper limb to gesture
or write, premorbidly literate in English, maximum 2 weeks between onset and trial entry,
language severity 10th to 80th PICA overall, non-institutionalised living environment,
outside assistant volunteer available
Exclusion: none listed
Group 1: 38 participants
Group 2: 40 participants
Groups comparable at baseline

Interventions

1. Conventional SLT: 8 to10 hours weekly for 12 weeks


2. No SLT: deferred SLT for 12 weeks
Conventional SLT: delivered by therapist in clinic; stimulus-response (auditory comprehension, reading, oral-expressive language and writing); aphasia-specific techniques;
followed by 12 weeks of no SLT

Outcomes

Porch Index of Communicative Ability, Communicative Abilities in Daily Living, Reading Comprehension Battery for Aphasia, Token Test
Assessed at baseline, 6 and 12 weeks with follow ups at 18 and 24 weeks

Notes

USA over 5 sites


Estimated sample size
Drop-outs: 20 participants (conventional SLT 9; no SLT 11)

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Unclear

Allocation concealment?

Unclear

Blinding?
All outcomes

Yes

Outcome assessors blinded

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

No

ITT analysis not used

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

76

Wertz 1986i

(Continued)

Free of selective reporting?

Yes

Statistical data included within the review

Free of other bias?

Yes

Groups comparable at baseline

Wertz 1986ii
Methods

Cross-over group RCT (only data collected prior to cross-over treatment included in this
review)

Participants

Inclusion criteria: male veteran, maximum 75 years old, 2 to 24 weeks post-onset, single left thromboembolic CVA, no previous neurologic involvement/co-existing serious
medical or psychological disorder, no worse than 20/100 corrected vision in better eye,
hearing no worse than 40 dB unaided in better ear, sensory/motor ability in 1 upper
limb to gesture/write, premorbidly literate in English, maximum 2 weeks between onset
and trial entry, language severity 10th to 80th PICA overall, non-institutionalised living
environment, outside assistant volunteer available
Exclusion: none listed
Group 1: 43 participants
Group 2: 40 participants
Groups comparable at baseline

Interventions

1. Volunteer-facilitated SLT: 8 to 10 hours weekly for 12 weeks


2. No SLT: deferred conventional SLT
Volunteer-facilitated SLT: planned and directed by speech and language therapist, administered at home by trained volunteer (family member/friend) with no previous healthcare
experience, followed by 12 weeks of no SLT
Volunteers received 6 to10 hours training, information about aphasia, observation of
treatment on videotapes, demonstration and practise with techniques; weekly face-toface and telephone contact with SLT for advice and support; every 2 weeks volunteers
videotaped a session to be reviewed with SLT and adjustments suggested

Outcomes

Porch Index of Communicative Ability, Communicative Abilities in Daily Living, Reading Comprehension Battery for Aphasia, Token Test
Assessed at baseline, 6 and 12 weeks with follow ups at 18 and 24 weeks

Notes

USA over 5 sites


Estimated sample size
Drop-outs: 18 participants (trained volunteer SLT 7; no SLT 11)

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Unclear

Allocation concealment?

Unclear

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

77

Wertz 1986ii

(Continued)

Blinding?
All outcomes

Yes

Outcome assessors blinded

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

No

ITT analysis was not used

Free of selective reporting?

Unclear

Statistical data included within the review

Free of other bias?

Unclear

Groups comparable at baseline

Wertz 1986iii
Methods

Cross-over group RCT (only data collected prior to cross-over treatment included in this
review)

Participants

Inclusion criteria: male veteran, maximum 75 years old, 2 to 24 weeks after single left
thromboembolic stroke, no previous neurologic involvement/co-existing serious medical
or psychological disorder, at least 20/100 corrected vision, hearing at least 40 dB unaided,
sensory/motor ability in 1 upper limb to gesture or write, premorbidly literate in English,
maximum 2 weeks between onset and trial entry, language severity 10th to 80th percentile
on PICA, non-institutionalised living, volunteer available
Exclusion: none listed
Group 1: 43 participants
Group 2: 38 participants
Groups comparable at baseline

Interventions

1. Volunteer-facilitated SLT: 8 to 10 hours weekly for 12 weeks


2. Conventional SLT: 8 to 10 hours weekly for 12 weeks
Volunteer-facilitated SLT: prepared by speech and language therapist; administered at
home by trained volunteer (family member/friend) with no previous healthcare experience; followed by 12 weeks of no SLT
Volunteers received 6 to 10 hours training, information about aphasia, observation of
treatment on videotapes, demonstration and practise with techniques; weekly face-toface and telephone contact with SLT for advice and support; every 2 weeks volunteers
videotaped a session to be reviewed with SLT and adjustments suggested
Conventional SLT: delivered by therapist in clinic; stimulus-response (auditory comprehension, reading, oral-expressive language and writing); aphasia-specific techniques;
followed by 12 weeks of no SLT

Outcomes

Porch Index of Communicative Ability, Communicative Abilities in Daily Living, Reading Comprehension Battery for Aphasia, Token Test
Assessed at baseline, 6 and 12 weeks with follow ups at 18 and 24 weeks

Notes

USA over 5 sites


Estimated sample size
Drop-outs: 16 participants (Volunteer-facilitated SLT 9; conventional SLT 7)

Risk of bias
Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)
Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

78

Wertz 1986iii

(Continued)

Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Unclear

Allocation concealment?

Unclear

Blinding?
All outcomes

Yes

Outcome assessors blinded

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

No

ITT analysis not used

Free of selective reporting?

Unclear

Statistical data included within the review

Free of other bias?

Unclear

Groups comparable at baseline

Wu 2004
Methods

Parallel group RCT

Participants

Inclusion criteria: none described


Exclusion criteria: none described
Group 1: 120 participants
Group 2: 116 participants
Unclear whether groups were comparable at baseline

Interventions

1. Conventional SLT: frequency of therapy unclear; for 6 months


2. No SLT
Conventional SLT: 2-part intervention including visual stimulation, gesture and word
pattern for comprehension, pronunciation, reading single words and entertainments
(not described) Part 1: inpatient intervention (doctors); Part 2: outpatient intervention
(family members trained by doctors)

Outcomes

None available

Notes

China
Translated by Chinese Cochrane Centre

Risk of bias
Item

Authors judgement

Description

Adequate sequence generation?

Unclear

Allocation concealment?

Unclear

Blinding?
All outcomes

Yes

Outcome assessor blinded

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

79

Wu 2004

(Continued)

Incomplete outcome data addressed?


All outcomes

Yes

All randomised participants included in analyses

Free of selective reporting?

No

Statistical data not reported

Free of other bias?

Unclear

Unclear whether groups were comparable at baseline


Sample size calculation not reported

ADL: activities of daily living


ANELT: Amsterdam-Nijmegen Everyday Language Test
BDAE: Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination
CADL: communicative abilities of daily living
CETI: Communicative Effectiveness Index
CVA: cerebrovascular accident
dB: decibels
GP: general practitioner
ITT: intention-to-treat
MCA: middle cerebral artery
MTDDA: Minnesota Test for the Differential Diagnosis of Aphasia
NHS: National Health Service (UK)
PALPA: psycholinguistic assessments of language processing in aphasia
PICA: Porch Index of Communicative Abilities
RCT: randomised controlled trial
SD: standard deviation
SLT: speech and language therapy/therapist
SPICA: Shortened Porch Index of Communicative Abilities
STACDAP: systematic therapy for auditory comprehension disorders in aphasic patients

Characteristics of excluded studies [ordered by study ID]

Study

Reason for exclusion

Cherney 2007

Experimental and control groups had same SLT intervention with experimental group also receiving cortical
stimulation

Cohen 1992

Included conditions other than stroke


Unable to obtain aphasia-specific data

Cohen 1993

Included conditions other than stroke


Unable to obtain aphasia-specific data

Gu 2003

Unable to obtain aphasia-specific data

Hartman 1987

Quasi -randomisation

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

80

(Continued)

Jungblut 2004

Randomisation to groups not adequate: group allocation could be predicted

Kagan 2001

Quasi-randomisation

Kalra 1993

Not all participants had aphasia


Unable to obtain aphasia-specific data

Kinsey 1986

Randomisation dictated order of task presentation


Aimed to establish impact of task delivery on performance
Not a therapeutic intervention

Meinzer 2005

Randomisation to groups inadequate; group allocation could be predicted

Rudd 1997

Unable to obtain aphasia-specific data

Stoicheff 1960

Included conditions other than stroke


Unable to obtain aphasia-specific data

Wang 2004

Not all participants had aphasia


Unable to obtain aphasia-specific data

Wolfe 2000

Unable to obtain aphasia-specific data

Wood 1984

Included conditions other than stroke


Unable to obtain aphasia-specific data

Zhang 2004

Unable to obtain aphasic-specific data

SLT: speech and language therapy

Characteristics of studies awaiting assessment [ordered by study ID]


Liu 2006
Methods

Aphasia rehabilitation programme, 30 to 60 minutes daily or every other day

Participants

Interventions

1. SLT aphasia rehabilitation


2. Control

Outcomes

Unclear
Follow-up measures at 3 months

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

81

Liu 2006

(Continued)

Notes

SLT: speech and language therapy

Characteristics of ongoing studies [ordered by study ID]


ACTNow
Trial name or title

ACTNow: Assessing the effectiveness of Communication Therapy in the North west

Methods

Pragmatic, multi-centre RCT with a nested qualitative study and full economic evaluation

Participants

170 (revised from 330)

Interventions

1. Early SLT from NHS therapists; up to 3 sessions per week for maximum of 16 weeks
2. Control group: similar level of contact with a visitor (paid part-time staff ) trained to deliver a manualised
attention control

Outcomes

Primary outcome: functional communication; expert blinded therapist rating of semi-structured conversation
using Therapy Outcomes Measures Scale (TOMS)
Secondary outcome: participant and carers own perception of functional communication and quality of life
Costs of communication therapy compared to that of attention control

Starting date

October 2006

Contact information

Emma Patchick or Audrey Bowen


emma.patchick@manchester.ac.uk or audrey.bowen@manchester.ac.uk
http://www.psych-sci.manchester.ac.uk/actnow/

Notes

Expected completion: 2010

IHCOP
Trial name or title

The effects of phoneme discrimination and semantic therapies for speech perception deficits in aphasia

Methods

Participants

20

Interventions

1. Phoneme discrimination therapy, e.g. discrimination tasks or matching spoken to written words
2. Semantic therapy, e.g. word to picture matching with provided semantic context

Outcomes

Minimal pair discrimination with pictures


Lexical decision
Synonym judgement

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

82

IHCOP

(Continued)

Telephone message task


Control task: written sentence to picture matching
Treated versus untreated words using a cross-modal priming task
Starting date

February 2006

Contact information

Dr Celia Woolf

Notes

Expected completion: 2009

Kukkonen 2007
Trial name or title

Methods

40 participants with aphasia randomised into 4 groups that vary in the intensity of SLT allocated and in the
onset of therapy
Participants have also been stratified by age: younger group (50 to 65 years) and older group (66 to 80 years)
SLT was provided over a 1-year period with periods of therapy sessions and family counselling

Participants

Inclusion criteria: aged 50 to 80 years old, first CVA in the left hemisphere, living locally, diagnosis in university
hospital, diagnosis confirmed by CT/MRI, availability of a relative

Interventions

1. High intensity SLT: 45 minutes 2 times per day, 5 days per week for 6 weeks
2. Moderate intensity SLT: 45 minutes 2 times per day, 2 days per week for 6 weeks
3. Conventional SLT: 45 minutes per week for 6 weeks
4. Control group: no individual SLT
Spouses or carers received support and information from the speech and language therapists 3 times

Outcomes

Speech comprehension (Token Test, BDAE)


Speech production (BDAE and BNT), story telling from cartoon frames
Functional communicative skills (CETI)
Functional Independence Measurement and 15D
Pizzamigglio Sentence Test
Quick Aphasia Screening Test
Montgomery & berg Depression scale and with Beck s Depression scale
Assessments were administered at 1 week, 1, 4, 7 and 13 months post-stroke
Each participants had a 1.5 year follow up

Starting date

Contact information

Tarja Kukkonen
Speech Therapist, Lecturer in Logopedics
Puheopin laitos
33014 Tampereen yliopisto
Finland
Tel. +358 3 35514086
Tarja.Kukkonen@uta.fi

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

83

Kukkonen 2007

(Continued)

Notes

Laska 2008
Trial name or title

Early speech and language therapy in patients with acute stroke and aphasia

Methods

Patients stratified according to NIHSS result


Randomised by use of consecutive sealed envelopes
Outcome measures conducted and assessed by blinded speech and language therapists

Participants

Consecutive admissions to Stroke Unit


Inclusion criteria: first ischaemic stroke with aphasia, can start SLT within 2 days of stroke onset
Exclusion criteria: dementia, drug abuse, unable to participate in therapy

Interventions

1. Early Intensive SLT (language enrichment therapy): 45 minutes per day for 15 working days
2. No SLT for 3 weeks

Outcomes

Primary outcome: ANELT at day 16 Secondary outcome: Norsk Grunntest for Afasi (NGA) at day 16
Other measures include NIHSS, ADL measured at baseline, 3 weeks and 6 months, NGA, ANELT, Nottingham Health Profile (NHP), EuQoL at 3 weeks and 6 months
Relatives complete the CETI at 3 weeks and 6 months

Starting date

Recruitment complete

Contact information

Ann Charlotte Laska


Department of Internal Medicine
Danderyd Hospital
Stockholm
Sweden
Tel: +46 8 655 6409
ann-charlotte.laska@ds.se

Notes

Funded by the Stockholm County Council Foundation (Expo-95), Karolinska Institutet, Marianne and
Marcus Wallenberg Foundation and AFA Insurances
Results expected Autumn 2009

Maher 2008
Trial name or title

An investigation of constraint-induced language therapy for aphasia

Methods

2 different intensities of therapy

Participants

48 participants collected at 3 sites (Houston, Gainesville and Tampa VAMCs)


Inclusion criteria: moderate - moderately severe, non-fluent aphasia, unilateral left CVA, right-handed, English
as first language, adequate hearing and vision to participate in therapy
Exclusion criteria: multiple strokes, history of other neurological impairment, non-English speaking, inadequate auditory comprehension, severe speech apraxia

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

84

Maher 2008

(Continued)

Interventions

1. Intensive CILT
2. Intensive PACE therapy
3. Distributed CILT
4. Distributed PACE therapy

Outcomes

Language assessment, discourse sample, daily probe measures and qualitative interviews will be used to measure
treatment effects
1-month follow up

Starting date

August 2002

Contact information

Lynn M Maher
Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders
University of Houston
lmmaher@uh.edu

Notes

Completion date: June 2006

RATS2
Trial name or title

RATS2: the efficacy of cognitive linguistic therapy in the acute stage of aphasia: a randomised control trial

Methods

Cognitive linguistic SLT versus conventional SLT (from 3 weeks up to 6 months post-onset)

Participants

80

Interventions

1. Cognitive linguistic therapy (paper and computer) using BOX (lexical semantic treatment programme)
and/or FIKS (phonological treatment programme) or a combination of the two depending on individual
language disorders
2. Control group: communicative therapy targeting verbal and nonverbal strategies to communicate message
(e.g. PACE); no focus on semantics, phonology or syntax is permitted

Outcomes

Primary outcome: ANELT-A


Secondary outcome: Verbal Semantic Association Task (SAT), semantic association of words with low imageability (PALPA), non-words repetition (PALPA) and auditory lexical decision (PALPA), semantic word
fluency, letter fluency and Boston Naming Test

Starting date

September 2006

Contact information

Dr EG Visch-Brink
e.visch-brink@erasmusmc.nl
Dr M de Jon-Hagelstein
m.hagelstein@erasmusmc.nl
http://www.controlled-trials.com/ISRCTN67723958

Notes

Results available at the end of 2009

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

85

RATS3
Trial name or title

The efficacy of cognitive linguistic therapy in the acute stage of aphasia: a randomised control trial

Methods

Cognitive linguistic SLT versus no SLT


Massed practice: 2 weeks post-onset up to 2 months post-onset

Participants

80

Interventions

1. Cognitive linguistic therapy: BOX (semantic therapy) or/and FIKS (phonological therapy) for 6 weeks
2. No SLT: deferred SLT after 6 weeks

Outcomes

Primary outcome: ANELT-A


Secondary outcome: Verbal Semantic Association Test (SAT), semantic Association of words with low imageability (PALPA), non-words repetition (PALPA), Auditory Lexical Decision (PALPA), Semantic Word
Fluency, Letter Fluency, Boston Naming Test

Starting date

Autumn 2009

Contact information

EG Visch-Brink e.visch-brink@erasmusmc.nl
M de Jong-Hagelstein m.hagelstein@erasmusmc.nl
http://www.controlled-trials.com/ISRCTN67723958

Notes

Results available at the end of 2012

SEATAS
Trial name or title

Study of Early Aphasia Therapy After Stroke (SEATAS)

Methods

Prospective, randomised, open-label, single-blinded controlled trial

Participants

59 participants with moderate to severe aphasia following stroke

Interventions

1. Intensive daily SLT (32 participants): 30 to 80 minutes 5 days per week up to 4 weeks or 20 sessions
2. Conventional SLT (weekly) (27 participants): 1 session per week up to 4 weeks or 20 sessions
Three therapy types used:
Lexical-sematic (BOX) therapy
Mapping Therapy
Semantic Feature Analysis
All participants had a SLT programme individually tailored to suit their needs and therapists were instructed
to provide treatment from the above therapy types, according the participants needs
The therapist could use only these therapy approaches (one or more)
Therapy types and tasks for each participant were recorded
Picture description task: all participants receiving SLT attempted a picture description task at each session
during the acute hospital stay

Outcomes

Primary outcome measures: Aphasia Quotient (AQ) and Functional Communication Profile (FCP) at acute
hospital discharge
Secondary outcome measures: AQ, FCP and Discourse Analysis (DA) scores at six months post stroke

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

86

SEATAS

(Continued)

Starting date

Recruitment April 2000 to September 2003

Contact information

Erin Godecke
Human Communication Science, School of Psychology and Speech Pathology
Curtin University of Technology, GPO Box U1987 Perth, Australia
Tel: +61 8 9266 3039
e.godecke@curtin.edu.au

Notes

Results available 2010

SP-I-RiT
Trial name or title

SPeech Intensive Rehabilitation Therapy

Methods

Participants

120

Interventions

To evaluate the efficacy of intensive speech therapy in aphasic stroke patients

Outcomes

Primary outcome: increase of the Aphasia Quotient of at least 15% at the end of therapy
Secondary outcome: differences in Aphasia Quotient defined by Lisbon Aphasia Battery
Functional Communication Profile
Sustained improvement in the intensive speech therapy group between 10th and 50th week
Costs of therapy, per therapeutic group
Number of missed therapeutic sessions and non-attendances in each group
Patient satisfaction as measured by patient global impression scale

Starting date

September 2004

Contact information

Dr Martin Lauterbach
email: mlauterbach@fm.ul.pt
http://www.imm.ul.pt

Notes

Expected completion: 2008

Varley 2005
Trial name or title

Methods

Self-administered intervention for word production impairments following stroke

Participants

50 participants with apraxia of speech; 20 participants with non-apraxic word production impairments

Interventions

Both interventions self-administered via software programs loaded onto laptop computer
1. Speech program is based around SWORD, a word-level intervention for apraxia of speech
2. Placebo intervention: does not target speech, but trains visual attention and memory

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

87

Varley 2005

(Continued)

Outcomes

Word production measured across sets of treated, untreated phonetically matched, and untreated phonetically
unmatched words immediately post-treatment and at 8 weeks post-treatment
Word production evaluated for functional adequacy and acoustic measures of speech cohesion
Generalisation to spontaneous speech measured via narrative production
Untreated control behaviours (word reading and spoken sentence comprehension) evaluated
Study also includes health economic assessment

Starting date

June 2008

Contact information

Professor Rosemary Varley, Human Communication


Sciences, University of Sheffield

Notes

Funded by The BUPA Foundation. Expected Completion: October 2010

ADL: activities of daily living


ANELT: Amsterdam-Nijmegen Everyday Language Test
BDAE: Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination
BNT: Boston Naming Test
CETI: Communicative Effectiveness Index
CILT: constraint-induced language therapy
CT: computerised tomography
CVA: cerebrovascular accident
NHS: National Health Service (UK)
NIHSS: National Institutes of Health Stroke Scales
MRI: magnetic resonance imaging
PALPA: psycholinguistic assessments of language processing in aphasia
PACE: promoting aphasics communicative effectiveness therapy
RCT: randomised controlled trial
SLT: speech and language therapy

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

88

DATA AND ANALYSES

Comparison 1. SLT versus no SLT

Outcome or subgroup title


1 Functional communication
1.1 WAB
1.2 ANELT-A
1.3 Functional
Communication Profile
2 Receptive language: auditory
comprehension
2.1 PICA subtest
2.2 Token Test
3 Receptive language: reading
comprehension
3.1 Reading Comprehension
Battery for Aphasia
3.2 PICA reading subtest
4 Receptive language: other
4.1 PICA Gestural subtest
5 Receptive language: gesture
comprehension (unnamed)
5.1 Gesture (unnamed)
5.2 Gesture (unnamed)
2-month follow up
6 Expressive language: naming
6.1 Boston Naming Test
6.2 WAB Naming subtest
7 Expressive language: general
7.1 PICA Verbal subtest
8 Expressive language: written
8.1 PICA Copying subtest
8.2 PICA Writing subtest
8.3 PICA Graphic
9 Expressive language: repetition
9.1 WAB Repetition subtest
10 Severity of impairment:
Aphasia Battery Score (+ PICA)
10.1 Aphasia Quotient
(CRRCAE)
10.2 Porch Index of
Communicative Ability
11 Severity of impairment: Aphasia
Battery Score (3-month follow
up)
11.1 Aphasia Quotient
(CRRCAE) 3-month follow up

No. of
studies

No. of
participants

5
2
1
2

176
55
18
103

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.28 [-0.03, 0.59]


0.14 [-0.40, 0.69]
0.88 [-0.10, 1.87]
0.25 [-0.16, 0.66]

191

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.10 [-0.20, 0.39]

2
3
4

55
136
158

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.15 [-0.40, 0.69]


0.08 [-0.27, 0.43]
0.11 [-0.21, 0.44]

103

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.11 [-0.30, 0.52]

2
4
4
1

55

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.12 [-0.42, 0.67]


Subtotals only
8.04 [1.55, 14.52]
Subtotals only

158

Statistical method

Effect size

1
1

33
17

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

-0.92 [-2.19, 0.35]


-1.14 [-3.23, 0.95]

3
1
2
4
4
4
2
2
2
2
2
6

73
18
55
158
158

55
249

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.20 [-0.27, 0.68]


-0.00 [-0.93, 0.93]
0.27 [-0.27, 0.82]
5.28 [-1.33, 11.89]
5.28 [-1.33, 11.89]
Subtotals only
3.88 [-5.75, 13.50]
8.38 [-4.47, 21.22]
5.15 [-3.64, 13.94]
Subtotals only
0.92 [-0.76, 2.61]
0.18 [-0.09, 0.44]

84

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.02 [-0.43, 0.47]

165

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.26 [-0.07, 0.58]

Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI)

Subtotals only

Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI)

20.74 [-12.01,
53.48]

55
55
103

84

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

89

12 Psychosocial: MAACL
12.1 Anxiety Scale (MAACL)
12.2 Depression Scale
(MAACL)
12.3 Hostility Scale (MAACL)
13 Number of drop-outs (any
reason)

1
1
1

137
137

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

Subtotals only
0.40 [-0.57, 1.37]
0.70 [-1.38, 2.78]

1
10

137
714

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Odds Ratio (M-H, Fixed, 95% CI)

-0.10 [-0.90, 0.70]


0.86 [0.61, 1.20]

Comparison 2. SLT versus social support and stimulation

No. of
studies

No. of
participants

1 Functional communication
1.1 Functional
Communication Profile
1.2 FCP (3-month follow up)
1.3 FCP (6-month follow up)
2 Receptive language: auditory
comprehension
2.1 Sentence Comprehension
Test (PCB)
2.2 Picture Comprehension
Test (PCB)
2.3 Token Test

1
1

96

3 Receptive language: other


3.1 PICA Gestural subtest
4 Expressive language: single words
4.1 Object Naming Test
(ONT)
4.2 Word fluency
5 Expressive language: sentences
5.1 Caplan & Hanna Test:
total
5.2 Caplan & Hanna Test:
treated
5.3 Caplan & Hanna Test:
untreated
6 Expressive language: picture
description
6.1 Picture description
6.2 Picture description with
structure modelling: treated
items
6.3 Picture description with
structure modelling: untreated
items

1
1
1
1

Outcome or subgroup title

Statistical method

Effect size

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

Subtotals only
-2.20 [-10.75, 6.35]

1
1
2

73
73

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

1.40 [-8.01, 10.81]


1.30 [-8.07, 10.67]
Subtotals only

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

6.00 [-12.94, 24.94]

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

8.0 [-10.94, 26.94]

18

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

-3.83 [-18.95,
11.29]
Subtotals only
-0.87 [-1.70, -0.04]
Subtotals only
-5.00 [-11.67, -2.33]
-14.0 [-20.35, -7.65]
Subtotals only
2.0 [-2.73, 6.73]

18
18

1
1
1

18
5

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

3.00 [0.63, 5.37]

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

-1.0 [-5.31, 3.31]

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

Subtotals only

2
2
1

23
5

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.26 [-0.62, 1.15]


0.45 [-1.44, 2.33]

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.41 [-1.46, 2.28]

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

90

7 Expressive language: overall


spoken
7.1 PICA verbal subtest
8 Expressive language: written
8.1 PICA graphic subtests
9 Severity of impairment: Aphasia
Battery Score
9.1 PICA
10 Number of drop-outs for any
reason

1
1
1
1
1

18

1
4

18
260

18

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

Subtotals only

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

-1.56 [-2.46, -0.66]


Subtotals only
-1.39 [-2.49, -0.29]
Subtotals only

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Odds Ratio (M-H, Random, 95% CI)

-1.13 [-1.91, -0.35]


0.65 [0.38, 1.12]

Comparison 3. Functional SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)

Outcome or subgroup title


1 Functional communication
1.1 CADL (change from
baseline)
1.2 CETI
2 Functional communication:
catalogue ordering
2.1 Telephone order (change
from baseline)
2.2 Telephone order (+
concurrent task) (change from
baseline)
2.3 Written order (change
from baseline)
2.4 Written order (+
concurrent task) (change from
baseline)
3 Expressive language: spoken
3.1 Oral naming: PALPA
(change from baseline)
4 Expressive language: written
4.1 Written naming: PALPA
(change from baseline)

No. of
studies
1
1

No. of
participants

12

Statistical method

Effect size

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

Subtotals only
-9.3 [-15.01, -3.59]

1
1

12

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

-3.90 [-8.65, 0.85]


Subtotals only

12

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

32.8 [16.16, 49.44]

12

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

16.9 [1.31, 32.49]

12

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

-5.00 [-19.81, 9.81]

12

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

9.8 [-10.13, 29.73]

1
1

12

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

Subtotals only
6.0 [-8.21, 20.21]

1
1

12

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

Subtotals only
-2.20 [-11.26, 6.86]

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

91

Comparison 4. Intensive SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)

Outcome or subgroup title


1 Receptive language: auditory
comprehension (change from
baseline)
1.1 AAT Comprehension
subtest
1.2 Token Test
2 Expressive language: spoken
(change from baseline scores)
2.1 Naming subtest (AAT)
2.2 Repetition subtest (AAT)
3 Written language: (change from
baseline scores)
3.1 Written subtest (AAT)
4 Severity of impairment: Aphasia
Battery Score
4.1 Aphasia Quotient (WAB)
4.2 AAT profile (change from
baseline)
4.3 Aphasia Quotient (WAB)
3-month follow up
5 Number of drop-outs for any
reason

No. of
studies

No. of
participants

Statistical method

Effect size

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

Subtotals only

17

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

10.3 [-0.52, 21.12]

1
1

17

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

6.2 [-3.32, 15.72]


Subtotals only

1
1
1

17
17

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

5.70 [-1.69, 13.09]


2.80 [-3.86, 9.46]
Subtotals only

1
3

17

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

8.9 [1.81, 15.99]


Subtotals only

2
1

86
17

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

2.00 [-8.21, 12.20]


5.70 [-0.76, 12.16]

66

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

1.90 [-10.53, 14.33]

127

Odds Ratio (M-H, Fixed, 95% CI)

2.10 [0.99, 4.46]

Comparison 5. Volunteer-facilitated SLT (SLTA) versus professional SLT (SLTB)

Outcome or subgroup title


1 Functional communication
1.1 CADL
1.2 Functional
Communication Profile
2 Receptive language: auditory
comprehension
2.1 Token Test
2.2 AAT subtest
3 Receptive language: reading
comprehension
3.1 Reading Comprehension
Battery for Aphasia
4 Receptive language: other
4.1 PICA gestural subtest
5 Expressive language: spoken

No. of
studies

No. of
participants

1
1
1

68
68

Statistical method

Effect size

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

Subtotals only
1.64 [-11.70, 14.98]
2.70 [-7.16, 12.56]

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

Subtotals only

2
1
1

88
20

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.06 [-0.36, 0.47]


-0.37 [-1.25, 0.52]
Subtotals only

68

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.34 [-8.64, 9.32]

1
1
2

68
68

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

-2.54 [-13.18, 8.10]


-2.54 [-13.18, 8.10]
Subtotals only

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

92

5.1 AAT naming subtest


5.2 PICA verbal subtest
6 Expressive language: repetition
6.1 AAT Repetition subtest
7 Expressive language: written
7.1 AAT written language
subtest
7.2 PICA graphic subtests
8 Severity of impairment: Aphasia
Battery Score
8.1 PICA
8.2 AAT
9 Number of drop-outs for any
reason

1
1
1
1
2
1

20
68

20

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

8.40 [-12.68, 29.48]


0.93 [-8.18, 10.04]
Subtotals only
13.5 [0.19, 26.81]
Subtotals only
9.5 [-11.63, 30.63]

1
3

68
126

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

2.22 [-6.90, 11.34]


-0.12 [-0.47, 0.23]

2
1
3

106
20
206

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Odds Ratio (M-H, Random, 95% CI)

-0.06 [-0.44, 0.32]


-0.45 [-1.34, 0.44]
1.05 [0.54, 2.04]

20

Comparison 6. Group SLT (SLTA) versus 1-to-1 SLT (SLTB)

Outcome or subgroup title


1 Receptive language: auditory
comprehension
1.1 Token Test
1.2 AAT comprehension
subtest
2 Receptive language: other
2.1 PICA gestural subtest
3 Expressive language: spoken
3.1 AAT naming subtest
3.2 PICA verbal subtest
4 Expressive language: repetition
4.1 AAT repetition subtest
5 Expressive language: written
5.1 PICA graphic
6 Severity of impairment: Aphasia
Battery Score
6.1 Aphasia Quotient
CRRCAE
6.2 PICA overall
6.3 AAT overall
7 Severity of impairment: Aphasia
Battery Score (3-month follow
up)
7.1 Aphasia Quotient
CRRCAE (3-month follow up)
8 Number of drop-outs for any
reason

No. of
studies

No. of
participants

2
2
1

51
17

Statistical method

Effect size

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

Subtotals only

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.25 [-0.30, 0.81]


0.47 [-0.51, 1.45]
Subtotals only
1.78 [-15.51, 19.07]
Subtotals only
0.34 [-0.64, 1.31]
0.04 [-0.63, 0.71]
-0.64 [-7.27, 5.99]
-0.64 [-7.27, 5.99]
Subtotals only
-6.03 [-20.67, 8.61]
0.17 [-0.22, 0.56]

1
1
2
1
1
1
1
1
1
3

34
105

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

54

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.30 [-0.24, 0.84]

1
1
1

34
17

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

-0.06 [-0.73, 0.61]


0.23 [-0.74, 1.20]
Subtotals only

54

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

33.41 [16.76, 50.06]

67

Odds Ratio (M-H, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.94 [0.36, 2.46]

34
17
34
17
17

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

93

Comparison 7. Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)

Outcome or subgroup title


1 Functional communication
1.1 Functional expression
2 Receptive language: auditory
comprehension - word
2.1 Word comprehension
(BDAE subtest)
2.2 Identify body part (BDAE
subtest)
2.3 Peabody PVT
3 Receptive language: other
auditory comprehension
3.1 Sentence comprehension
3.2 AAT comprehension
subtest
3.3 Token Test
4 Receptive language: auditory
comprehension (treated items)
4.1 Word comprehension
(phonology)
4.2 Word comprehension
(lexicon)
4.3 Sentence comprehension
(morphosyntax)
5 Receptive language: reading
comprehension
5.1 Reading comprehension
6 Receptive language: other
6.1 PICA gestural subtest
7 Expressive language: spoken
naming
7.1 Naming
7.2 Naming (3-week follow
up)
8 Expressive language: spoken
sentence construction
8.1 Sentence construction
(AmAT)
8.2 Sentence construction
(AmAT) 3-week follow up
9 Expressive language: other
spoken tasks
9.1 Word fluency
9.2 Picture description
9.3 PICA verbal subtest

No. of
studies

No. of
participants

2
2
2

31

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

Subtotals only
-0.25 [-0.96, 0.46]
Subtotals only

33

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

-0.02 [-0.70, 0.67]

21

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

-0.22 [-1.08, 0.64]

1
5

12

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.13 [-1.01, 1.26]


Subtotals only

1
1

21
17

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

-0.51 [-1.39, 0.36]


0.47 [-0.51, 1.45]

5
1

74

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

-0.00 [-0.46, 0.46]


Subtotals only

21

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

-2.0 [-8.45, 4.45]

21

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

-5.0 [-22.80, 12.80]

21

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

-12.00 [-52.89,
24.89]
Subtotals only

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

5.0 [-6.51, 16.51]


Subtotals only
-0.29 [-0.97, 0.39]
Subtotals only

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.04 [-0.43, 0.51]


0.70 [-0.60, 2.00]

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

Subtotals only

1
1
3
3
5

21

5
1

72
10

36

Statistical method

Effect size

31

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

0.02 [-0.69, 0.72]

10

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

-0.25 [-1.50, 1.00]

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

Subtotals only

Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

-1.05 [-1.93, -0.17]


-0.20 [-1.04, 0.64]
-0.31 [-0.99, 0.37]

3
2
2
3

24
24
36

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

94

10 Expressive language: spoken


(treated items)
10.1 Naming (treated)
10.2 Sentence construction
(treated)
10.3 Naming (treated: 3-week
follow up)
10.4 Sentence construction
(treated: 3-week follow up)
11 Expressive language: repetition
11.1 AAT repetition subtest
12 Expressive language: written
12.1 PICA graphic subtest
13 Severity of impairment
13.1 PICA overall
13.2 AAT overall
14 Number of drop-outs for any
reason

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

Subtotals only

1
1

10
10

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

8.0 [-0.46, 16.46]


3.2 [-1.06, 7.46]

10

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

6.20 [-0.37, 12.77]

10

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

4.00 [-1.16, 9.16]

1
1
3
3
4
3
1
1

17
17

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Odds Ratio (M-H, Random, 95% CI)

-0.64 [-7.27, 5.99]


-0.64 [-7.27, 5.99]
Subtotals only
-0.85 [-1.69, -0.01]
Subtotals only
-0.74 [-1.50, 0.01]
1.44 [-4.48, 7.36]
6.27 [0.70, 56.40]

36
36
17
52

Comparison 8. Operant training SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)

Outcome or subgroup title


1 Receptive language: auditory
comprehension
1.1 Word comprehension
(BDAE subtest)
1.2 Peabody PCT
1.3 Token Test
2 Receptive language: other
2.1 PICA gestural subtest
3 Expressive language: spoken
3.1 Naming
3.2 Word fluency
3.3 Picture description
3.4 PICA verbal subtest
4 Expressive language: written
4.1 PICA graphic subtest
5 Severity of impairment
5.1 PICA overall

No. of
studies

No. of
participants

Statistical method

Effect size

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

Subtotals only

12

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

1.5 [-10.73, 13.73]

1
3
3
3
3
3
2
2
3
3
3
3
3

12
36

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

1.5 [-10.73, 13.73]


4.99 [-7.05, 17.02]
Subtotals only
-0.29 [-0.97, 0.39]
Subtotals only
-0.25 [-0.92, 0.41]
-1.05 [-1.93, -0.17]
-0.20 [-1.04, 0.64]
-0.31 [-0.99, 0.37]
Subtotals only
-0.85 [-1.69, -0.01]
Subtotals only
-0.74 [-1.50, 0.01]

36
36
24
24
36
36
36

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

95

Comparison 9. Semantic SLT (SLTA) versus phonological SLT (SLT B)

Outcome or subgroup title


1 Functional communication
1.1 ANELT-A
2 Receptive language: auditory
2.1 Semantic Association Test:
PALPA (change from baseline)
2.2 Auditory lexical decision:
PALPA (change from baseline)
3 Receptive language: reading
3.1 Synonym judgement:
PALPA (change from baseline)
4 Expressive language: repetition
4.1 Non-words: PALPA
(change from baseline)
5 Number of drop-outs for any
reason

No. of
studies
1
1
1
1

No. of
participants

Statistical method

Effect size

46

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

Subtotals only
0.40 [-5.68, 6.48]
Subtotals only
1.30 [-1.00, 3.60]

46

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

-3.5 [-6.23, -0.77]

1
1

46
46

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

1.60 [-1.85, 5.05]


1.60 [-1.85, 5.05]

1
1

46

Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)


Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

Subtotals only
-1.7 [-4.49, 1.09]

58

Odds Ratio (M-H, Fixed, 95% CI)

1.0 [0.28, 3.56]

55

Comparison 10. Filmed programmed instruction SLT (SLT A) versus non-programmed activity SLT (SLTB)

Outcome or subgroup title


1 Receptive language: auditory
1.1 Word comprehension
2 Receptive language: reading
2.1 Reading comprehension
2.2 Reading recognition

No. of
studies

No. of
participants

1
1
1
1
1

14
14
14
14

Statistical method
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)
Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI)

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Effect size
0.72 [-2.41, 3.85]
0.72 [-2.41, 3.85]
Subtotals only
-0.08 [-1.50, 1.34]
-0.10 [-1.45, 1.25]

96

Analysis 1.1. Comparison 1 SLT versus no SLT, Outcome 1 Functional communication.


Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 1 SLT versus no SLT


Outcome: 1 Functional communication

Study or subgroup

SLT

No SLT

Std. Mean Difference

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Std. Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Katz 1997i

10

13.8 (5.3)

15

13.7 (5)

15.1 %

0.02 [ -0.78, 0.82 ]

Katz 1997ii

11

13.8 (5.3)

19

12.2 (6.7)

17.4 %

0.25 [ -0.50, 1.00 ]

32.5 %

0.14 [ -0.40, 0.69 ]

10.0 %

0.88 [ -0.10, 1.87 ]

10.0 %

0.88 [ -0.10, 1.87 ]

1 WAB

Subtotal (95% CI)

21

34

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 0.17, df = 1 (P = 0.68); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.51 (P = 0.61)
2 ANELT-A
Doesborgh 2004b

Subtotal (95% CI)

34.3 (8.4)

10

25.5 (10.3)

10

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 1.75 (P = 0.080)
3 Functional Communication Profile
Wertz 1986i

31

59.35 (19.62)

17

55.6 (19.56)

27.5 %

0.19 [ -0.40, 0.78 ]

Wertz 1986ii

37

62.05 (21.83)

18

55.6 (19.56)

30.1 %

0.30 [ -0.27, 0.87 ]

35

57.6 %

0.25 [ -0.16, 0.66 ]

79

100.0 %

0.28 [ -0.03, 0.59 ]

Subtotal (95% CI)

68

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 0.07, df = 1 (P = 0.79); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 1.18 (P = 0.24)

Total (95% CI)

97

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 1.94, df = 4 (P = 0.75); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 1.74 (P = 0.081)
Test for subgroup differences: Chi2 = 1.70, df = 2 (P = 0.43), I2 =0.0%

-2

-1

Favours No SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Favours SLT

97

Analysis 1.2. Comparison 1 SLT versus no SLT, Outcome 2 Receptive language: auditory comprehension.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 1 SLT versus no SLT


Outcome: 2 Receptive language: auditory comprehension

Study or subgroup

SLT

No SLT

Std. Mean Difference

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Std. Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Katz 1997i

10

61.7 (19.8)

15

58.7 (25.3)

13.6 %

0.12 [ -0.68, 0.93 ]

Katz 1997ii

11

61.7 (19.8)

19

57.9 (23.9)

15.8 %

0.16 [ -0.58, 0.91 ]

29.3 %

0.15 [ -0.40, 0.69 ]

1 PICA subtest

Subtotal (95% CI)

21

34

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 0.01, df = 1 (P = 0.94); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.52 (P = 0.60)
2 Token Test
Smania 2006

15

18.2 (7.65)

18

14.94 (10.23)

18.3 %

0.35 [ -0.34, 1.04 ]

Wertz 1986i

31

118.39 (41.95)

17

119.91 (38.48)

24.9 %

-0.04 [ -0.63, 0.55 ]

Wertz 1986ii

37

119.89 (45.06)

18

119.91 (38.48)

27.5 %

0.00 [ -0.56, 0.56 ]

53

70.7 %

0.08 [ -0.27, 0.43 ]

87

100.0 %

0.10 [ -0.20, 0.39 ]

Subtotal (95% CI)

83

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 0.80, df = 2 (P = 0.67); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.43 (P = 0.67)

Total (95% CI)

104

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 0.85, df = 4 (P = 0.93); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.64 (P = 0.52)
Test for subgroup differences: Chi2 = 0.04, df = 1 (P = 0.83), I2 =0.0%

-2

-1

Favours No SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Favours SLT

98

Analysis 1.3. Comparison 1 SLT versus no SLT, Outcome 3 Receptive language: reading comprehension.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 1 SLT versus no SLT


Outcome: 3 Receptive language: reading comprehension

Study or subgroup

SLT
N

No SLT
Mean(SD)

Std. Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Std. Mean Difference


IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 Reading Comprehension Battery for Aphasia


Wertz 1986i

31

76.9 (16.97)

17

75.03 (18.06)

30.5 %

0.11 [ -0.49, 0.70 ]

Wertz 1986ii

37

77.24 (20.79)

18

75.03 (18.06)

33.6 %

0.11 [ -0.45, 0.67 ]

64.1 %

0.11 [ -0.30, 0.52 ]

Subtotal (95% CI)

68

35

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 0.00, df = 1 (P = 0.99); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.52 (P = 0.61)
2 PICA reading subtest
Katz 1997i

10

69.8 (22.6)

15

69.3 (20.2)

16.7 %

0.02 [ -0.78, 0.82 ]

Katz 1997ii

11

69.8 (22.6)

19

65.1 (22.2)

19.3 %

0.20 [ -0.54, 0.95 ]

34

35.9 %

0.12 [ -0.42, 0.67 ]

69

100.0 %

0.11 [ -0.21, 0.44 ]

Subtotal (95% CI)

21

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 0.11, df = 1 (P = 0.74); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.43 (P = 0.67)

Total (95% CI)

89

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 0.11, df = 3 (P = 0.99); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.67 (P = 0.50)
Test for subgroup differences: Chi2 = 0.00, df = 1 (P = 0.97), I2 =0.0%

-1

-0.5

Favours No SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

0.5

Favours SLT

99

Analysis 1.4. Comparison 1 SLT versus no SLT, Outcome 4 Receptive language: other.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 1 SLT versus no SLT


Outcome: 4 Receptive language: other

Study or subgroup

SLT

No SLT

Mean Difference

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Katz 1997i

10

79.8 (14.1)

15

66.3 (21.9)

21.1 %

13.50 [ -0.61, 27.61 ]

Katz 1997ii

11

79.8 (14.1)

19

68.3 (23)

23.8 %

11.50 [ -1.78, 24.78 ]

Wertz 1986i

31

65.32 (19.03)

17

59.68 (20.98)

29.1 %

5.64 [ -6.37, 17.65 ]

Wertz 1986ii

37

62.78 (25.67)

18

59.68 (20.98)

25.9 %

3.10 [ -9.64, 15.84 ]

100.0 %

8.04 [ 1.55, 14.52 ]

1 PICA Gestural subtest

Subtotal (95% CI)

89

69

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 1.57, df = 3 (P = 0.67); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 2.43 (P = 0.015)

-50

-25

Favours No SLT

25

50

Favours SLT

Analysis 1.5. Comparison 1 SLT versus no SLT, Outcome 5 Receptive language: gesture comprehension
(unnamed).
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 1 SLT versus no SLT


Outcome: 5 Receptive language: gesture comprehension (unnamed)

Study or subgroup

SLT

No SLT

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

15

7.36 (2.17)

18

8.28 (1.36)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 Gesture (unnamed)
Smania 2006

Subtotal (95% CI)

15

18

100.0 %

-0.92 [ -2.19, 0.35 ]

100.0 %

-0.92 [ -2.19, 0.35 ]

100.0 %

-1.14 [ -3.23, 0.95 ]

100.0 %

-1.14 [ -3.23, 0.95 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 1.43 (P = 0.15)
2 Gesture (unnamed) 2-month follow up
Smania 2006

Subtotal (95% CI)

6.75 (2.81)

7.89 (1.17)

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 1.07 (P = 0.29)

-10

-5

Favours No SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

10

Favours SLT

100

Analysis 1.6. Comparison 1 SLT versus no SLT, Outcome 6 Expressive language: naming.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 1 SLT versus no SLT


Outcome: 6 Expressive language: naming

Study or subgroup

SLT

No SLT

Std. Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

Doesborgh 2004b

75.6 (38.7)

10

75.7 (36.7)

Subtotal (95% CI)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Std. Mean Difference


IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 Boston Naming Test

10

25.8 %

0.00 [ -0.93, 0.93 ]

25.8 %

0.00 [ -0.93, 0.93 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.01 (P = 1.0)
2 WAB Naming subtest
Katz 1997i

10

7 (2.4)

15

6.9 (2.8)

34.9 %

0.04 [ -0.76, 0.84 ]

Katz 1997ii

11

7 (2.4)

19

5.5 (3.3)

39.3 %

0.48 [ -0.27, 1.24 ]

34

74.2 %

0.27 [ -0.27, 0.82 ]

44

100.0 %

0.20 [ -0.27, 0.68 ]

Subtotal (95% CI)

21

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 0.64, df = 1 (P = 0.42); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.98 (P = 0.33)

Total (95% CI)

29

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 0.89, df = 2 (P = 0.64); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.84 (P = 0.40)
Test for subgroup differences: Chi2 = 0.25, df = 1 (P = 0.62), I2 =0.0%

-2

-1

Favours No SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Favours SLT

101

Analysis 1.7. Comparison 1 SLT versus no SLT, Outcome 7 Expressive language: general.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 1 SLT versus no SLT


Outcome: 7 Expressive language: general

Study or subgroup

SLT

No SLT

Mean Difference

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Katz 1997i

10

62.3 (22.3)

15

58.1 (19.1)

15.4 %

4.20 [ -12.67, 21.07 ]

Katz 1997ii

11

62.3 (22.3)

19

50.6 (24.5)

14.8 %

11.70 [ -5.48, 28.88 ]

Wertz 1986i

31

56.48 (18.29)

17

52.8 (19.48)

34.3 %

3.68 [ -7.60, 14.96 ]

Wertz 1986ii

37

57.41 (20.1)

18

52.8 (19.48)

35.5 %

4.61 [ -6.48, 15.70 ]

Total (95% CI)

89

100.0 %

5.28 [ -1.33, 11.89 ]

1 PICA Verbal subtest

69

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 0.64, df = 3 (P = 0.89); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 1.57 (P = 0.12)
Test for subgroup differences: Not applicable

-50

-25

Favours No SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

25

50

Favours SLT

102

Analysis 1.8. Comparison 1 SLT versus no SLT, Outcome 8 Expressive language: written.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 1 SLT versus no SLT


Outcome: 8 Expressive language: written

Study or subgroup

SLT

No SLT

Mean Difference

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Katz 1997i

10

61.9 (14.8)

15

60.4 (19)

52.5 %

1.50 [ -11.79, 14.79 ]

Katz 1997ii

11

61.9 (14.8)

19

55.4 (24.2)

47.5 %

6.50 [ -7.46, 20.46 ]

100.0 %

3.88 [ -5.75, 13.50 ]

1 PICA Copying subtest

Subtotal (95% CI)

21

34

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 0.26, df = 1 (P = 0.61); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.79 (P = 0.43)
2 PICA Writing subtest
Katz 1997i

10

66.9 (23.2)

15

59.2 (23.1)

48.0 %

7.70 [ -10.83, 26.23 ]

Katz 1997ii

11

66.9 (23.2)

19

57.9 (25.3)

52.0 %

9.00 [ -8.82, 26.82 ]

100.0 %

8.38 [ -4.47, 21.22 ]

Subtotal (95% CI)

21

34

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 0.01, df = 1 (P = 0.92); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 1.28 (P = 0.20)
3 PICA Graphic
Wertz 1986i

31

72.64 (16.6)

17

68.57 (22.69)

51.4 %

4.07 [ -8.20, 16.34 ]

Wertz 1986ii

37

74.86 (21.74)

18

68.57 (22.69)

48.6 %

6.29 [ -6.32, 18.90 ]

100.0 %

5.15 [ -3.64, 13.94 ]

Subtotal (95% CI)

68

35

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 0.06, df = 1 (P = 0.80); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 1.15 (P = 0.25)
Test for subgroup differences: Chi2 = 0.31, df = 2 (P = 0.86), I2 =0.0%

-50

-25

Favours No SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

25

50

Favours SLT

103

Analysis 1.9. Comparison 1 SLT versus no SLT, Outcome 9 Expressive language: repetition.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 1 SLT versus no SLT


Outcome: 9 Expressive language: repetition

Study or subgroup

SLT

No SLT

Mean Difference

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Katz 1997i

10

7.3 (2.9)

15

6.7 (3.4)

46.0 %

0.60 [ -1.89, 3.09 ]

Katz 1997ii

11

7.3 (2.9)

19

6.1 (3.4)

54.0 %

1.20 [ -1.10, 3.50 ]

100.0 %

0.92 [ -0.76, 2.61 ]

1 WAB Repetition subtest

Subtotal (95% CI)

21

34

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 0.12, df = 1 (P = 0.73); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 1.07 (P = 0.28)
Test for subgroup differences: Not applicable

-4

-2

Favours No SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Favours SLT

104

Analysis 1.10. Comparison 1 SLT versus no SLT, Outcome 10 Severity of impairment: Aphasia Battery
Score (+ PICA).
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 1 SLT versus no SLT


Outcome: 10 Severity of impairment: Aphasia Battery Score (+ PICA)

Study or subgroup

SLT

No SLT

Std. Mean Difference

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Std. Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Jufeng 2005i

30

66.93 (25.62)

15

62.4 (27.46)

17.9 %

0.17 [ -0.45, 0.79 ]

Jufeng 2005ii

24

57.8 (34.81)

15

62.4 (27.46)

16.6 %

-0.14 [ -0.79, 0.51 ]

34.5 %

0.02 [ -0.43, 0.47 ]

1 Aphasia Quotient (CRRCAE)

Subtotal (95% CI)

54

30

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 0.46, df = 1 (P = 0.50); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.09 (P = 0.93)
2 Porch Index of Communicative Ability
Katz 1997i

11

66.4 (19.4)

15

61.3 (17.4)

11.3 %

0.27 [ -0.51, 1.05 ]

Katz 1997ii

10

66.4 (19.4)

19

56.3 (20.9)

11.4 %

0.48 [ -0.30, 1.26 ]

Wertz 1986i

38

65.65 (24.64)

18

61.66 (21.21)

21.9 %

0.17 [ -0.40, 0.73 ]

Wertz 1986ii

37

67.19 (24.64)

17

61.66 (21.21)

20.8 %

0.23 [ -0.35, 0.81 ]

69

65.5 %

0.26 [ -0.07, 0.58 ]

99

100.0 %

0.18 [ -0.09, 0.44 ]

Subtotal (95% CI)

96

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 0.43, df = 3 (P = 0.93); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 1.57 (P = 0.12)

Total (95% CI)

150

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 1.60, df = 5 (P = 0.90); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 1.32 (P = 0.19)
Test for subgroup differences: Chi2 = 0.72, df = 1 (P = 0.40), I2 =0.0%

-2

-1

Favours No SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Favours SLT

105

Analysis 1.11. Comparison 1 SLT versus no SLT, Outcome 11 Severity of impairment: Aphasia Battery
Score (3-month follow up).
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 1 SLT versus no SLT


Outcome: 11 Severity of impairment: Aphasia Battery Score (3-month follow up)

Study or subgroup

SLT

No SLT

Mean(SD)

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Weight

IV,Random,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Random,95% CI

1 Aphasia Quotient (CRRCAE) 3-month follow up


Jufeng 2005i

30

71.16 (33.79)

15

33.66 (31.3)

49.8 %

37.50 [ 17.57, 57.43 ]

Jufeng 2005ii

24

37.75 (28.61)

15

33.66 (31.3)

50.2 %

4.09 [ -15.45, 23.63 ]

100.0 %

20.74 [ -12.01, 53.48 ]

Subtotal (95% CI)

54

30

Heterogeneity: Tau2 = 456.72; Chi2 = 5.50, df = 1 (P = 0.02); I2 =82%


Test for overall effect: Z = 1.24 (P = 0.21)

-100

-50

50

Favours No SLT

100

Favours SLT

Analysis 1.12. Comparison 1 SLT versus no SLT, Outcome 12 Psychosocial: MAACL.


Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 1 SLT versus no SLT


Outcome: 12 Psychosocial: MAACL

Study or subgroup

SLT

No SLT

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

75

3 (3.2)

62

2.6 (2.6)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 Anxiety Scale (MAACL)


Lincoln 1984a

Subtotal (95% CI)

75

62

100.0 %

0.40 [ -0.57, 1.37 ]

100.0 %

0.40 [ -0.57, 1.37 ]

100.0 %

0.70 [ -1.38, 2.78 ]

100.0 %

0.70 [ -1.38, 2.78 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.81 (P = 0.42)
2 Depression Scale (MAACL)
Lincoln 1984a

Subtotal (95% CI)

75

75

6.9 (6.6)

62

6.2 (5.8)

62

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.66 (P = 0.51)

-4

-2

Favours SLT

Favours No SLT

(Continued . . . )

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

106

Study or subgroup

SLT

No SLT

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

75

2.7 (2.7)

62

2.8 (2.1)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

(. . . Continued)
Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

3 Hostility Scale (MAACL)


Lincoln 1984a

Subtotal (95% CI)

75

62

100.0 %

-0.10 [ -0.90, 0.70 ]

100.0 %

-0.10 [ -0.90, 0.70 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.24 (P = 0.81)

-4

-2

Favours SLT

Favours No SLT

Analysis 1.13. Comparison 1 SLT versus no SLT, Outcome 13 Number of drop-outs (any reason).
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 1 SLT versus no SLT


Outcome: 13 Number of drop-outs (any reason)

Study or subgroup

SLT

No SLT

Odds Ratio

n/N

n/N

M-H,Fixed,95% CI

1/9

0/10

0.6 %

3.71 [ 0.13, 103.11 ]

Katz 1997i

0/10

6/21

5.7 %

0.11 [ 0.01, 2.24 ]

Katz 1997ii

0/11

2/21

2.3 %

0.34 [ 0.01, 7.70 ]

Lincoln 1984a

76/163

90/164

65.8 %

0.72 [ 0.46, 1.11 ]

MacKay 1988

0/48

1/48

2.0 %

0.33 [ 0.01, 8.22 ]

Smania 2006

12/20

12/21

6.4 %

1.13 [ 0.32, 3.90 ]

Smith 1981i

10/16

0/9

0.3 %

30.69 [ 1.52, 621.02 ]

Smith 1981ii

6/14

0/8

0.5 %

13.00 [ 0.63, 268.93 ]

Wertz 1986i

9/38

5/20

6.9 %

0.93 [ 0.26, 3.28 ]

Wertz 1986ii

7/43

6/20

9.4 %

0.45 [ 0.13, 1.59 ]

Total (95% CI)

372

342

100.0 %

0.86 [ 0.61, 1.20 ]

Doesborgh 2004b

Weight

Odds Ratio
M-H,Fixed,95% CI

Total events: 121 (SLT), 122 (No SLT)


Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 13.56, df = 9 (P = 0.14); I2 =34%
Test for overall effect: Z = 0.89 (P = 0.38)

0.02

0.1

Favours SLT

10

50

Favours No SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

107

Analysis 2.1. Comparison 2 SLT versus social support and stimulation, Outcome 1 Functional
communication.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 2 SLT versus social support and stimulation


Outcome: 1 Functional communication

Study or subgroup

SLT
N

Social Support

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

67 (20.3)

48

69.2 (22.4)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 Functional Communication Profile


David 1982

Subtotal (95% CI)

48

48

48

100.0 %

-2.20 [ -10.75, 6.35 ]

100.0 %

-2.20 [ -10.75, 6.35 ]

100.0 %

1.40 [ -8.01, 10.81 ]

100.0 %

1.40 [ -8.01, 10.81 ]

100.0 %

1.30 [ -8.07, 10.67 ]

100.0 %

1.30 [ -8.07, 10.67 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.50 (P = 0.61)
2 FCP (3-month follow up)
David 1982

Subtotal (95% CI)

37

70.4 (19.1)

37

36

69 (21.8)

36

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.29 (P = 0.77)
3 FCP (6-month follow up)
David 1982

Subtotal (95% CI)

37

69.3 (19.6)

37

36

68 (21.2)

36

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.27 (P = 0.79)
Test for subgroup differences: Chi2 = 0.41, df = 2 (P = 0.81), I2 =0.0%

-20

-10

Favours Social Support

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

10

20

Favours SLT

108

Analysis 2.2. Comparison 2 SLT versus social support and stimulation, Outcome 2 Receptive language:
auditory comprehension.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 2 SLT versus social support and stimulation


Outcome: 2 Receptive language: auditory comprehension

Study or subgroup

SLT
N

Social Support
Mean(SD)

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

66 (4)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 Sentence Comprehension Test (PCB)


Rochon 2005

Subtotal (95% CI)

72 (16)

100.0 %

6.00 [ -12.94, 24.94 ]

100.0 %

6.00 [ -12.94, 24.94 ]

100.0 %

8.00 [ -10.94, 26.94 ]

100.0 %

8.00 [ -10.94, 26.94 ]

100.0 %

-3.83 [ -18.95, 11.29 ]

100.0 %

-3.83 [ -18.95, 11.29 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.62 (P = 0.53)
2 Picture Comprehension Test (PCB)
Rochon 2005

Subtotal (95% CI)

78 (16)

70 (4)

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.83 (P = 0.41)
3 Token Test
Lincoln 1982iii

Subtotal (95% CI)

12

59 (13.93)

12

62.83 (16.13)

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.50 (P = 0.62)
Test for subgroup differences: Chi2 = 1.13, df = 2 (P = 0.57), I2 =0.0%

-50

-25

Favours Social Support

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

25

50

Favours SLT

109

Analysis 2.3. Comparison 2 SLT versus social support and stimulation, Outcome 3 Receptive language:
other.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 2 SLT versus social support and stimulation


Outcome: 3 Receptive language: other

Study or subgroup

SLT

Social Support

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

12

12.14 (0.8)

13.01 (0.87)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 PICA Gestural subtest


Lincoln 1982iii

Subtotal (95% CI)

12

100.0 %

-0.87 [ -1.70, -0.04 ]

100.0 %

-0.87 [ -1.70, -0.04 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 2.05 (P = 0.040)
Test for subgroup differences: Not applicable

-2

-1

Favours Social Support

Favours SLT

Analysis 2.4. Comparison 2 SLT versus social support and stimulation, Outcome 4 Expressive language:
single words.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 2 SLT versus social support and stimulation


Outcome: 4 Expressive language: single words

Study or subgroup

SLT

Social Support

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

12

9.83 (6.32)

16.83 (3.76)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 Object Naming Test (ONT)


Lincoln 1982iii

Subtotal (95% CI)

12

100.0 %

-7.00 [ -11.67, -2.33 ]

100.0 %

-7.00 [ -11.67, -2.33 ]

100.0 %

-14.00 [ -20.35, -7.65 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 2.94 (P = 0.0033)
2 Word fluency
Lincoln 1982iii

Subtotal (95% CI)

12

10 (5.98)

12

24 (6.72)

100.0 % -14.00 [ -20.35, -7.65 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 4.32 (P = 0.000016)
Test for subgroup differences: Chi2 = 3.03, df = 1 (P = 0.08), I2 =67%

-20

-10

Favours Social Support

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

10

20

Favours SLT

110

Analysis 2.5. Comparison 2 SLT versus social support and stimulation, Outcome 5 Expressive language:
sentences.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 2 SLT versus social support and stimulation


Outcome: 5 Expressive language: sentences

Study or subgroup

SLT

Social Support

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

7 (2)

5 (3)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 Caplan % Hanna Test: total


Rochon 2005

Subtotal (95% CI)

100.0 %

2.00 [ -2.73, 6.73 ]

100.0 %

2.00 [ -2.73, 6.73 ]

100.0 %

3.00 [ 0.63, 5.37 ]

100.0 %

3.00 [ 0.63, 5.37 ]

100.0 %

-1.00 [ -5.31, 3.31 ]

100.0 %

-1.00 [ -5.31, 3.31 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.83 (P = 0.41)
2 Caplan % Hanna Test: treated
Rochon 2005

Subtotal (95% CI)

6 (2)

3 (0.5)

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 2.48 (P = 0.013)
3 Caplan % Hanna Test: untreated
Rochon 2005

Subtotal (95% CI)

1 (1)

2 (3)

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.45 (P = 0.65)
Test for subgroup differences: Chi2 = 2.54, df = 2 (P = 0.28), I2 =21%

-10

-5

Favours Social Support

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

10

Favours SLT

111

Analysis 2.6. Comparison 2 SLT versus social support and stimulation, Outcome 6 Expressive language:
picture description.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 2 SLT versus social support and stimulation


Outcome: 6 Expressive language: picture description

Study or subgroup

SLT

Social Support

Std. Mean Difference

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Std. Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Lincoln 1982iii

12

33.67 (22)

30.67 (7.87)

81.5 %

0.15 [ -0.83, 1.13 ]

Rochon 2005

34.67 (4.04)

27 (11.31)

18.5 %

0.76 [ -1.30, 2.83 ]

100.0 %

0.26 [ -0.62, 1.15 ]

100.0 %

0.45 [ -1.44, 2.33 ]

100.0 %

0.45 [ -1.44, 2.33 ]

100.0 %

0.41 [ -1.46, 2.28 ]

100.0 %

0.41 [ -1.46, 2.28 ]

1 Picture description

Subtotal (95% CI)

15

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 0.27, df = 1 (P = 0.60); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.59 (P = 0.56)
2 Picture description with structure modelling: treated items
Rochon 2005

Subtotal (95% CI)

16 (2.65)

14 (4.24)

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.46 (P = 0.64)
3 Picture description with structure modelling: untreated items
Rochon 2005

Subtotal (95% CI)

18.67 (3.06)

16 (7.07)

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.43 (P = 0.67)
Test for subgroup differences: Chi2 = 0.04, df = 2 (P = 0.98), I2 =0.0%

-4

-2

Favours Social Support

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Favours SLT

112

Analysis 2.7. Comparison 2 SLT versus social support and stimulation, Outcome 7 Expressive language:
overall spoken.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 2 SLT versus social support and stimulation


Outcome: 7 Expressive language: overall spoken

Study or subgroup

SLT

Social Support

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

12

10.52 (1.2)

12.08 (0.74)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 PICA verbal subtest


Lincoln 1982iii

Subtotal (95% CI)

12

100.0 %

-1.56 [ -2.46, -0.66 ]

100.0 %

-1.56 [ -2.46, -0.66 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 3.39 (P = 0.00069)
Test for subgroup differences: Not applicable

-2

-1

Favours Social Support

Favours SLT

Analysis 2.8. Comparison 2 SLT versus social support and stimulation, Outcome 8 Expressive language:
written.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 2 SLT versus social support and stimulation


Outcome: 8 Expressive language: written

Study or subgroup

SLT

Social Support

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

12

7.52 (1.34)

8.91 (1)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 PICA graphic subtests


Lincoln 1982iii

Subtotal (95% CI)

12

100.0 %

-1.39 [ -2.49, -0.29 ]

100.0 %

-1.39 [ -2.49, -0.29 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 2.47 (P = 0.013)
Test for subgroup differences: Not applicable

-4

-2

Favours Social Support

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Favours SLT

113

Analysis 2.9. Comparison 2 SLT versus social support and stimulation, Outcome 9 Severity of impairment:
Aphasia Battery Score.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 2 SLT versus social support and stimulation


Outcome: 9 Severity of impairment: Aphasia Battery Score

Study or subgroup

SLT

Social Support

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

12

10.3 (1.01)

11.43 (0.67)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 PICA
Lincoln 1982iii

Subtotal (95% CI)

12

100.0 %

-1.13 [ -1.91, -0.35 ]

100.0 %

-1.13 [ -1.91, -0.35 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 2.83 (P = 0.0047)
Test for subgroup differences: Not applicable

-2

-1

Favours Social Support

Favours SLT

Analysis 2.10. Comparison 2 SLT versus social support and stimulation, Outcome 10 Number of drop-outs
for any reason.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 2 SLT versus social support and stimulation


Outcome: 10 Number of drop-outs for any reason

Study or subgroup

SLT

Social Support

n/N

n/N

Odds Ratio

Weight

David 1982

34/71

48/84

73.0 %

0.69 [ 0.37, 1.30 ]

Elman 1999

3/14

4/14

9.9 %

0.68 [ 0.12, 3.83 ]

Shewan 1984ii

6/28

3/13

11.9 %

0.91 [ 0.19, 4.39 ]

Shewan 1984iii

1/24

3/12

5.2 %

0.13 [ 0.01, 1.42 ]

Total (95% CI)

137

123

100.0 %

0.65 [ 0.38, 1.12 ]

M-H,Random,95% CI

Odds Ratio
M-H,Random,95% CI

Total events: 44 (SLT), 58 (Social Support)


Heterogeneity: Tau2 = 0.0; Chi2 = 1.95, df = 3 (P = 0.58); I2 =0.0%
Test for overall effect: Z = 1.54 (P = 0.12)

0.005

0.1

Favours SLT

10

200

Favours Social Support

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

114

Analysis 3.1. Comparison 3 Functional SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 1 Functional
communication.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 3 Functional SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 1 Functional communication

Study or subgroup

Functional SLT

Conventional SLT

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

0.5 (5)

9.8 (5.1)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 CADL (change from baseline)


Hinckley 2001

Subtotal (95% CI)

100.0 %

-9.30 [ -15.01, -3.59 ]

100.0 % -9.30 [ -15.01, -3.59 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 3.19 (P = 0.0014)
2 CETI
Hinckley 2001

Subtotal (95% CI)

9.8 (4.3)

13.7 (4.1)

100.0 %

-3.90 [ -8.65, 0.85 ]

100.0 %

-3.90 [ -8.65, 0.85 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 1.61 (P = 0.11)

-20

-10

Favours Conventional SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

10

20

Favours Functional SLT

115

Analysis 3.2. Comparison 3 Functional SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 2 Functional
communication: catalogue ordering.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 3 Functional SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 2 Functional communication: catalogue ordering

Study or subgroup

Functional SLT
N

Conventional SLT

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

32.3 (15)

-0.5 (14.4)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 Telephone order (change from baseline)


Hinckley 2001

Subtotal (95% CI)

100.0 %

32.80 [ 16.16, 49.44 ]

100.0 % 32.80 [ 16.16, 49.44 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 3.86 (P = 0.00011)
2 Telephone order (+ concurrent task) (change from baseline)
Hinckley 2001

Subtotal (95% CI)

24.2 (15.5)

100.0 %

16.90 [ 1.31, 32.49 ]

100.0 %

16.90 [ 1.31, 32.49 ]

100.0 %

-5.00 [ -19.81, 9.81 ]

7.3 (11.8)

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 2.13 (P = 0.034)
3 Written order (change from baseline)
Hinckley 2001

Subtotal (95% CI)

6.8 (14.5)

11.8 (11.5)

100.0 % -5.00 [ -19.81, 9.81 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.66 (P = 0.51)
4 Written order (+ concurrent task) (change from baseline)
Hinckley 2001

Subtotal (95% CI)

24.3 (20.6)

14.5 (14)

100.0 %

9.80 [ -10.13, 29.73 ]

100.0 % 9.80 [ -10.13, 29.73 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.96 (P = 0.34)

-100

-50

Favours Conventional SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

50

100

Favours Functional SLT

116

Analysis 3.3. Comparison 3 Functional SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 3 Expressive
language: spoken.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 3 Functional SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 3 Expressive language: spoken

Study or subgroup

Functional SLT
N

Conventional SLT
Mean(SD)

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

7.5 (10.1)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 Oral naming: PALPA (change from baseline)


Hinckley 2001

Subtotal (95% CI)

13.5 (14.6)

100.0 %

6.00 [ -8.21, 20.21 ]

100.0 % 6.00 [ -8.21, 20.21 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.83 (P = 0.41)

-100

-50

Favours Conventional SLT

50

100

Favours Functional SLT

Analysis 3.4. Comparison 3 Functional SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 4 Expressive
language: written.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 3 Functional SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 4 Expressive language: written

Study or subgroup

Functional SLT
N

Conventional SLT
Mean(SD)

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

9.7 (8.4)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 Written naming: PALPA (change from baseline)


Hinckley 2001

Subtotal (95% CI)

7.5 (7.6)

100.0 %

-2.20 [ -11.26, 6.86 ]

100.0 % -2.20 [ -11.26, 6.86 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.48 (P = 0.63)
Test for subgroup differences: Not applicable

-100

-50

Favours Conventional SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

50

100

Favours Funcational SLT

117

Analysis 4.1. Comparison 4 Intensive SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 1 Receptive
language: auditory comprehension (change from baseline).
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 4 Intensive SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 1 Receptive language: auditory comprehension (change from baseline)

Study or subgroup

Intensive

Conventional

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

12.6 (15.2)

2.3 (3.8)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 AAT Comprehension subtest


Denes 1996

Subtotal (95% CI)

100.0 %

10.30 [ -0.52, 21.12 ]

100.0 % 10.30 [ -0.52, 21.12 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 1.87 (P = 0.062)
2 Token Test
Denes 1996

Subtotal (95% CI)

11.4 (11.6)

5.2 (7.8)

100.0 %

6.20 [ -3.32, 15.72 ]

100.0 %

6.20 [ -3.32, 15.72 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 1.28 (P = 0.20)
Test for subgroup differences: Chi2 = 0.31, df = 1 (P = 0.58), I2 =0.0%

-20

-10

Favours Conventional SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

10

20

Favours Intensive SLT

118

Analysis 4.2. Comparison 4 Intensive SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 2 Expressive
language: spoken (change from baseline scores).
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 4 Intensive SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 2 Expressive language: spoken (change from baseline scores)

Study or subgroup

Intensive

Conventional

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

10.2 (9.9)

4.5 (4.2)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 Naming subtest (AAT)


Denes 1996

Subtotal (95% CI)

100.0 %

5.70 [ -1.69, 13.09 ]

100.0 %

5.70 [ -1.69, 13.09 ]

100.0 %

2.80 [ -3.86, 9.46 ]

100.0 %

2.80 [ -3.86, 9.46 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 1.51 (P = 0.13)
2 Repetition subtest (AAT)
Denes 1996

Subtotal (95% CI)

8.9 (7.7)

6.1 (6.1)

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.82 (P = 0.41)

-20

-10

Favours Conventional SLT

10

20

Favours Intensive SLT

Analysis 4.3. Comparison 4 Intensive SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 3 Written
language: (change from baseline scores).
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 4 Intensive SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 3 Written language: (change from baseline scores)

Study or subgroup

Intensive

Conventional

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

11 (9.8)

2.1 (3.1)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 Written subtest (AAT)


Denes 1996

Subtotal (95% CI)

100.0 %

8.90 [ 1.81, 15.99 ]

100.0 %

8.90 [ 1.81, 15.99 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 2.46 (P = 0.014)

-20

-10

Favours Conventional SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

10

20

Favours Intensive SLT

119

Analysis 4.4. Comparison 4 Intensive SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 4 Severity of
impairment: Aphasia Battery Score.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 4 Intensive SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 4 Severity of impairment: Aphasia Battery Score

Study or subgroup

Intensive

Conventional

Mean Difference

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Bakheit 2007

35

70.3 (26.9)

38

66.2 (26.2)

69.9 %

4.10 [ -8.10, 16.30 ]

ORLA 2006

57.58 (14.82)

60.48 (19.35)

30.1 %

-2.90 [ -21.50, 15.70 ]

100.0 %

2.00 [ -8.21, 12.20 ]

100.0 %

5.70 [ -0.76, 12.16 ]

100.0 %

5.70 [ -0.76, 12.16 ]

100.0 %

1.90 [ -10.53, 14.33 ]

1 Aphasia Quotient (WAB)

Subtotal (95% CI)

41

45

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 0.38, df = 1 (P = 0.54); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.38 (P = 0.70)
2 AAT profile (change from baseline)
Denes 1996

Subtotal (95% CI)

10 (8.6)

4.3 (3.8)

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 1.73 (P = 0.084)
3 Aphasia Quotient (WAB) 3-month follow up
Bakheit 2007

Subtotal (95% CI)

31

69.9 (25.2)

31

35

68 (26.3)

100.0 % 1.90 [ -10.53, 14.33 ]

35

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.30 (P = 0.76)
Test for subgroup differences: Chi2 = 0.52, df = 2 (P = 0.77), I2 =0.0%

-50

-25

Favours Conventional SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

25

50

Favours Intensive SLT

120

Analysis 4.5. Comparison 4 Intensive SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 5 Number of
drop-outs for any reason.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 4 Intensive SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 5 Number of drop-outs for any reason

Study or subgroup

Intensive

Conventional

Odds Ratio

n/N

n/N

M-H,Fixed,95% CI

Bakheit 2007

20/51

11/46

74.6 %

2.05 [ 0.85, 4.95 ]

Smith 1981iii

10/16

6/14

25.4 %

2.22 [ 0.51, 9.61 ]

67

60

100.0 %

2.10 [ 0.99, 4.46 ]

Total (95% CI)

Weight

Odds Ratio
M-H,Fixed,95% CI

Total events: 30 (Intensive), 17 (Conventional)


Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 0.01, df = 1 (P = 0.93); I2 =0.0%
Test for overall effect: Z = 1.92 (P = 0.055)

0.01

0.1

Favours Intensive SLT

10

100

Favours Conventional SLT

Analysis 5.1. Comparison 5 Volunteer-facilitated SLT (SLTA) versus professional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 1
Functional communication.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 5 Volunteer-facilitated SLT (SLTA) versus professional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 1 Functional communication

Study or subgroup

Volunteer Facilitated SLT


N

Professional SLT
Mean(SD)

Mean Difference
Mean(SD)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 CADL
Wertz 1986iii

Subtotal (95% CI)

37 105.38 (31.67)

37

31 103.74 (24.42)

100.0 %

31

1.64 [ -11.70, 14.98 ]

100.0 % 1.64 [ -11.70, 14.98 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.24 (P = 0.81)
2 Functional Communication Profile
Wertz 1986iii

Subtotal (95% CI)

37

62.05 (21.83)

37

100.0 %

31 59.35 (19.62)

2.70 [ -7.16, 12.56 ]

100.0 % 2.70 [ -7.16, 12.56 ]

31

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.54 (P = 0.59)
Test for subgroup differences: Chi2 = 0.02, df = 1 (P = 0.90), I2 =0.0%

-100

-50

Favours Professional SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

50

100

Favours Volunteer SLT

121

Analysis 5.2. Comparison 5 Volunteer-facilitated SLT (SLTA) versus professional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 2
Receptive language: auditory comprehension.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 5 Volunteer-facilitated SLT (SLTA) versus professional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 2 Receptive language: auditory comprehension

Study or subgroup

Volunteer Facilitated SLT

Professional SLT

Std. Mean Difference

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Std. Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Meinzer 2007

10

23.2 (13.25)

10

21.1 (17.84)

22.8 %

0.13 [ -0.75, 1.01 ]

Wertz 1986iii

37 119.89 (45.06)

31 118.39 (41.95)

77.2 %

0.03 [ -0.44, 0.51 ]

100.0 %

0.06 [ -0.36, 0.47 ]

100.0 %

-0.37 [ -1.25, 0.52 ]

100.0 %

-0.37 [ -1.25, 0.52 ]

1 Token Test

Subtotal (95% CI)

47

41

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 0.03, df = 1 (P = 0.85); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.26 (P = 0.80)
2 AAT subtest
Meinzer 2007

Subtotal (95% CI)

10

10

90 (15.78)

10

95.7 (13.92)

10

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.81 (P = 0.42)

-4

-2

Favours Professional SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Favours Vol Facilitated

122

Analysis 5.3. Comparison 5 Volunteer-facilitated SLT (SLTA) versus professional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 3
Receptive language: reading comprehension.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 5 Volunteer-facilitated SLT (SLTA) versus professional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 3 Receptive language: reading comprehension

Study or subgroup

Volunteer Facilitated SLT

Professional SLT

Mean(SD)

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 Reading Comprehension Battery for Aphasia


Wertz 1986iii

37 77.24 (20.79)

Subtotal (95% CI)

100.0 %

31 76.9 (16.97)

37

31

0.34 [ -8.64, 9.32 ]

100.0 % 0.34 [ -8.64, 9.32 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.07 (P = 0.94)
Test for subgroup differences: Not applicable

-100

-50

Favours Professional

50

100

Favours Volunteer

Analysis 5.4. Comparison 5 Volunteer-facilitated SLT (SLTA) versus professional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 4
Receptive language: other.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 5 Volunteer-facilitated SLT (SLTA) versus professional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 4 Receptive language: other

Study or subgroup Volunteer Facilitated SLT


N

Professional SLT
Mean(SD)

Mean Difference
Mean(SD)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 PICA gestural subtest


Wertz 1986iii

Total (95% CI)

37 62.78 (25.67)

37

31 65.32 (19.03)

100.0 %

-2.54 [ -13.18, 8.10 ]

100.0 % -2.54 [ -13.18, 8.10 ]

31

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.47 (P = 0.64)
Test for subgroup differences: Not applicable

-20

-10

Favours Professional

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

10

20

Favours Volunteer

123

Analysis 5.5. Comparison 5 Volunteer-facilitated SLT (SLTA) versus professional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 5
Expressive language: spoken.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 5 Volunteer-facilitated SLT (SLTA) versus professional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 5 Expressive language: spoken

Study or subgroup

Volunteer Facilitated SLT

Professional SLT

Mean(SD)

10

87.5 (19.65)

Mean Difference
Mean(SD)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 AAT naming subtest


Meinzer 2007

Subtotal (95% CI)

10 79.1 (27.77)

10

100.0 %

10

8.40 [ -12.68, 29.48 ]

100.0 % 8.40 [ -12.68, 29.48 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.78 (P = 0.43)
2 PICA verbal subtest
Wertz 1986iii

37 57.41 (20.01)

Subtotal (95% CI)

100.0 %

31 56.48 (18.29)

37

31

0.93 [ -8.18, 10.04 ]

100.0 % 0.93 [ -8.18, 10.04 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.20 (P = 0.84)

-100

-50

Favours Professional SLT

50

100

Favours Vol Facilitated

Analysis 5.6. Comparison 5 Volunteer-facilitated SLT (SLTA) versus professional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 6
Expressive language: repetition.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 5 Volunteer-facilitated SLT (SLTA) versus professional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 6 Expressive language: repetition

Study or subgroup

Volunteer Facilitated SLT


N

Professional SLT
Mean(SD)

Mean Difference
Mean(SD)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 AAT Repetition subtest


Meinzer 2007

Subtotal (95% CI)

10 129 (13.53)

10

10 115.5 (16.68)

100.0 %

13.50 [ 0.19, 26.81 ]

100.0 % 13.50 [ 0.19, 26.81 ]

10

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 1.99 (P = 0.047)
Test for subgroup differences: Not applicable

-50

-25

Favours Professional SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

25

50

Favours Vol Facilitated

124

Analysis 5.7. Comparison 5 Volunteer-facilitated SLT (SLTA) versus professional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 7
Expressive language: written.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 5 Volunteer-facilitated SLT (SLTA) versus professional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 7 Expressive language: written

Study or subgroup

Volunteer Facilitated SLT

Professional SLT

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

10

58.1 (24.4)

10

48.6 (23.8)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 AAT written language subtest


Meinzer 2007

Subtotal (95% CI)

10

100.0 %

10

9.50 [ -11.63, 30.63 ]

100.0 % 9.50 [ -11.63, 30.63 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.88 (P = 0.38)
2 PICA graphic subtests
Wertz 1986iii

Subtotal (95% CI)

37 74.86 (21.74)

37

31 72.64 (16.6)

100.0 %

31

2.22 [ -6.90, 11.34 ]

100.0 % 2.22 [ -6.90, 11.34 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.48 (P = 0.63)

-100

-50

Favours Professional SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

50

100

Favours Vol Facilitated

125

Analysis 5.8. Comparison 5 Volunteer-facilitated SLT (SLTA) versus professional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 8
Severity of impairment: Aphasia Battery Score.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 5 Volunteer-facilitated SLT (SLTA) versus professional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 8 Severity of impairment: Aphasia Battery Score

Study or subgroup

Volunteer Facilitated SLT

Professional SLT

Std. Mean Difference

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Std. Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Meikle 1979

15

62.2 (27.12)

16

72 (22.9)

24.3 %

-0.38 [ -1.09, 0.33 ]

Wertz 1986iii

37 67.19 (24.64)

38 65.65 (18.85)

60.1 %

0.07 [ -0.38, 0.52 ]

84.5 %

-0.06 [ -0.44, 0.32 ]

15.5 %

-0.45 [ -1.34, 0.44 ]

1 PICA

Subtotal (95% CI)

52

54

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 1.10, df = 1 (P = 0.29); I2 =9%


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.31 (P = 0.76)
2 AAT
Meinzer 2007

Subtotal (95% CI)

10

52.96 (5.49)

10 55.54 (5.44)

10

10

15.5 %

-0.45 [ -1.34, 0.44 ]

62

64

100.0 %

-0.12 [ -0.47, 0.23 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 1.00 (P = 0.32)

Total (95% CI)

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 1.73, df = 2 (P = 0.42); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.68 (P = 0.50)
Test for subgroup differences: Chi2 = 0.63, df = 1 (P = 0.43), I2 =0.0%

-2

-1

Favours Professional SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Favours Vol Facilitated

126

Analysis 5.9. Comparison 5 Volunteer-facilitated SLT (SLTA) versus professional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 9
Number of drop-outs for any reason.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 5 Volunteer-facilitated SLT (SLTA) versus professional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 9 Number of drop-outs for any reason

Study or subgroup

Volunteer Facilitated SLT

Professional SLT

n/N

n/N

21/59

13/35

58.9 %

0.94 [ 0.39, 2.23 ]

Meikle 1979

0/16

2/15

4.6 %

0.16 [ 0.01, 3.71 ]

Wertz 1986iii

9/38

7/43

36.6 %

1.60 [ 0.53, 4.81 ]

Total (95% CI)

113

93

100.0 %

1.05 [ 0.54, 2.04 ]

Leal 1993

Odds Ratio

Weight

M-H,Random,95% CI

Odds Ratio
M-H,Random,95% CI

Total events: 30 (Volunteer Facilitated SLT), 22 (Professional SLT)


Heterogeneity: Tau2 = 0.0; Chi2 = 2.00, df = 2 (P = 0.37); I2 =0.0%
Test for overall effect: Z = 0.14 (P = 0.89)

0.01

0.1

Favours Vol Facilitated

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

10

100

Favours Professional SLT

127

Analysis 6.1. Comparison 6 Group SLT (SLTA) versus 1-to-1 SLT (SLTB), Outcome 1 Receptive language:
auditory comprehension.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 6 Group SLT (SLTA) versus 1-to-1 SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 1 Receptive language: auditory comprehension

Study or subgroup

Group SLT

1-to-1 SLT

Std. Mean Difference

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Std. Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Pulvermuller 2001

10

53 (7.24)

54 (8.16)

33.3 %

-0.12 [ -1.09, 0.84 ]

Wertz 1981

16

40.19 (13.93)

18

33.89 (13.93)

66.7 %

0.44 [ -0.24, 1.12 ]

100.0 %

0.25 [ -0.30, 0.81 ]

100.0 %

0.47 [ -0.51, 1.45 ]

100.0 %

0.47 [ -0.51, 1.45 ]

1 Token Test

Subtotal (95% CI)

26

25

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 0.88, df = 1 (P = 0.35); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.89 (P = 0.37)
2 AAT comprehension subtest
Pulvermuller 2001

10

Subtotal (95% CI)

10

60.3 (9.29)

55.29 (11.19)

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.94 (P = 0.35)
Test for subgroup differences: Chi2 = 0.14, df = 1 (P = 0.71), I2 =0.0%

-4

-2

Favours 1-to-1 SLT

Favours Group SLT

Analysis 6.2. Comparison 6 Group SLT (SLTA) versus 1-to-1 SLT (SLTB), Outcome 2 Receptive language:
other.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 6 Group SLT (SLTA) versus 1-to-1 SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 2 Receptive language: other

Study or subgroup

Group SLT

1-to-1 SLT

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

16

72 (25.67)

18

70.22 (25.67)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 PICA gestural subtest


Wertz 1981

Subtotal (95% CI)

16

100.0 %

1.78 [ -15.51, 19.07 ]

100.0 % 1.78 [ -15.51, 19.07 ]

18

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.20 (P = 0.84)

-50

-25

Favours 1-to-1 SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

25

50

Favours Group SLT

128

Analysis 6.3. Comparison 6 Group SLT (SLTA) versus 1-to-1 SLT (SLTB), Outcome 3 Expressive language:
spoken.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 6 Group SLT (SLTA) versus 1-to-1 SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 3 Expressive language: spoken

Study or subgroup

Group SLT

1-to-1 SLT

Std. Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

Pulvermuller 2001

10

56.5 (6.35)

54.14 (7.01)

Subtotal (95% CI)

10

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Std. Mean Difference


IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 AAT naming subtest

100.0 %

0.34 [ -0.64, 1.31 ]

100.0 %

0.34 [ -0.64, 1.31 ]

100.0 %

0.04 [ -0.63, 0.71 ]

100.0 %

0.04 [ -0.63, 0.71 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.68 (P = 0.50)
2 PICA verbal subtest
Wertz 1981

Subtotal (95% CI)

16

66.25 (20.01)

16

18

65.44 (20.01)

18

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.12 (P = 0.91)
Test for subgroup differences: Chi2 = 0.24, df = 1 (P = 0.62), I2 =0.0%

-1

-0.5

Favours 1-to-1 SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

0.5

Favours Group SLT

129

Analysis 6.4. Comparison 6 Group SLT (SLTA) versus 1-to-1 SLT (SLTB), Outcome 4 Expressive language:
repetition.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 6 Group SLT (SLTA) versus 1-to-1 SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 4 Expressive language: repetition

Study or subgroup

Group SLT

1-to-1 SLT

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

10

52.5 (4.22)

53.14 (8.23)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 AAT repetition subtest


Pulvermuller 2001

Total (95% CI)

10

100.0 %

-0.64 [ -7.27, 5.99 ]

100.0 %

-0.64 [ -7.27, 5.99 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.19 (P = 0.85)
Test for subgroup differences: Not applicable

-10

-5

Favours 1-to-1 SLT

10

Favours Group SLT

Analysis 6.5. Comparison 6 Group SLT (SLTA) versus 1-to-1 SLT (SLTB), Outcome 5 Expressive language:
written.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 6 Group SLT (SLTA) versus 1-to-1 SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 5 Expressive language: written

Study or subgroup

Group SLT

1-to-1 SLT

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

16

72.25 (21.74)

18

78.28 (21.74)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 PICA graphic
Wertz 1981

Subtotal (95% CI)

16

100.0 %

-6.03 [ -20.67, 8.61 ]

100.0 % -6.03 [ -20.67, 8.61 ]

18

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.81 (P = 0.42)
Test for subgroup differences: Not applicable

-50

-25

Favours 1-to-1 SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

25

50

Favours Group SLT

130

Analysis 6.6. Comparison 6 Group SLT (SLTA) versus 1-to-1 SLT (SLTB), Outcome 6 Severity of
impairment: Aphasia Battery Score.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 6 Group SLT (SLTA) versus 1-to-1 SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 6 Severity of impairment: Aphasia Battery Score

Study or subgroup

Group SLT

1-to-1 SLT

Std. Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

30

66.93 (25.62)

24

57.8 (34.81)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Std. Mean Difference


IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 Aphasia Quotient CRRCAE


Jufeng 2005iii

Subtotal (95% CI)

30

51.2 %

0.30 [ -0.24, 0.84 ]

51.2 %

0.30 [ -0.24, 0.84 ]

32.9 %

-0.06 [ -0.73, 0.61 ]

32.9 %

-0.06 [ -0.73, 0.61 ]

15.9 %

0.23 [ -0.74, 1.20 ]

15.9 %

0.23 [ -0.74, 1.20 ]

49

100.0 %

0.17 [ -0.22, 0.56 ]

24

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 1.09 (P = 0.28)
2 PICA overall
Wertz 1981

Subtotal (95% CI)

16

70.69 (24.64)

16

18

72.17 (24.64)

18

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.17 (P = 0.86)
3 AAT overall
Pulvermuller 2001

10

Subtotal (95% CI)

10

55.58 (5.88)

54.14 (6.3)

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.46 (P = 0.65)

Total (95% CI)

56

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 0.68, df = 2 (P = 0.71); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.86 (P = 0.39)
Test for subgroup differences: Chi2 = 0.68, df = 2 (P = 0.71), I2 =0.0%

-2

-1

Favours One-to-one SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Favours Group SLT

131

Analysis 6.7. Comparison 6 Group SLT (SLTA) versus 1-to-1 SLT (SLTB), Outcome 7 Severity of
impairment: Aphasia Battery Score (3-month follow up).
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 6 Group SLT (SLTA) versus 1-to-1 SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 7 Severity of impairment: Aphasia Battery Score (3-month follow up)

Study or subgroup

Group SLT

1-to-1 SLT

Mean(SD)

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

24

37.75 (28.61)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 Aphasia Quotient CRRCAE (3-month follow up)


Jufeng 2005iii

Subtotal (95% CI)

30

71.16 (33.79)

30

100.0 %

24

33.41 [ 16.76, 50.06 ]

100.0 % 33.41 [ 16.76, 50.06 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 3.93 (P = 0.000084)
Test for subgroup differences: Not applicable

-50

-25

Favours One-to-one SLT

25

50

Favours Group SLT

Analysis 6.8. Comparison 6 Group SLT (SLTA) versus 1-to-1 SLT (SLTB), Outcome 8 Number of drop-outs
for any reason.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 6 Group SLT (SLTA) versus 1-to-1 SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 8 Number of drop-outs for any reason

Study or subgroup

Wertz 1981

Total (95% CI)

Group SLT

1-to-1 SLT

Odds Ratio

n/N

n/N

M-H,Fixed,95% CI

Weight

Odds Ratio

17/35

16/32

100.0 %

0.94 [ 0.36, 2.46 ]

35

32

100.0 %

0.94 [ 0.36, 2.46 ]

M-H,Fixed,95% CI

Total events: 17 (Group SLT), 16 (1-to-1 SLT)


Heterogeneity: not applicable
Test for overall effect: Z = 0.12 (P = 0.91)

0.01

0.1

Favours Group

10

100

Favours 1-to-1 SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

132

Analysis 7.1. Comparison 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 1
Functional communication.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 1 Functional communication

Study or subgroup

Task Specific SLT

Conventional SLT

Std. Mean Difference

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Std. Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

IV,Fixed,95% CI

10

2.9 (1.8)

11

3.4 (3.7)

68.4 %

-0.16 [ -1.02, 0.70 ]

van Steenbrugge 1981

3.86 (1.41)

4.8 (2.37)

31.6 %

-0.44 [ -1.70, 0.83 ]

Subtotal (95% CI)

15

100.0 %

-0.25 [ -0.96, 0.46 ]

1 Functional expression
Prins 1989

16

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 0.12, df = 1 (P = 0.73); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.69 (P = 0.49)
Test for subgroup differences: Not applicable

-2

-1

Favours Conventional SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Favours Task Specific SLT

133

Analysis 7.2. Comparison 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 2
Receptive language: auditory comprehension - word.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 2 Receptive language: auditory comprehension - word

Study or subgroup

Task Specific SLT


N

Conventional SLT
Mean(SD)

Std. Mean Difference


Mean(SD)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Std. Mean Difference


IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 Word comprehension (BDAE subtest)


Lincoln 1984b
Prins 1989

Subtotal (95% CI)

6 39.67 (10.89)
10

27.6 (8.5)

16

6 38.17 (10.72)
11

28.4 (6.6)

17

36.4 %

0.13 [ -1.01, 1.26 ]

63.6 %

-0.10 [ -0.96, 0.76 ]

100.0 %

-0.02 [ -0.70, 0.67 ]

100.0 %

-0.22 [ -1.08, 0.64 ]

100.0 %

-0.22 [ -1.08, 0.64 ]

100.0 %

0.13 [ -1.01, 1.26 ]

100.0 %

0.13 [ -1.01, 1.26 ]

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 0.10, df = 1 (P = 0.75); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.05 (P = 0.96)
2 Identify body part (BDAE subtest)
Prins 1989

Subtotal (95% CI)

10

15.2 (4.3)

10

11

16.1 (3.5)

11

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.51 (P = 0.61)
3 Peabody PVT
Lincoln 1984b

Subtotal (95% CI)

6 39.67 (10.89)

6 38.17 (10.72)

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.22 (P = 0.82)

-2

-1

Favours Conventional SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Favours Task Specific SLT

134

Analysis 7.3. Comparison 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 3
Receptive language: other auditory comprehension.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 3 Receptive language: other auditory comprehension

Study or subgroup

Task Specific SLT

Conventional SLT

Std. Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

10

15.3 (5.9)

11

18.4 (5.7)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Std. Mean Difference


IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 Sentence comprehension
Prins 1989

Subtotal (95% CI)

10

11

100.0 %

-0.51 [ -1.39, 0.36 ]

100.0 %

-0.51 [ -1.39, 0.36 ]

100.0 %

0.47 [ -0.51, 1.45 ]

100.0 %

0.47 [ -0.51, 1.45 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 1.15 (P = 0.25)
2 AAT comprehension subtest
Pulvermuller 2001

10

Subtotal (95% CI)

10

60.3 (9.29)

7 55.29 (11.19)

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.94 (P = 0.35)
3 Token Test
Lincoln 1982i

6 67.83 (14.82)

6 60.33 (17.24)

16.1 %

0.43 [ -0.72, 1.58 ]

Lincoln 1982ii

62.5 (25.36)

6 66.33 (14.47)

16.5 %

-0.17 [ -1.31, 0.96 ]

Lincoln 1984b

6 36.83 (21.24)

6 27.67 (17.61)

16.0 %

0.43 [ -0.72, 1.58 ]

Prins 1989

10

5.1 (3.4)

11

6.3 (4.4)

28.6 %

-0.29 [ -1.15, 0.57 ]

Pulvermuller 2001

10

53 (7.24)

54 (8.16)

22.7 %

-0.12 [ -1.09, 0.84 ]

Subtotal (95% CI)

38

100.0 %

0.00 [ -0.46, 0.46 ]

36

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 1.67, df = 4 (P = 0.80); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.01 (P = 1.0)
Test for subgroup differences: Chi2 = 2.19, df = 2 (P = 0.33), I2 =9%

-2

-1

Favours Conventional SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Favours Task Specific SLT

135

Analysis 7.4. Comparison 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 4
Receptive language: auditory comprehension (treated items).
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 4 Receptive language: auditory comprehension (treated items)

Study or subgroup

Task Specific SLT

Conventional SLT

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

10

28.1 (9.3)

11

30.1 (4.9)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 Word comprehension (phonology)


Prins 1989

Subtotal (95% CI)

10

11

100.0 %

-2.00 [ -8.45, 4.45 ]

100.0 %

-2.00 [ -8.45, 4.45 ]

100.0 %

-5.00 [ -22.80, 12.80 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.61 (P = 0.54)
2 Word comprehension (lexicon)
Prins 1989

Subtotal (95% CI)

10

69.4 (21.8)

10

11

74.4 (19.6)

11

100.0 % -5.00 [ -22.80, 12.80 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.55 (P = 0.58)
3 Sentence comprehension (morphosyntax)
Prins 1989

Subtotal (95% CI)

10

78.7 (45.7)

10

11

100.0 %

92.7 (45.1)

11

-14.00 [ -52.89, 24.89 ]

100.0 % -14.00 [ -52.89, 24.89 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.71 (P = 0.48)
Test for subgroup differences: Chi2 = 0.43, df = 2 (P = 0.81), I2 =0.0%

-100

-50

Favours Conventional SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

50

100

Favours Task Specific SLT

136

Analysis 7.5. Comparison 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 5
Receptive language: reading comprehension.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 5 Receptive language: reading comprehension

Study or subgroup

Task Specific SLT

Conventional SLT

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

10

35.9 (12.9)

11

30.9 (14)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 Reading comprehension
Prins 1989

Subtotal (95% CI)

10

100.0 %

5.00 [ -6.51, 16.51 ]

100.0 % 5.00 [ -6.51, 16.51 ]

11

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.85 (P = 0.39)

-20

-10

Favours Conventional SLT

10

20

Favours Task Specific SLT

Analysis 7.6. Comparison 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 6
Receptive language: other.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 6 Receptive language: other

Study or subgroup

Task Specific SLT

Conventional SLT

Mean Difference
Mean(SD)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Lincoln 1982i

12.57 (0.4)

6 12.53 (1.25)

42.2 %

0.04 [ -1.01, 1.09 ]

Lincoln 1982ii

6 12.58 (1.15)

6 13.26 (0.46)

47.4 %

-0.68 [ -1.67, 0.31 ]

Lincoln 1984b

6 11.02 (2.16)

6 10.86 (1.54)

10.3 %

0.16 [ -1.96, 2.28 ]

1 PICA gestural subtest

Subtotal (95% CI)

18

18

100.0 % -0.29 [ -0.97, 0.39 ]

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 1.15, df = 2 (P = 0.56); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.83 (P = 0.41)
Test for subgroup differences: Not applicable

-4

-2

Favours Conventional SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Favours Task Specific SLT

137

Analysis 7.7. Comparison 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 7
Expressive language: spoken naming.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 7 Expressive language: spoken naming

Study or subgroup

Task Specific SLT

Conventional SLT

Std. Mean Difference

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Std. Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Lincoln 1982i

10.5 (6.16)

13.5 (7.53)

16.8 %

-0.40 [ -1.55, 0.75 ]

Lincoln 1982ii

6 12.83 (7.86)

6 17.33 (5.24)

16.1 %

-0.62 [ -1.79, 0.55 ]

13.3 (14.9)

29.8 %

0.27 [ -0.59, 1.13 ]

7 54.14 (7.01)

23.3 %

0.34 [ -0.64, 1.31 ]

14.0 %

0.37 [ -0.89, 1.63 ]

100.0 %

0.04 [ -0.43, 0.51 ]

100.0 %

0.70 [ -0.60, 2.00 ]

100.0 %

0.70 [ -0.60, 2.00 ]

1 Naming

Prins 1989

10

17 (10.8)

Pulvermuller 2001

10

56.5 (6.35)

van Steenbrugge 1981

29.4 (2.3)

Subtotal (95% CI)

37

11

27 (7.97)

35

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 2.70, df = 4 (P = 0.61); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.18 (P = 0.86)
2 Naming (3-week follow up)
van Steenbrugge 1981

Subtotal (95% CI)

31.8 (3.35)

26.6 (8.88)

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 1.05 (P = 0.29)

-2

-1

Favours Conventional SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Favours Task Specific SLT

138

Analysis 7.8. Comparison 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 8
Expressive language: spoken sentence construction.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 8 Expressive language: spoken sentence construction

Study or subgroup

Task Specific SLT

Conventional SLT

Std. Mean Difference

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Std. Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

IV,Fixed,95% CI

10

19 (15.2)

11

17.7 (24.8)

67.7 %

0.06 [ -0.80, 0.92 ]

van Steenbrugge 1981

3.2 (1.3)

3.4 (3.36)

32.3 %

-0.07 [ -1.31, 1.17 ]

Subtotal (95% CI)

15

100.0 %

0.02 [ -0.69, 0.72 ]

100.0 %

-0.25 [ -1.50, 1.00 ]

100.0 %

-0.25 [ -1.50, 1.00 ]

1 Sentence construction (AmAT)


Prins 1989

16

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 0.03, df = 1 (P = 0.86); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.05 (P = 0.96)
2 Sentence construction (AmAT) 3-week follow up
van Steenbrugge 1981

Subtotal (95% CI)

3 (1.58)

3.6 (2.61)

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.39 (P = 0.69)
Test for subgroup differences: Chi2 = 0.14, df = 1 (P = 0.71), I2 =0.0%

-4

-2

Favours Conventional SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Favours Task Specific SLT

139

Analysis 7.9. Comparison 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 9
Expressive language: other spoken tasks.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 9 Expressive language: other spoken tasks

Study or subgroup

Task Specific SLT

Conventional SLT

Std. Mean Difference

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Std. Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Lincoln 1982i

8.83 (5.85)

16.5 (6.06)

47.8 %

-1.19 [ -2.46, 0.08 ]

Lincoln 1982ii

14.5 (12.58)

24 (4.77)

52.2 %

-0.92 [ -2.14, 0.30 ]

1 Word fluency

Subtotal (95% CI)

12

100.0 % -1.05 [ -1.93, -0.17 ]

12

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 0.09, df = 1 (P = 0.77); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 2.34 (P = 0.019)
2 Picture description
Lincoln 1982i

6 38.83 (17.07)

6 30.67 (16.21)

52.8 %

0.45 [ -0.70, 1.61 ]

Lincoln 1982ii

6 36.67 (4.89)

47.2 %

-0.93 [ -2.15, 0.29 ]

100.0 %

-0.20 [ -1.04, 0.64 ]

Subtotal (95% CI)

23 (18.55)

12

12

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 2.61, df = 1 (P = 0.11); I2 =62%


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.47 (P = 0.64)
3 PICA verbal subtest
Lincoln 1982i

11.33 (1.43)

6 10.61 (2.34)

35.4 %

0.34 [ -0.80, 1.49 ]

Lincoln 1982ii

10.39 (2)

6 12.37 (0.95)

28.9 %

-1.17 [ -2.43, 0.10 ]

Lincoln 1984b

5.09 (2.26)

35.7 %

-0.26 [ -1.39, 0.88 ]

100.0 %

-0.31 [ -0.99, 0.37 ]

Subtotal (95% CI)

18

5.61 (1.4)

18

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 3.02, df = 2 (P = 0.22); I2 =34%


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.88 (P = 0.38)
Test for subgroup differences: Chi2 = 2.29, df = 2 (P = 0.32), I2 =13%

-4

-2

Favours Conventional SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Favours Task Specific SLT

140

Analysis 7.10. Comparison 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 10
Expressive language: spoken (treated items).
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 10 Expressive language: spoken (treated items)

Study or subgroup

Task Specific SLT

Conventional SLT

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

van Steenbrugge 1981

35 (5.15)

27 (8.16)

Subtotal (95% CI)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 Naming (treated)
100.0 %

8.00 [ -0.46, 16.46 ]

100.0 % 8.00 [ -0.46, 16.46 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 1.85 (P = 0.064)
2 Sentence construction (treated)
van Steenbrugge 1981

Subtotal (95% CI)

8 (2.74)

4.8 (4.02)

100.0 %

3.20 [ -1.06, 7.46 ]

100.0 % 3.20 [ -1.06, 7.46 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 1.47 (P = 0.14)
3 Naming (treated: 3-week follow up)
van Steenbrugge 1981

Subtotal (95% CI)

33.2 (3.96)

100.0 %

27 (6.36)

6.20 [ -0.37, 12.77 ]

100.0 % 6.20 [ -0.37, 12.77 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 1.85 (P = 0.064)
4 Sentence construction (treated: 3-week follow up)
van Steenbrugge 1981

Subtotal (95% CI)

7.6 (3.78)

3.6 (4.51)

100.0 %

4.00 [ -1.16, 9.16 ]

100.0 % 4.00 [ -1.16, 9.16 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 1.52 (P = 0.13)
Test for subgroup differences: Chi2 = 1.31, df = 3 (P = 0.73), I2 =0.0%

-20

-10

Favours Conventional SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

10

20

Favours Task Specific SLT

141

Analysis 7.11. Comparison 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 11
Expressive language: repetition.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 11 Expressive language: repetition

Study or subgroup

Task Specific SLT

Conventional SLT

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

10

52.5 (4.22)

53.14 (8.23)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 AAT repetition subtest


Pulvermuller 2001

Total (95% CI)

10

100.0 %

-0.64 [ -7.27, 5.99 ]

100.0 % -0.64 [ -7.27, 5.99 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.19 (P = 0.85)
Test for subgroup differences: Not applicable

-10

-5

Favours Conventional SLT

10

Favours Task Specific SLT

Analysis 7.12. Comparison 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 12
Expressive language: written.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 12 Expressive language: written

Study or subgroup

Operant Training SLT

Conventional SLT

Mean Difference

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Lincoln 1982i

7.45 (1.94)

8.21 (1.59)

17.5 %

-0.76 [ -2.77, 1.25 ]

Lincoln 1982ii

7.64 (1.82)

10.22 (1.7)

17.7 %

-2.58 [ -4.57, -0.59 ]

Lincoln 1984b

7.25 (0.55)

7.65 (1.18)

64.8 %

-0.40 [ -1.44, 0.64 ]

1 PICA graphic subtest

Subtotal (95% CI)

18

18

100.0 % -0.85 [ -1.69, -0.01 ]

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 3.62, df = 2 (P = 0.16); I2 =45%


Test for overall effect: Z = 1.98 (P = 0.047)
Test for subgroup differences: Not applicable

-4

-2

Favours Conventional SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Fav Operant Training SLT

142

Analysis 7.13. Comparison 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 13
Severity of impairment.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 13 Severity of impairment

Study or subgroup

Task Specific SLT

Conventional SLT

Mean Difference
Mean(SD)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Lincoln 1982i

10.5 (0.8)

6 10.65 (1.37)

35.1 %

-0.15 [ -1.42, 1.12 ]

Lincoln 1982ii

6 10.45 (1.21)

6 12.07 (0.86)

40.1 %

-1.62 [ -2.81, -0.43 ]

Lincoln 1984b

24.8 %

-0.17 [ -1.68, 1.34 ]

1 PICA overall

Subtotal (95% CI)

8.45 (1.45)

18

8.62 (1.21)

18

100.0 % -0.74 [ -1.50, 0.01 ]

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 3.48, df = 2 (P = 0.18); I2 =43%


Test for overall effect: Z = 1.94 (P = 0.052)
2 AAT overall
Pulvermuller 2001

Subtotal (95% CI)

10 55.58 (5.88)

10

100.0 %

54.14 (6.3)

1.44 [ -4.48, 7.36 ]

100.0 % 1.44 [ -4.48, 7.36 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.48 (P = 0.63)
Test for subgroup differences: Chi2 = 0.51, df = 1 (P = 0.47), I2 =0.0%

-10

-5

Favours Conventional SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

10

Favours Task Specific SLT

143

Analysis 7.14. Comparison 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 14
Number of drop-outs for any reason.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 7 Task-specific SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 14 Number of drop-outs for any reason

Study or subgroup

Task Specific SLT

Conventional SLT

n/N

n/N

Odds Ratio

Weight

Odds Ratio

Shewan 1984i

6/28

1/24

100.0 %

6.27 [ 0.70, 56.40 ]

Total (95% CI)

28

24

100.0 %

6.27 [ 0.70, 56.40 ]

M-H,Random,95% CI

M-H,Random,95% CI

Total events: 6 (Task Specific SLT), 1 (Conventional SLT)


Heterogeneity: not applicable
Test for overall effect: Z = 1.64 (P = 0.10)
Test for subgroup differences: Not applicable

0.001 0.01 0.1

10 100 1000

Favours Task Specific SLT

Favours Conventional SLT

Analysis 8.1. Comparison 8 Operant training SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 1
Receptive language: auditory comprehension.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 8 Operant training SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 1 Receptive language: auditory comprehension

Study or subgroup

Operant Training SLT


N

Conventional SLT
Mean(SD)

Mean Difference
Mean(SD)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 Word comprehension (BDAE subtest)


Lincoln 1984b

Subtotal (95% CI)

6 39.67 (10.89)

6 38.17 (10.72)

6 39.67 (10.89)

6 38.17 (10.72)

100.0 %

1.50 [ -10.73, 13.73 ]

100.0 % 1.50 [ -10.73, 13.73 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.24 (P = 0.81)
2 Peabody PCT
Lincoln 1984b

Subtotal (95% CI)

100.0 %

1.50 [ -10.73, 13.73 ]

100.0 % 1.50 [ -10.73, 13.73 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.24 (P = 0.81)

-50

-25

Favours Conventional SLT

25

50

Fav Operant Training SLT

(Continued . . . )

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

144

(. . .
Study or subgroup

Operant Training SLT


N

Conventional SLT
Mean(SD)

Mean Difference
Mean(SD)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Continued)
Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

3 Token Test
Lincoln 1982i

6 67.83 (14.82)

6 60.33 (17.24)

43.8 %

7.50 [ -10.69, 25.69 ]

Lincoln 1982ii

6 62.5 (25.36)

6 66.33 (14.47)

26.5 %

-3.83 [ -27.19, 19.53 ]

Lincoln 1984b

6 36.83 (21.24)

6 27.67 (17.61)

29.7 %

9.16 [ -12.92, 31.24 ]

Subtotal (95% CI)

18

18

100.0 % 4.99 [ -7.05, 17.02 ]

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 0.76, df = 2 (P = 0.68); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.81 (P = 0.42)
Test for subgroup differences: Chi2 = 0.21, df = 2 (P = 0.90), I2 =0.0%

-50

-25

Favours Conventional SLT

25

50

Fav Operant Training SLT

Analysis 8.2. Comparison 8 Operant training SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 2
Receptive language: other.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 8 Operant training SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 2 Receptive language: other

Study or subgroup

Operant Training SLT

Conventional SLT

Mean Difference
Mean(SD)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Lincoln 1982i

12.57 (0.4)

6 12.53 (1.25)

42.2 %

0.04 [ -1.01, 1.09 ]

Lincoln 1982ii

6 12.58 (1.15)

6 13.26 (0.46)

47.4 %

-0.68 [ -1.67, 0.31 ]

Lincoln 1984b

6 11.02 (2.16)

6 10.86 (1.54)

10.3 %

0.16 [ -1.96, 2.28 ]

1 PICA gestural subtest

Subtotal (95% CI)

18

18

100.0 % -0.29 [ -0.97, 0.39 ]

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 1.15, df = 2 (P = 0.56); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.83 (P = 0.41)
Test for subgroup differences: Not applicable

-4

-2

Favours Conventional SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Fav Operant Training SLT

145

Analysis 8.3. Comparison 8 Operant training SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 3
Expressive language: spoken.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 8 Operant training SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 3 Expressive language: spoken

Study or subgroup

Operant Training SLT

Conventional SLT

Std. Mean Difference

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Std. Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Lincoln 1982i

10.5 (6.16)

13.5 (7.53)

33.6 %

-0.40 [ -1.55, 0.75 ]

Lincoln 1982ii

6 12.83 (7.86)

6 17.33 (5.24)

32.2 %

-0.62 [ -1.79, 0.55 ]

Lincoln 1984b

34.2 %

0.24 [ -0.90, 1.38 ]

100.0 %

-0.25 [ -0.92, 0.41 ]

1 Naming

Subtotal (95% CI)

0.83 (1.6)

18

0.5 (0.84)

18

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 1.16, df = 2 (P = 0.56); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.75 (P = 0.45)
2 Word fluency
Lincoln 1982i

8.83 (5.85)

16.5 (6.06)

47.8 %

-1.19 [ -2.46, 0.08 ]

Lincoln 1982ii

6 14.5 (12.58)

24 (4.77)

52.2 %

-0.92 [ -2.14, 0.30 ]

Subtotal (95% CI)

12

100.0 % -1.05 [ -1.93, -0.17 ]

12

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 0.09, df = 1 (P = 0.77); I2 =0.0%


Test for overall effect: Z = 2.34 (P = 0.019)
3 Picture description
Lincoln 1982i

6 38.83 (17.07)

6 30.67 (16.21)

52.8 %

0.45 [ -0.70, 1.61 ]

Lincoln 1982ii

6 36.67 (4.89)

47.2 %

-0.93 [ -2.15, 0.29 ]

100.0 %

-0.20 [ -1.04, 0.64 ]

Subtotal (95% CI)

23 (18.55)

12

12

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 2.61, df = 1 (P = 0.11); I2 =62%


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.47 (P = 0.64)
4 PICA verbal subtest
Lincoln 1982i

6 11.33 (1.43)

6 10.61 (2.34)

35.4 %

0.34 [ -0.80, 1.49 ]

Lincoln 1982ii

10.39 (2)

6 12.37 (0.95)

28.9 %

-1.17 [ -2.43, 0.10 ]

Lincoln 1984b

5.09 (2.26)

35.7 %

-0.26 [ -1.39, 0.88 ]

100.0 %

-0.31 [ -0.99, 0.37 ]

Subtotal (95% CI)

18

5.61 (1.4)

18

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 3.02, df = 2 (P = 0.22); I2 =34%


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.88 (P = 0.38)
Test for subgroup differences: Chi2 = 2.57, df = 3 (P = 0.46), I2 =0.0%

-4

-2

Favours Conventional SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Fav Operant Training SLT

146

Analysis 8.4. Comparison 8 Operant training SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 4
Expressive language: written.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 8 Operant training SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 4 Expressive language: written

Study or subgroup

Operant Training SLT

Conventional SLT

Mean Difference

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Lincoln 1982i

7.45 (1.94)

8.21 (1.59)

17.5 %

-0.76 [ -2.77, 1.25 ]

Lincoln 1982ii

7.64 (1.82)

10.22 (1.7)

17.7 %

-2.58 [ -4.57, -0.59 ]

Lincoln 1984b

7.25 (0.55)

7.65 (1.18)

64.8 %

-0.40 [ -1.44, 0.64 ]

1 PICA graphic subtest

Subtotal (95% CI)

18

100.0 % -0.85 [ -1.69, -0.01 ]

18

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 3.62, df = 2 (P = 0.16); I2 =45%


Test for overall effect: Z = 1.98 (P = 0.047)
Test for subgroup differences: Not applicable

-4

-2

Favours Conventional SLT

Fav Operant Training SLT

Analysis 8.5. Comparison 8 Operant training SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB), Outcome 5
Severity of impairment.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 8 Operant training SLT (SLTA) versus conventional SLT (SLTB)


Outcome: 5 Severity of impairment

Study or subgroup

Operant Training SLT

Conventional SLT

Mean Difference
Mean(SD)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Lincoln 1982i

10.5 (0.8)

6 10.65 (1.37)

35.1 %

-0.15 [ -1.42, 1.12 ]

Lincoln 1982ii

6 10.45 (1.21)

6 12.07 (0.86)

40.1 %

-1.62 [ -2.81, -0.43 ]

Lincoln 1984b

24.8 %

-0.17 [ -1.68, 1.34 ]

1 PICA overall

Subtotal (95% CI)

8.45 (1.45)

18

8.62 (1.21)

100.0 % -0.74 [ -1.50, 0.01 ]

18

Heterogeneity: Chi2 = 3.48, df = 2 (P = 0.18); I2 =43%


Test for overall effect: Z = 1.94 (P = 0.052)
Test for subgroup differences: Not applicable

-4

-2

Favours Conventional SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Fav Operant Training SLT

147

Analysis 9.1. Comparison 9 Semantic SLT (SLTA) versus phonological SLT (SLT B), Outcome 1 Functional
communication.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 9 Semantic SLT (SLTA) versus phonological SLT (SLT B)


Outcome: 1 Functional communication

Study or subgroup

Semantic SLT

Phonological SLT

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

Doesborgh 2004a

29

29.9 (12)

26

29.5 (11)

Subtotal (95% CI)

29

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 ANELT-A
100.0 %

0.40 [ -5.68, 6.48 ]

100.0 % 0.40 [ -5.68, 6.48 ]

26

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.13 (P = 0.90)

-20

-10

Favours Phonological SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

10

20

Favours Semantic SLT

148

Analysis 9.2. Comparison 9 Semantic SLT (SLTA) versus phonological SLT (SLT B), Outcome 2 Receptive
language: auditory.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 9 Semantic SLT (SLTA) versus phonological SLT (SLT B)


Outcome: 2 Receptive language: auditory

Study or subgroup

Semantic SLT

Phonological SLT

Mean(SD)

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

23

1.6 (4.04)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 Semantic Association Test: PALPA (change from baseline)


Doesborgh 2004a

23

Subtotal (95% CI)

23

2.9 (3.93)

23

100.0 %

1.30 [ -1.00, 3.60 ]

100.0 %

1.30 [ -1.00, 3.60 ]

100.0 %

-3.50 [ -6.23, -0.77 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 1.11 (P = 0.27)
2 Auditory lexical decision: PALPA (change from baseline)
Doesborgh 2004a

23

Subtotal (95% CI)

23

-0.5 (5.32)

23

3 (4.04)

23

100.0 % -3.50 [ -6.23, -0.77 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 2.51 (P = 0.012)

-10

-5

Favours Phonological SLT

10

Favours Semantic SLT

Analysis 9.3. Comparison 9 Semantic SLT (SLTA) versus phonological SLT (SLT B), Outcome 3 Receptive
language: reading.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 9 Semantic SLT (SLTA) versus phonological SLT (SLT B)


Outcome: 3 Receptive language: reading

Study or subgroup

Semantic SLT
N

Phonological SLT
Mean(SD)

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

23

0.1 (5.43)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 Synonym judgement: PALPA (change from baseline)


Doesborgh 2004a

Total (95% CI)

23

1.7 (6.47)

23

100.0 %

1.60 [ -1.85, 5.05 ]

100.0 % 1.60 [ -1.85, 5.05 ]

23

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.91 (P = 0.36)
Test for subgroup differences: Not applicable

-10

-5

Favours Phonological SLT

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

10

Favours Semantic SLT

149

Analysis 9.4. Comparison 9 Semantic SLT (SLTA) versus phonological SLT (SLT B), Outcome 4 Expressive
language: repetition.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 9 Semantic SLT (SLTA) versus phonological SLT (SLT B)


Outcome: 4 Expressive language: repetition

Study or subgroup

Semantic SLT

Phonological SLT

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

1.3 (5.66)

23

3 (3.81)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 Non-words: PALPA (change from baseline)


Doesborgh 2004a

23

Subtotal (95% CI)

23

100.0 %

23

-1.70 [ -4.49, 1.09 ]

100.0 % -1.70 [ -4.49, 1.09 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 1.19 (P = 0.23)

-10

-5

Favours Phonology

10

Favours Semantic

Analysis 9.5. Comparison 9 Semantic SLT (SLTA) versus phonological SLT (SLT B), Outcome 5 Number of
drop-outs for any reason.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 9 Semantic SLT (SLTA) versus phonological SLT (SLT B)


Outcome: 5 Number of drop-outs for any reason

Study or subgroup

Semantic SLT

Phonological SLT

n/N

n/N

6/29

6/29

100.0 %

1.00 [ 0.28, 3.56 ]

29

29

100.0 %

1.00 [ 0.28, 3.56 ]

Doesborgh 2004a

Total (95% CI)

Odds Ratio

Weight

M-H,Fixed,95% CI

Odds Ratio
M-H,Fixed,95% CI

Total events: 6 (Semantic SLT), 6 (Phonological SLT)


Heterogeneity: not applicable
Test for overall effect: Z = 0.0 (P = 1.0)

0.01

0.1

Favours Semantic

10

100

Favours Phonology

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

150

Analysis 10.1. Comparison 10 Filmed programmed instruction SLT (SLT A) versus non-programmed
activity SLT (SLTB), Outcome 1 Receptive language: auditory.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 10 Filmed programmed instruction SLT (SLT A) versus non-programmed activity SLT (SLTB)
Outcome: 1 Receptive language: auditory

Study or subgroup Programme Instruction SLT


N

Non-Prog Instruction SLT

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

7 22.43 (2.76)

21.71 (3.2)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 Word comprehension
Di Carlo 1980

Total (95% CI)

100.0 %

0.72 [ -2.41, 3.85 ]

100.0 % 0.72 [ -2.41, 3.85 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.45 (P = 0.65)
Test for subgroup differences: Not applicable

-10

-5

Fav Non-Prog Inst SLT

10

Fav Prog Inst SLT

Analysis 10.2. Comparison 10 Filmed programmed instruction SLT (SLT A) versus non-programmed
activity SLT (SLTB), Outcome 2 Receptive language: reading.
Review:

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke

Comparison: 10 Filmed programmed instruction SLT (SLT A) versus non-programmed activity SLT (SLTB)
Outcome: 2 Receptive language: reading

Study or subgroup

Programme Instruction SLT

Non-Prog Instruction SLT

Mean Difference

Mean(SD)

Mean(SD)

4.56 (1.31)

4.64 (1.4)

Weight

IV,Fixed,95% CI

Mean Difference
IV,Fixed,95% CI

1 Reading comprehension
Di Carlo 1980

Subtotal (95% CI)

100.0 %

-0.08 [ -1.50, 1.34 ]

100.0 % -0.08 [ -1.50, 1.34 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.11 (P = 0.91)
2 Reading recognition
Di Carlo 1980

Subtotal (95% CI)

4.27 (1.28)

100.0 %

4.37 (1.3)

-0.10 [ -1.45, 1.25 ]

100.0 % -0.10 [ -1.45, 1.25 ]

Heterogeneity: not applicable


Test for overall effect: Z = 0.15 (P = 0.88)
Test for subgroup differences: Chi2 = 0.00, df = 1 (P = 0.98), I2 =0.0%

-4

-2

Fav Non-Prog Instruction

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Fav Prog Instruction SLT

151

ADDITIONAL TABLES
Table 1. Characteristics of participants in included studies

Study ID

Number

Male/female

Bakheit 2007

97

Intensive: 26/25
Intensive:
71.2
Conventional: 21/ (14.9) (range 26 to
25
92)
Conventional: 69.7
(15) (range 17 to
91)

David 1982

133 (of 155 ran- Conventional: 35/ Conventional: 70 Conventional: me- Not reported
domised)
30
(8.7)
dian 4 (range 4 to
Social support: 42/ Social support: 65 266) weeks
26
(10.6)
Social support: median 5 (range 4 to
432) weeks

Denes 1996

17

Intensive: 5/3
Conventional: 3/6

Intensive:
58.1 Intensive: 3.2 (1.8)
(11.8)
months
Conventional: 62.1 Conventional:
3
(8.7)
(1.6) months

AAT
Intensive: severe
Conventional:
severe

Di Carlo 1980

14

Programmed
instruction: 7/0
Non-programmed
instruction: 7/0

Programmed
instruction: 57.6 (9.2)
(range 44 to 69)
Non-programmed
instruction:
55.3 (13) (range 32
to 70)

Programmed
instruction: severe
Non-programmed
instruction: severe

Doesborgh 2004a

58

Semantic: 18/11
Phonologic: 15/14

Semantic: 66 (10)
Semantic:
Phonologic: 58 (14) mean 4 (range 3 to
5) months
Phonologic: mean
4 (range 3 to 5)
months

ANELT-A score
Semantic: 24.8 (11)
Phonologic: 23.3
(8)

Doesborgh 2004b

18
(of
randomised)

Computermediated: 62 (9.0)
No SLT: 65 (12.0)

Computer-mediated: ANELT- A 34
(9); BNT 63 (37)
No SLT: ANELT-A
29 (12); BNT 74

19 Computermediated: 4/4
No SLT: 5/5

Age in years mean Post-onAphasia


sever(standard
set mean (standard ity mean (standard
deviation)
deviation) (range) deviation)

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Intensive:
34.2 WAB scores
(19.1) days
Intensive:
44.2
Conventional: 28.1 (30.2)
(14.9) days
Conventional: 37.9
(27.2)

Programmed
instruction:
24.7 (23.6) (range 0
to 66) months
Non-programmed
instruction:
16.3 (16.9) (range 1
to 38) months

Computermediated: 13 (range
11 to 16) months
No SLT: 13 (range
11 to 17) months

152

Table 1. Characteristics of participants in included studies

(Continued)

(35)
Drummond 1981

Not reported

Gesture cue: 52.9 Gesture cue: 15.3 Not reported


(6.0)
(4.1) (range 10 to
Conventional:
20) months
50.04 (4.5)
Conventional: 17.8
(7.1) (range 9 to 24)
months

Elman 1999

24

Conventional: 7/5
Social support: 6/6

Conventional: 58.3
(11.4) (range 38 to
79)
Social support: 60.7
(10.6) (range 47 to
80)

Conventional: 32.5
(28.7) (range 7 to
103) months
Social support: 71.7
(94.2) (range 7 to
336) months

Conventional:
SPICA 7 mild-moderate, 7 moderate to
severe
Social support: SPICA
7 mild-moderate, 7
moderate to severe

Hinckley 2001

12

Functional SLT: 5/1 Functional:


51.6 Functional:
Conventional SLT: (15)
26.8 (20.1) (range 6
6/0
Conventional: 50.3 to 58) months
(13.6)
Conventional: 26.8
(37.6) (range 4 to
102) months

BDAE Severity Rating


Functional:
2.5
(0.8)
Conventional: 1.83
(0.9)

Jufeng 2005i

60

Group SLT: unclear


No SLT: unclear
(Jufeng 2005: 50/
34)

Group SLT: unclear Unclear


No SLT: unclear
(Jufeng 2005: < 40
years = 3; 40s = 23;
50s = 23; 60s = 25;
70s = 8; > 80 years =
2)

Unclear

Jufeng 2005ii

54

Group SLT: unclear


No SLT: unclear
(Jufeng 2005: 50/
34)

Group SLT: unclear Unclear


No SLT: unclear
(Jufeng 2005: < 40
years = 3; 40s = 23;
50s = 23; 60s = 25;
70s = 8; > 80 years =
2)

Unclear

Jufeng 2005iii

54

Group SLT: unclear


No SLT: unclear
(Jufeng 2005: 50/
34)

Group SLT: unclear Unclear


No SLT: unclear
(Jufeng 2005: < 40
years = 3; 40s = 23;
50s = 23; 60s = 25;
70s = 8; > 80 years =
2)

Unclear

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

153

Table 1. Characteristics of participants in included studies

(Continued)

Katz 1997i

42 (reported data on Computer36)


mediated: unclear
No SLT: unclear
(Katz 1997: 44/11)

Computer-mediated: 61.6 (10)


No SLT: 62.8 (5.1)

ComNot reported
puter-mediated: 6.2
(5.2) years
No SLT: 8.5 (5.4)
years

Katz 1997ii

40
(of
randomised)

Computer-mediated: 61.6 (10)


Computer placebo:
66.4 (6)

Compute-mediNot reported
ated: 6.2 (5.2) years
Computer placebo:
5.4 (4.6) years

Leal 1993

94

Conventional: 38/
21
Volunteerfacilitated: 22/13

Conventional: 56 Within first month Conventional:


(17)
after stroke
moderate-severe
VolunteerVolunteer-facilfacilitated: 59 (13)
itated: moderate-severe

Lincoln 1982i

12

SLT/operant train:
3/3
SLT/Social support:
4/2

SLT/Operant train:
54.33 (6.68) (range
45 to 63)
SLT/Social support:
51.33 (7.97) (range
39 to 63)

SLT/Operant train:
3.17 (1.60) (range 1
to 5) months
SLT/Social support:
5.17 (3.43) (range 1
to 10) months

SLT/Operant train:
moderate
SLT/Social support:
moderate

Lincoln 1982ii

12

Operant train/SLT:
5/1
Social support/SLT:
5/1

Operant train/SLT:
57.67 (5.72) (range
51 to 64)
Social support/SLT:
42.33
(16.91)
(range 28 to 60)

Operant train/SLT:
2.33 (1.55) (range 1
to 5) months
Social support/SLT:
8.83 (13.59) (range
1 to 36) months

Operant train/SLT:
moderate
Social support/SLT:
moderate

Lincoln 1982iii

18

Conventional SLT: Conventional SLT: Conventional SLT:


7/5
52.83 (7.18) (range 4.17 (2.76) (range 1
Social support: 5/1 39 to 63)
to 10)
Social
sup- Social support: 8.83
port: 42.33 (16.91) (13.59) (range 1 to
(range 28 to 60)
36) months

Conventional SLT:
moderate
Social
support:
moderate

Lincoln 1984a
191
(Data for 58% of (of
randomised partici- randomised)
pants)

Lincoln 1984b

12

42 Computermediated: unclear
Computer placebo:
unclear
(Katz 1997: 44/11)

Conventional: un327 clear


No SLT: unclear
(Lincoln
1984a:
109/ 82)

Operant train: 4/2


Placebo: 5/1

Conventional: un- Conventional: 10 Not reported


clear
weeks
No SLT: unclear
No SLT: 10 weeks
Lincoln 1984a: 68.2
(10.2) (range 38 to
92)
Operant
Operant train: 5.5 Operant train: setrain: 52.33 (11.50) (4.89) (range 1 to vere

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

154

Table 1. Characteristics of participants in included studies

(Continued)

(range 32 to 64)
12) months
Placebo: severe
Placebo: 52.5 (14.9) Placebo:
(range 26 to 66)
2.83 (2.32) (range 1
to 7) months
Lyon 1997

30

Functional: unclear
No SLT: unclear
(Lyon 1997: person
with aphasia: 8/2;
caregiver: 4/6; communication partner:
1/9)

Functional: unclear
No SLT: unclear
(Lyon 1997: person with aphasia:
68.6 (12.1) (range
54 to 86); caregiver
60.2 (14.9) (range
28 to 84); communication partner:
44.9 (17.5) (range
25 to 74))

MacKay 1988

95
(of 96 randomised)

MacKay 1988: 46/ MacKay 1988: me- MacKay


1988: Not reported
49
dian 75
mean 30 months

Meikle 1979

31

VolunteerVolunteer-facilifacilitated: 12/3
tated: 67.2 (8.6)
Conventional: 10/6 Conventional: 64.8
(7.9)

Volunteer-facilitated: 30.9
(29.5) (range 4 to
115) weeks
Conventional: 39.8
(69.4) (range 4 to
268) weeks

PICA
percentile
Volunteer-facilitated: 53.9 (23.5)
Conventional: 55.8
(19.78)

Meinzer 2007

20

ConstraintConstraint-ininduced: 7/3 Volun- duced: 50.2 (10.13)


teer-facilitated: 9/1 Volunteerfacilitated: 62 (8.9)

Constrain-induced:
30.7 (18.9) (range 6
to 72) months
Volunteer-facilitated: 46.5
(17.2) (range 24 to
79) months

AAT profile score


Constraintinduced: 5 mild, 3
moderate, 2 severe
Volunteerfacilitated: 3 mild, 6
moderate, 1 severe

ORLA 2006

13

Intensive SLT: 6
Intensive SLT: 61.4
Conventional SLT: (9.72) (range 48.44
7
to 74.5)
Conventional SLT:
53.1 (18.1) (range
31.34 to 77.98).

Intensive SLT: 36.2


(28.2) (range 8.6 to
69.8) months
Conventional SLT:
43.6 (51.1) (range
7.3 to 154) months

WAB Aphasia Quotient Intensive SLT:


51.1 (17.8) (range
28.0 to 69.4)
Conventional SLT: 55.1
(18) (range 34.1 to
77.1)

Prins 1989

21

STADCAP: 5/5
Conventional: 5/6

STADCAP:
15.2 (range 3 to 35)
months
Conventional: 15.2

STADCAP: FE-scale 2.6


(0 to 6), Oral comp
(BDAE and Token

STADCAP: 70.3
(range 58 to 83)
Conventional: 66
(range 45 to 78)

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Functional: unclear
No SLT: unclear
(Lyon 1997: 43.5
(32.2) months)

Functional: unclear
No SLT: unclear
(Lyon 1997: receptive = mild; expressive = moderate)

155

Table 1. Characteristics of participants in included studies

(Continued)

(range 3 to 36) Test) 26.4 (0 to 46)


months
Conventional: FE-scale 2.7
(0 to 9), Oral comp
(BDAE and Token
Test) 29.6 (2 to 48)
Pulvermuller 2001

17

Constraintinduced: 6/4
Conventional: 6/1

Constraint-induced: 55.4 (10.9)


Conventional: 53.9
(7.4)

Constraint-induced: 98.2 (74.2)


months
Conventional: 24
(20.6) months

Constraintinduced: 2 mild, 5
moderate, 3 severe
Conventional:
2
mild, 4 moderate, 1
severe

Rochon 2005

Sentence mapping: Sentence mapping:


0/3
31 to 74
Social support: 0/2 Social support: 32
to 82

Sentence mapping:
(range 2 to 9) years
Social
support:
(range 2 to 4) years

Sentence mapping:
BDAE 1 to 2, phrase
length 2.5 to 4
Social
support: BDAE 1 to
2, phrase length 4

Shewan 1984i

52

Languageorientated: 18/10
Conventional: 14/
10

Language-orientated: 62.18 (range


29 to 82)
Conventional: 65.63 (range
48 to 85)

Languageorientated: (range 2
to 4) weeks
Conventional:
(range 2 to 4 weeks)

Languageorientated: 9 mild, 6
moderate, 13 severe
Conventional:
8 mild, 3 moderate,
13 severe

Shewan 1984ii

53

Languageorientated: 18/10
Social support: 14/
11

Language-orientated: 62.18 (range


29 to 82)
Social
support:
66.12 (range 39 to
82)

Languageorientated: (range 2
to 4) weeks
Social
support:
(range 2 to 4) weeks

Languageorientated: 9 mild, 6
moderate, 13 severe
Social support: 7
mild, 5 moderate,
13 severe

Shewan 1984iii

49

Conventional: 14/ Conven10


tional: 65.63 (range
Social support: 14/ 48 to 85)
11
Social
support:
66.12 (range 39 to
82)

Conventional:
(range 2 to 4) weeks
Social
support:
(range 2 to 4) weeks

Conventional:
8 mild, 3 moderate,
13 severe
Social support: 7
mild, 5 moderate,
13 severe

Smania 2006

33
(of
randomised)

Conventional: 17.4
(24.07) (range 2 to
36) months
No
SLT:
10.39 (7.96) (range
3 to 32) months

Aphasia
severity:
unclear
Neurological severity:
Conventional: 6.07
(4.3) (range 0 to16)
No SLT: 6.94 (5.83)

41 Conventional: 11/4
No SLT: 12/6

Conventional:
65.73 (8.78) (range
48 to 77)
No
SLT:
65.67 (9.83) (range
41 to 77)

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

156

Table 1. Characteristics of participants in included studies

(Continued)

(range 0 to 15)
Smith 1981i

33

Intensive: 12/4
No SLT: 10/7

Intensive: 62
No SLT: 65

Not reported

MTDDA (mean error score percentage)


Intensive: 39
No SLT: 26

Smith 1981ii

31

Conventional: 10/4
No SLT : 10/7

Conventional: 63
No SLT: 65

Not reported

MTDDA (mean error score percentage)


Conventional: 44
No SLT: 26

Smith 1981iii

30

Intensive: 12/4
Intensive: 62
Conventional: 10/4 Conventional: 63

Not reported

MTDDA (mean error score percentage)


Intensive: 39
Conventional: 44

van Steenbrugge
1981

10

Task-specific: 0/5
Conventional: 2/3

Task-specific: 61.8
(17.05) (range 40 to
77)
Conventional: 63.6
(10.9) (range 48 to
77)

Task-specific:
21 (22.4) (range 5 to
60) months
Conventional: 20.6
(23.7) (range 5 to
60) months

FEscale and M-S Comprehension Test


Task-specific:
4
(1.9)
Conventional:
6
(2.9)

Wertz 1981

67

Not reported

(15 weeks after Group SLT: 4 weeks


stroke)
Conventional:
4
Group SLT: 60.24 weeks
(range 40 to 79)
Conventional: 57.07 (range
41 to 79)

(15 weeks after


stroke)
PICA overall percentile
Group SLT: 45.21
(range 15 to 74)
Conventional: 45.62 (range
16 to 74)

Wertz 1986i

78

Conventional: un- Conventional: 59.2 Conventional: 6.6


clear
(6.7)
(4.8) weeks
No SLT: unclear
No SLT: 57.2 (6.8) No SLT: 7.8 (6.6)
weeks

PICA
overall percentile Conventional:
46.59
(16.05)
No SLT: 49.18
(19.46)

Wertz 1986ii

83

Volunteerfacilitated: 37/6
No SLT: unclear

PICA overall percentile


Volunteerfacilitated: 49.97

Volunteer-facilitated: 60.2 (6.7)


No SLT: 57.2 (6.8)

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Volunteerfacilitated: 7.1 (5.8)


weeks
No SLT: 7.8 (6.6)

157

Table 1. Characteristics of participants in included studies

(Continued)

weeks

Wertz 1986iii

81

Volunteerfacilitated: 37/6
Conventional: unclear

Volunteer-facilitated:60.2 (6.7)
Conventional: 59.2
(6.7)

Wu 2004

236

Conventional: unclear
No SLT: unclear
(Wu 2004: 159/ 77)

Conventional:
Not reported
(range 39 to 81)
No SLT: (range 40
to 78)

(22.77)
No SLT:
(19.46)

Volunteerfacilitated: 7.1 (5.8)


weeks
Conventional: 6.6
(4.8) weeks

49.18

PICA overall percentile


Volunteerfacilitated: 49.97
(22.77)
Conventional:
46.59 (16.05)
Not reported

AAT: Aachen Aphasia Test


ANELT: Amsterdam-Nijmegen Everyday Language Test
BDAE: Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination
BNT: Boston Naming Test
FE-scale: Functional-Expression scale
MTDDA: Minnesota Test for the Differential Diagnosis of Aphasia
PICA: Porch Index of Communicative Abilities
SLT: speech and language therapy/therapist
SPICA: Shortened Porch Index of Communicative Abilities
STACDAP: systematic therapy for auditory comprehension disorders in aphasic patients
WAB: Western Aphasia Battery

Table 2. Details of drop-outs

Study ID

Intervention

Reasons

Follow up

Bakheit 2007

Intensive: 16
Conventional: 8

Intensive: 2 died, 14 with- Intensive: 4


drew
Conventional: 3
Conventional: 8 withdrew
(Across trial: withdrew 13,
died 4, illness 4, not tolerating therapy 3, relocation
2, further stroke 1, diagnosis revised 1)

Not reported

David 1982

Conventional: 23
Social support: 36

Conventional: 4 died; 5 Conventional: 11


new stroke; 2 self dis- Social support: 12
charge; 5 illness , 3 moved,
4 other
Social support: 6 died, 5

Not reported

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Reasons

158

Table 2. Details of drop-outs

(Continued)

new stroke, 5 transport,


6 self-discharge, 3 illness,
4 volunteer issues, 2 relocated, 5 other undescribed
Doesborgh 2004a

Semantic: 6
Phonological: 6

Semantic: 4 received less No follow up


than 40 hours treatment,
2 severe neurological illness
Phonological: 2 received
less than 40 hours treatment, 1 severe neurological illness, 3 ANELT score
missing (2 refused, 1 missing)

Doesborgh 2004b

Computer-mediated: 1
No SLT: 0

Computer-mediated:
illness
No SLT: 0

Elman 1999

Conventional: 2
Social support: 3

Conventional: 1 trans- Conventional: 0


port, 1 time constraints, 1 Social support: 0
medical complications
Social support: 2 time
constraints

Katz 1997i

Computer-mediated: 0
No SLT: 6

Prolonged illness, new Computer-mediated: 0


stroke, death
No SLT: 0

Katz 1997ii

Computer-mediated: 0
Prolonged illness, new Computer-mediated: 0
No
SLT
(computer stroke, death
No
SLT
(computer
placebo): 2
placebo): 0

Leal 1993

Conventional: 21
Volunteer-facilitated: 13

Lincoln 1982i

Social support: ? Operant Homesickness, illness


training: ?
(13: groups unclear)

No follow up

Lincoln 1982ii

Social support: ? Operant Homesickness, illness


training: ?

No follow up

1 No follow up

Conventional: 2 death, 3 Conventional: 0


new stroke, 3 transport; 4 Volunteer-facilitated: 0
refused, 2 moved, 5 illness,
2 transfer
Volunteerfacilitated: 1 death, 1 new
stroke, 3 transport, 4 refused, 2 moved, 0 illness,
2 transfer

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

159

Table 2. Details of drop-outs

(Continued)

(13: groups unclear)


Lincoln 1982iii

Social support: ? Operant Homesickness, illness


training: ?
(13: groups unclear)

No follow up

Lincoln 1984a

Conventional: 78
No SLT: 79

Death, refused, illness, re- No follow up


covered, unsuitable, relocated

MacKay 1988

Volunteer-facilitated: 0
No SLT: 1

Reason not reported

Meikle 1979

Conventional: 0
Volunteer-facilitated: 2

Conventional: 0
No follow up
Volunteer-facilitated: 1 refused, 1 moved

Shewan 1984i

Language orientated: 6
Conventional: 1

Language orientated: 1 No follow up


death, 2 relocation, 3
withdrew
Conventional: 1 death

Shewan 1984ii

Language orientated: 6
Social support: 6

Language orientated: 1 No follow up


death, 2 relocation, 3
withdrew
Social support: 1 death,
2 illness, 1 relocation, 2
withdrew

Shewan 1984iii

Conventional: 1
Social support: 6

Conventional: 1 death
No follow up
Social support: 1 death,
2 illness, 1 relocation, 2
withdrew

Smania 2006

Conventional: 5
No SLT: 3

Conventional: 3 unco-op- Conventional: 7


erative, 2 illness
No SLT: 9
No SLT: 1 unco-operative,
2 illness

Conventional: 3 illness, 4
refused
No SLT: 1 death, 2 illness,
4 refused, 2 relocated

Smith 1981i

Intensive: 6
No SLT: not reported

Reasons not detailed


Intensive: 4
Additional 5 withdrawn No SLT: not reported
but not advised of groupings

Not reported

Smith 1981ii

Conventional: 2
No SLT: not reported

Reasons not detailed


Conventional: 4
Additional 5 withdrawn No SLT: not reported
but not advised of groupings

Not reported

No follow up

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

160

Table 2. Details of drop-outs

(Continued)

Smith 1981iii

Intensive: 6
Conventional: 2

Reasons not detailed


Intensive: 4
Additional 5 withdrawn Conventional: 4
but not advised of groupings

Not reported

Wertz 1981

Group: 17
Conventional: 16

22 self-discharged (return No follow up


home or declined to
travel), 4 illness, 2 stroke,
3 died, 2 returned to work

Wertz 1986i

Conventional: 7
No SLT: 5

Illness, new stroke

Conventional: 2
No SLT: 6

Illness, new stroke

Wertz 1986ii

Volunteer-facilitated: 6
No SLT: 5

Illness, new stroke

Volunteer-facilitated: 1
No SLT: 6

Illness, new stroke

Wertz 1986iii

Conventional: 7
Volunteer-facilitated: 6

Illness, new stroke

Conventional: 2
Volunteer-facilitated: 1

Illness, new stroke

ANELT: Amsterdam-Nijmegen Everyday Language Test


SLT: speech and language therapy

APPENDICES
Appendix 1. MEDLINE search strategy
1. exp aphasia/
2. language disorders/ or anomia/
3. (aphasi$ or dysphasi$ or anomia or anomic).tw.
4. ((language or linguistic) adj5 (disorder$ or impair$ or problem$ or dysfunction)).tw.
5. 1 or 2 or 3 or 4
6. language therapy/ or speech therapy/
7. Speech-Language Pathology/
8. ((speech or language or aphasia or dysphasia) adj5 (therap$ or train$ or rehabilitat$ or treat$ or remediat$ or pathol$)).tw.
9. remedial therap$.tw.
10. 6 or 7 or 8 or 9
11. 5 and 10
12. exp aphasia/rh, th or language disorders/rh, th or anomia/rh, th
13. 11 or 12
14. Randomized Controlled Trials/
15. random allocation/
16. Controlled Clinical Trials/
17. control groups/
18. clinical trials/
19. double-blind method/
Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)
Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

161

20. single-blind method/


21. Multicenter Studies/
22. Therapies, Investigational/
23. Research Design/
24. Program Evaluation/
25. evaluation studies/
26. randomized controlled trial.pt.
27. controlled clinical trial.pt.
28. clinical trial.pt.
29. multicenter study.pt.
30. evaluation studies.pt.
31. random$.tw.
32. (controlled adj5 (trial$ or stud$)).tw.
33. (clinical$ adj5 trial$).tw.
34. ((control or treatment or experiment$ or intervention) adj5 (group$ or subject$ or patient$)).tw.
35. (quasi-random$ or quasi random$ or pseudo-random$ or pseudo random$).tw.
36. ((multicenter or multicentre or therapeutic) adj5 (trial$ or stud$)).tw.
37. ((control or experiment$ or conservative) adj5 (treatment or therapy or procedure or manage$)).tw.
38. ((singl$ or doubl$ or tripl$ or trebl$) adj5 (blind$ or mask$)).tw.
39. (coin adj5 (flip or flipped or toss$)).tw.
40. latin square.tw.
41. versus.tw.
42. (assign$ or alternate or allocat$ or counterbalance$ or multiple baseline).tw.
43. controls.tw.
44. or/14-43
45. 13 and 44
46. child$.ti.
47. 45 not 46

Appendix 2. CINAHL search strategy


1. aphasia/ or aphasia, broca/ or aphasia, wernicke/
2. Language Disorders/
3. (aphasi$ or dysphasi$ or anomia or anomic).tw.
4. ((language or linguistic) adj5 (disorder$ or impair$ or problem$ or dysfunction)).tw.
5. 1 or 2 or 3 or 4
6. rehabilitation, speech and language/ or alternative and augmentative communication/ or language therapy/ or exp speech,
alaryngeal/ or speech therapy/
7. Speech-Language Pathology/ or communication skills training/
8. Speech-Language Pathologists/
9. ((speech or language or aphasia or dysphasia) adj5 (therap$ or train$ or rehabilitat$ or treat$ or pathol$)).tw.
10. 6 or 7 or 8 or 9
11. 5 and 10
12. language disorders/rh, th or aphasia/rh, th or aphasia, broca/rh, th or aphasia, wernicke/rh, th
13. 11 or 12
14. random assignment/
15. random sample/
16. Crossover design/
17. exp Clinical trials/
18. Comparative studies/
19. control (research)/
20. Control group/
Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)
Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

162

21. Factorial design/


22. quasi-experimental studies/
23. Nonrandomized trials/
24. Clinical nursing research/ or Clinical research/
25. Community trials/ or Experimental studies/ or One-shot case study/ or Pretest-posttest design/ or Solomon four-group design/ or
Static group comparison/ or Study design/
26. clinical trial.pt.
27. random$.tw.
28. ((singl$ or doubl$ or tripl$ or trebl$) adj25 (blind$ or mask$)).tw.
29. (cross?over or control$ or factorial or sham?).tw.
30. ((clin$ or intervention$ or compar$ or experiment$ or therapeutic) adj10 trial$).tw.
31. (assign$ or alternate or allocat$ or counterbalance$ or multiple baseline$ or ABAB design$).tw.
32. ((control or treatment or experiment$ or intervention) adj5 (group$ or subject$ or patient$)).tw.
33. or/14-32
34. 13 and 33
35. 34 not child$.ti.

Appendix 3. Speech and Language Therapy approaches

Type of SLT

Speech and Language Therapy

Study ID

Conventional

Any form of targeted practice tasks or methodologies that aim to maximise the understanding
and production of language and communication
abilities across spoken and written modalities.
Generally conducted on a patient-therapist (1to-1) basis and using stimulation-facilitation approaches

Bakheit 2007; David 1982; Denes 1996; Di


Carlo 1980; Elman 1999; Drummond 1981;
Hinckley 2001; (Jufeng 2005ii; Jufeng 2005iii)
; Leal 1993; Lincoln 1982i; Lincoln 1984a;
Lincoln 1984b; Meikle 1979; Prins 1989;
Pulvermuller 2001; Shewan 1984i; Shewan
1984iii; Smania 2006; Smith 1981i; Smith
1981ii; Smith 1981iii; van Steenbrugge 1981;
Wertz 1981; Wertz 1986i; Wertz 1986iii; (Wu
2004)

Computer-mediated

Targeted practice tasks or methodologies that Doesborgh 2004b; Katz 1997i; Katz 1997ii;
aim to improve a patients language or commu- ORLA 2006
nication abilities but that are accessed via a computer program

Constraint-induced

Participants required to use spoken communica- Meinzer 2007; Pulvermuller 2001


tion alone
Other communicative methods such as gesture
are not encouraged or permitted

Functional

Targets improvement in communication tasks Denes 1996; Elman 1999; Hinckley 2001; Lyon
considered to be useful in day-to-day function- 1997
ing

Gestural cueing

Use of gesture as a cue to facilitate word-finding Drummond 1981 (AMERIND)

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

163

(Continued)

Group

A SLT intervention involving 2 or more partici- Elman 1999; Jufeng 2005i; Jufeng 2005iii;
pants with aphasia
Wertz 1981

Intensive

At least 4 or more hours of therapeutic interven- Bakheit 2007; Denes 1996; ORLA 2006; Smith
tion each week
1981i; Smith 1981iii

Language-orientated

Follows psycholinguistic principles

Operant training

Not a widely practiced approach to SLT but it Lincoln 1984a; Lincoln 1982i
is a verbal conditioning procedure with the purpose (in the examples included in this review) of
improving communication skills

Phonological treatment

Focuses on improving the sound structure of lan- Doesborgh 2004a


guage
Therapy is directed at the phonological input
and output routes

Semantic treatment

Focuses on interpretation of language with the Doesborgh 2004a


aim of improving semantic processing

Sentence mapping

Targets the mapping between the meaning and Rochon 2005


syntactic structure of sentences

Task-specific

Therapy focused on specific areas of communi- Prins 1989 (STACDAP); Rochon 2005 (Sencation impairment
tence Mapping Therapy); van Steenbrugge
1981 (naming and sentence construction);
Drummond 1981 (word finding); constraintinduced therapy (Meinzer 2007; Pulvermuller
2001)

Volunteer-facilitated (trained)

Targeted practice tasks or methodologies that Leal 1993; MacKay 1988; Meikle 1979; Meinzer
aim to improve a patients language or commu- 2007; Wertz 1986ii; Wertz 1986iii
nication abilities but delivered by a volunteer
Training, material and intervention plans are
usually provided to support the volunteer

Social support and stimulation

An intervention which provides social support or Elman 1999; David 1982; Rochon 2005;
stimulation but does not include targeted inter- Shewan 1984ii; Shewan 1984iii
ventions that aim to resolve participants expressive/receptive speech and language impairments

Programmed instruction

Behavioural intervention that employs a book or Di Carlo 1980


film to present materials for learning
Participants can progress through the tasks at
their own pace, using queries to test their new
learning
Progression to the next stage only occurs once

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Shewan 1984i; Shewan 1984ii

164

(Continued)

they have been successful at an earlier stage


Placebo

An intervention that mimics the experimental


intervention in nature but does not have components that aim to resolve or improve participants
expressive/receptive speech and language skills

Di Carlo 1980 (non-programmed activity); Katz


1997ii (arcade-style games: non-language computer based); Lincoln 1982i (attention non-specific); Lincoln 1984b (non-specific placebo)

Appendix 4. Assessments

Name of assessment

Abbreviation

Reference

Aachen Aphasia Test

AAT

Huber 1984

Affect Balance Scale

ABS

Bradburn 1969

Amsterdam Aphasia Test

AmAT

Prins 1980; Vermeulen 1979

Amsterdam-Nijmegen Everyday Language ANELT-A


Test-A

Blomert 1994

Auditory Comprehension Test for Sen- ACTS


tences

Shewan 1979

Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination

BDAE

Goodglass 1972 and Goodglass 1983

Boston Naming Test

BNT

Kaplan 1983

Caplan and Hanna Sentence Production CHSPT


Test

Caplan 1998

Chinese Rehabilitation Research Centre CRRCAE


Aphasia Examination

Reference unavailable

Communicative Abilities of Daily Living

CADL

Holland 1980; Holland 1998

Communicative Activity Log

CAL

Pulvermuller 2001

Communicative Effectiveness Index

CETI

Lomas 1989

Communicative Readiness and Use Scale and Psychological Wellbeing Index

Lyon 1997

Conversational Rating Scale

CRS

Wertz 1981

Functional Communication Profile

FCP

Sarno 1969

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

165

(Continued)

Functional-Expression scale

FE Scale

Prins 1980

General Health Questionnaire

GHQ

Goldberg 1972

Leal 1993 Aphasia Quotient

AQ

Castro-Caldas 1979

Minnesota Test for Differential Diagnosis MTDDA


of Aphasia

Schuell 1965

Multiple Adjective Affect CheckList

MAACL

Zuckerman 1965

Object Naming Test

ONT

Oldfield 1965

Philadelphia Comprehension Battery

PCB

Saffran 1988

Picture Description with Structured Mod- PDSM


eling

Fink 1994

Porch Index of Communicative Abilities

Porch 1967; Porch 1971; Porch 1981

PICA

Psycholinguistic Assessments of Language PALPA


Processing in Aphasia

Kay 1992; Bastiaanse 1995

Reading Comprehension Battery for Apha- RCBA


sia

LaPointe 1979

Semantic Association Test

Visch-Brink 1996

SAT

Token Test (shortened and standard ver- TT


sions)

DeRenzi 1962; Spreen 1969; Lincoln 1979

Western Aphasia Battery

Kertesz 1982

WAB

Western Aphasia Battery Aphasia Quotient WABAQ

Kertesz 1982

Word Fluency

Borkowski 1967

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

166

WHATS NEW
Last assessed as up-to-date: 8 November 2009.

Date

Event

Description

18 May 2010

Amended

Contact details updated.

HISTORY
Protocol first published: Issue 4, 1997
Review first published: Issue 4, 1999

Date

Event

Description

15 December 2008

New search has been performed

This is a major revision of the original review, which


was first published in 1999, and involves the use of
a new search strategy, amended objectives and refined
inclusion criteria for studies, types of interventions
and outcome measures of interest. Full details of the
amendments are listed in the Background section of
the review.
We have included 20 new trials, bringing the total
number of included studies to 30, involving 1840 participants

12 December 2008

New citation required but conclusions have not This update has been completed by a different team of
changed
authors

24 July 2008

Amended

Converted to new review format.

CONTRIBUTIONS OF AUTHORS
HK conducted the search, screened and retrieved references for inclusion or exclusion, contacted relevant authors and academic
institutions, obtained translations for non-English publications, obtained unpublished data, extracted the data from included trials,
evaluated methodological quality, entered data into RevMan, conducted data analysis, interpreted the data and co-wrote the review.
MB designed the review, retrieved references, screened references for inclusion and exclusion criteria and contributed to discussions
relating to these decisions, contacted relevant authors, obtained translations for non-English publications, obtained unpublished data,
extracted data from included trials, evaluated methodological quality, entered and analysed the data, interpreted the data and co-wrote
the review.
PE co-authored the original review and commented on the updated review.

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

167

DECLARATIONS OF INTEREST
Helen Kelly is a speech and language therapist.
Marian Brady is a speech and language therapist, member of the Royal College of Speech and Language Therapists, and is registered
with the Health Professions Council, UK.
Pam Enderby has been involved in two studies included in this review. She did not contribute to the assessment or interpretation of
either of these studies.

SOURCES OF SUPPORT
Internal sources
Nursing, Midwifery and Allied Health Professions Research Unit, UK.
Queen Margaret University, Edinburgh, UK.

External sources
Chief Scientist Office Scotland, UK.

INDEX TERMS
Medical Subject Headings (MeSH)

Language Therapy; Speech Therapy; Aphasia [ etiology; therapy]; Randomized Controlled Trials as Topic; Stroke [ complications]

MeSH check words


Humans

Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke (Review)


Copyright 2010 The Cochrane Collaboration. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

168

You might also like