You are on page 1of 11

Personality and Individual Differences 49 (2010) 554564

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

Review

A comprehensive meta-analysis of the relationship between


Emotional Intelligence and health q
Alexandra Martins a,*, Nelson Ramalho a, Estelle Morin b
a
ISCTE Lisbon University Institute, Cacifo 035, Av. Forcas Armadas, 1649-026 Lisbon, Portugal
b
HEC Montral, Service de lenseignement du management, 3000, ch. Cte Ste-Catherine, Montral, Qubec, Canada H3T 2A7

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: In everyday life, people have the notion that acknowledging and dealing effectively with emotions con-
Received 28 October 2009 tributes to their wellbeing. A recent meta-analysis by Schutte, Malouff, Thorsteinsson, Bhullar, and Rooke
Received in revised form 4 May 2010 (2007) indicated that Emotional Intelligence (EI) is associated with better health. Our purpose is to
Accepted 10 May 2010
expand their work by including: (1) studies published after the date considered by them; (2) non-English
Available online 19 June 2010
studies; and (3) a cumulative meta-analysis to check for the sufciency and stability in the history of this
research domain. Based on 105 effect sizes and 19,815 participants, the results globally support previous
Keywords:
ndings. When measured as a trait, EI was more strongly associated with health (r = .34) than when it
Emotional intelligence
Health
was measured as an ability (r = .17). The weighted average association with mental (r = .36) and psycho-
Meta-analysis somatic health (r = .33) was higher, than the association with physical health (r = .27). Within the trait
Cumulative meta-analysis approach, the TEIQue showed the strongest association with mental health (r = .50), followed by the
EQ-i (r = .44), SEIS (r = .29) and TMMS (r = .24). Furthermore, the cumulative meta-analysis indicated that
this line of research has already reached sufciency and stability. Overall, the results are encouraging
regarding the value of EI as a plausible health predictor.
2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction either as a test of maximum performance (e.g., MSCEIT Mayer-


Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test) or as a self-report ques-
In everyday life, people have the notion that acknowledging and tionnaire (e.g., TEIQue Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire),
dealing effectively with emotions contributes to their wellbeing. respectively. The lack of consensus endorsed the proliferation of
On the other hand, ignoring them or not dealing with them prop- many different instruments to measure this new construct, making
erly can deteriorate their welfare, especially if it happens on a reg- it difcult to take condent conclusions about EIs real value and
ular basis. For example, the perspective of an exam or an interview impact. One valuable way to put together disperse results from dif-
can make us feel anxious and if we are not able to nd ways to deal ferent studies is using meta-analysis, a rigorous quantitative ap-
with these emotions, we might end up feeling truly ill. proach which refers to the statistical integration of the results of
Being able to recognize what we and other people feel, and nd- independent studies, leading to conclusions that are more precise
ing ways to deal with those emotions is an important facet of what and more reliable than can be derived in any one primary study
psychologists generally call Emotional Intelligence (EI). After al- or in a narrative review (Johnson, Mullen, & Salas, 1995; Rosenthal
most 20 years of research in the eld of EI, doubts still exist about & DiMatteo, 2001).
its conceptualization and relevance in different life domains. Three important efforts have been made in this direction in EIs
Considered either as a set of interrelated abilities (e.g., Mayer & domain. Van Rooy and Viswesvaran (2004) used this approach to
Salovey, 1997) or as a constellation of emotional self-perceptions analyze EIs construct validity and Van Rooy, Viswesvaran, and
within the lower levels of personality hierarchies (Petrides, Pita & Pluta (2005) meta-analyzed its predictive validity concerning
Kokkinaki, 2007), EI has received divergent operationalisations, performance. More recently, Schutte et al. (2007) also used this
approach to examine the relationship between EI and health, an
area that had not received specic attention in the previous
q
This paper is based on a presentation held at the II Emotional Intelligence meta-analytic studies. Their undertaking showed that higher EI is
International Congress in September 16th 2009 at Santander (Spain).
linked with better health. However, since then, more studies have
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +351 917426632.
E-mail addresses: alexandra_martins@iscte.pt, alexmmartins@sapo.pt (A. Mar- been published that would be includable in the analysis of this
tins). relationship, as well as non-English studies (e.g., Spanish),

0191-8869/$ - see front matter 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.05.029
A. Martins et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 49 (2010) 554564 555

available both before and after their work. This has the advantage drawer, because they did not show signicant results (p > .05)
of increasing the sample size and, therefore, the statistical power of and which could threaten the overall conclusions. In other words,
the meta-analysis, besides doing justice to those studies (Johnson, it gives an approximation of the ndings resistance to the le
Scott-Sheldon, Snyder, Noar, & Huedo-Medina, 2008). Further- drawer problem (Rosenthal & DiMatteo, 2001). Without this proce-
more, to our knowledge, a cumulative meta-analysis has not yet dure it would lead to an overestimate of the number of signicant
been performed in this area. Mullen, Muellerleile, and Bryant results.
(2001) dened cumulative meta-analysis as the procedure of per-
forming a (new) meta-analysis at every point during the history 2.2. Literature search
of a research domain (pp. 1451). This procedure addresses the
questions of sufciency and stability in a specic area. The rst Using all the standard literature search techniques, an exhaus-
one indicates whether a certain phenomenon is already estab- tive search was conducted for studies examining the link between
lished or needs additional studies and the second one indicates emotional intelligence and health. Specically ABI/INFORM Global,
whether new studies would change the existing ndings. This Academic Search Complete, Business Source Complete, Economia Y
would give researchers the notion that more investigation is re- Negocios, ERIC, Fuente Acadmica, Medline, Academic Search Alumni
quired in order to test the relationship between EI and health. Edition, Business Source Alumni Edition, PsychArticles, and Google
In summary, the purpose of this paper is to expand the ndings Scholar, were searched using the following keywords individually
of Schutte et al.s (2007) work in three ways: (1) by including stud- and combined, in English, Portuguese, Spanish and French: emo-
ies published after their meta-analysis; (2) by including non-Eng- tional intelligence, health, depression, anxiety, burnout and personal-
lish studies that became available both before and after their ity disorders. These computer searches were supplemented by
meta-analysis; and (3) by performing a cumulative meta-analysis. ancestry searches (scrutinizing the reference sections of relevant
studies that have already been retrieved to locate earlier relevant
studies) and descendency searches (scrutinizing Social Science
2. Method
Citation Index to retrieve subsequent relevant studies that have ci-
ted earlier relevant studies), and browsing through the past
Since there are two distinct methods to measure EI (ability-
19 years of social psychology journals.
based vs. personality-like trait) and so many different instruments,
Studies were included if they met the following criteria: (1)
especially in the last case, it is important to investigate their rela-
Studies that reported, or allowed the precise reconstruction of
tive value as a health predictor. Therefore, two separate meta-anal-
a precise statistical test of the link between EI and Health.
yses were conducted: one considering the two distinct methods as
Therefore, studies that simply reported that a non-signicant
different tasks (ability vs. trait) and another considering the spe-
or signicant effect was found, without providing the statistical
cic instrument used as different tasks (e.g., EQ-i).
test, data with which the statistical test could be reconstructed
In order to compare the results with those found by Schutte
or a precise probability value were excluded. (2) Studies that
et al. (2007), health was also categorized in three different types
used adult or adolescent participants (11 years and above). (3)
(physical, psychosomatic, and mental), using the same criteria.
Studies that reported using at least one of the three relevant
Studies that used measures related to mental disorders (e.g.,
dependent measures (i.e., mental health, physical health or psy-
depression) were integrated in the mental health category, those
chosomatic health). (4) Studies that used predictors specically
that assessed physical (medical) symptoms were classied as
referred to as EI tests, precluding those that used other, although
physical health (e.g., bodily pain) and studies that mixed both
related constructs (e.g., social intelligence). (5) Studies that mea-
kinds of indicators were classied in the psychosomatic category
sured at least three or four dimensions of EI (even when using
(e.g., general health measures). Therefore, three separate meta-
short versions). Therefore, studies that simply used one or two
analyses were conducted to assess the relationship between EI
subscales of a complete measure of EI were not included (e.g.,
and each health category.
studies that simply measured Emotional Perception or Emotional
When analyzing the studies, whenever multiple measures for
Management).
the same variable were used (e.g., a measure of depression and a
As a result, the new literature search produced a total of 46
measure of anxiety to assess mental health; or two different trait
includable additional reports available as of January 2010, result-
questionnaires to measure EI) effect sizes were averaged to avoid
ing in further k = 63 hypothesis tests for the relationship between
biasing the results by deriving too many effect sizes from the same
EI and health. Adding these to the studies included in the original
sample. However, when there was an ability test and a trait ques-
research (Schutte et al., 2007) 35 studies, 44 effect sizes, 7898
tionnaire to measure EI in the same study, only one of the effect
participants we assembled a total of 80 studies and 105 hypoth-
sizes was considered in order to avoid mixing both constructs.
esis tests, in the present paper. The total sample included 19,815
The criterion used here consisted in selecting the operationalisa-
participants, with reported mean ages between 15 and 53 years.
tion that was more difcult to nd in the literature (i.e., ability
It should be noticed that for the sake of coherence regarding our
measures) in order to guarantee a more balanced pool of results
inclusion criteria, one study integrated in the original meta-analy-
to analyze.
sis was excluded from the present one. Specically, the Humpel,
Caputi, and Martin (2001) study was not considered here, because
2.1. Meta-analytic procedures only one scale (Perception of emotions) of a complete measure of
EI was used.
This meta-analysis is based on Rosenthal and Rubins tech- Also, in line with the original meta-analysis, three predictors
niques (1986), one of the approaches recommended by Johnson were derived for each hypothesis test: gender was operationalised
et al. (1995). Briey, the procedure entails converting study out- as either male (1), female (2) or both (0); age group was opera-
comes to standard normal metrics (Zs associated with one-tailed tionalised as either adults (1), adolescents (2) or both (0); nally,
probabilities for signicance levels and Fishers r-to-Z transforma- participants origin was operationalised as either students (1),
tion for effect sizes), combining them to produce weighted means community (2) or both (0). Based on Schutte et al.s (2007) results,
and examining them in diffuse and focused comparisons. Also, this it is expected that the magnitude of the relationship between EI
approach provides a fail-safe number, which estimates the number and health will vary as a function of gender, but not as a function
of unretrieved studies and that were probably left in the le of age group and participants prole (see Table 1 for the hypothe-
556 A. Martins et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 49 (2010) 554564

Table 1
Studies included in the meta-analysis of the relationship between EI and health.

Study Taskb Statistic (r) n Z Fisher Z p Genderg Ageh Origini

Mental health
Ali, Amorim, and Chamorro-Premuzic (2009) TEIQuec .37 84 .39 3.49 2.46E-004 0 1 1
Augusto-Landa, Berrios-Martos, Lpez-Zafra, and Aguilar-Luzn (2006) TMMSc .21 180 .21 2.84 .002 0 1 2
Austin, Saklofske, and Egan (2005)a SEISd .19 115 .19 2.04 .002 0 1 0
Bastian, Burns, and Nettelbeck (2005)a ABILITY .24 246 .24 3.80 7.00E-005 0 0 1
Bauld and Brown (2009) SEIS .51 116 .56 5.87 2.61E-009 2 1 2
Brackett and Mayer (2003)a ABILITY .04 188 .04 0.55 .29 0 1 1
Brackett, Mayer, and Warner (2004)a ABILITY .11 330 .11 2.00 .02 0 0 1
Brown and Schutte (2006)a SEIS .37 167 .39 4.94 4.15E-007 0 1 1
Ciarrochi, Chan, and Bajgar (2001)a SEIS .43 131 .46 5.15 1.42E-007 0 2 1
Ciarrochi, Deane, and Anderson (2002)a SEIS .23 302 .34 4.04 2.67E-005 0 1 1
Curbelo, Prez, Garca, and Fumero (2006) TMMSc .10 135 .10 1.16 .12 0 1 2
Dawda & Hart (Men) (2000)a EQ-i .57 118 .65 6.77 1.21E-011 1 0 1
Dawda & Hart (Women) (2000)a EQ-i .62 124 .73 7.71 2.77E-014 2 0 1
Day, Therrien, and Carroll (2005)a EQ-i .42 115 .45 4.70 1.38E-006 0 1 1
Donaldson-Feilder and Bond (2004)a TMMS c
.16 290 .15 2.74 .003 0 1 2
Dulewicz, Higgs, and Slaski (2003)a EIQ .46 59 .50 3.70 1.09E-004 0 1 2
Durn, Extremera, Rey, Fernndez-Berrocal, and Moltalbn (2006) TMMSc .22 373 .22 4.29 8.89E-006 0 1 1
Extremera & Fernndez-Berrocal (2002)a TMMS .23 99 .23 2.30 .011 2 1 1
Extremera & Fernndez-Berrocal (2005)a TMMSc .17 161 .17 2.16 .015 0 1 1
Extremera & Fernndez-Berrocal (2006) TMMSc .28 169 .29 3.70 1.09E-004 0 1 1
Extremera, Fernndez-Berrocal, and Durn (2003) TMMS .23 183 .23 3.14 8.42E-004 0 1 2
Extremera, Fernndez-Berrocal, Ruiz-Aranda, and Cabello (2006) ABILITY .13 625 .13 3.26 5.57E-004 0 0 1
Fernndez-Berrocal, Extremera, and Ramos (2004)a TMMSc . 23 292 .23 3.97 3.55E-005 0 1 1
Fernandez-Berrocal, Salovey, Vera, Extremera, and Ramos (2005) TMMSc .28 278 .29 4.75 1.04E-006 0 1 1
Fernndez-Berrocal, Alcaide, Extremera, and Pizarro (2006) TMMS .29 250 .30 4.67 1.52E-006 0 2 1
Fleming and Snell (2008) MIXED .20 87 .20 1.87 .031 0 1 1
Fritson, Wadkins, Gerdes, and Hof (2007) EQ-ic .38 32 .40 2.18 .015 0 1 1
Gardner and Qualter (2009) ABILITY .26 523 .27 6.04 9.78E-010 0 1 0
Ghorbani, Bing, Watson, Davison, and Mack (2002)a TMMS c
.32 451 .33 6.97 3.36E-012 0 1 1
Gohm, Corser, and Dalsky (2005)a ABILITY .08 158 .08 1.00 .158 0 1 1
Goldenberg, Matheson, and Mantler (2006)a ABILITY .14 223 .14 2.09 .018 0 1 2
Goldman, Kramer, and Salovey (1996)a TMMS .15 134 .15 1.74 .041 0 1 1
Grisham, Steketee, and Frost (2008) SEIS .27 60 .28 2.10 .018 0 1 2
Hemmati, Mills, and Kroner (2004)a EQ-i .57 119 .65 6.79 1.01E-011 1 1 2
c
Hervs and Vzquez (2006) TMMS .28 243 .29 4.44 4.55E-006 0 1 1
Jain and Sinha (2005)a EQ-i .25 250 .26 4.00 3.12E-005 1 1 2
Johnson and Spector (2007) WLEIS .24 176 .24 3.22 6.47E-004 0 0 2
Jorge, Garca, and Mrida (2008) TMMSc .33 50 .34 2.36 .009 0 2 1
Kaur, Schutte, and Thorsteinsson (2006) SEIS .46 117 .50 5.24 8.62E-008 0 1 2
Kee et al. (2009) ABILITY .33 50 .34 2.36 .009 0 1 2
Kemp et al. (2005) BRIEF .18 257 .18 2.90 .002 0 1 2
Leible and Snell (2004)a MIXED .22 1359 .22 8.21 1.02E-015 0 1 1
c
Limonero, Toms-Sbado, and Fernndez-Castro (2006) TMMS .19 25 .19 .93 .176 0 1 1
Martinez-Pons (1997)a TMMS .64 108 .76 7.49 1.16E-013 0 1 2
Martinez-Pons (1999)a EISRS .36 100 .38 3.70 1.09E-004 0 1 2
Matthews et al. (2006) ABILITY .20 200 .20 2.85 .002 0 1 1
Mavroveli, Petrides, Rieffe, and Bakker (2007) TEIQuec .60 282 .69 11.19 2.61E-024 0 2 1
Mikolajczak, Luminet, and Menil (Study 1) (2006) TEIQuec .76 80 .10 8.23 9.04E-0016 0 1 1
Mikolajczak, Luminet, and Menil (Study 2) (2006) TEIQuec .67 75 .81 6.62 3.03E-0011 0 1 1
Mikolajczak, Luminet, Leroy, and Roy (2007) TEIQue .65 82 .78 6.65 2.51E-011 0 1 0
Mikolajczak, Petrides & Hurry (2009) TEIQuec .48 490 .52 11.31 1.19E-024 0 2 1
c
Montes-Berges and Augusto-Landa (2007) TMMS .15 119 .15 1.64 .051 0 1 1
Nikolaou and Tsaousis (2002) EIQ .59 212 .68 9.49 1.86E-019 0 1 2
Noguera and Dez (2006) TMMSc .29 144 .30 3.54 2.02E-004 0 1 2
Oginska-Bulik (2005)a SEIS .16 330 .16 2.92 .002 0 1 2
Parker, Taylor, Eastabrook, Schell, and Wood (2008) EQ-Ic .28 667 .29 7.37 2.53E-013 0 2 2
Pau and Croucher (2003)a SEIS .29 213 .30 4.31 8.22E-006 0 1 1
e
Petrides, Prez-Gonzlez, and Furnham (Study 1) (2007) EQ-i .53 166 .59 7.37 2.63E-013 0 1 1
Petrides, Prez-Gonzlez, and Furnham (Study 2) (2007) TEIQue .57 200 .65 8.83 1.55E-017 0 1 1
Petrides, Prez-Gonzlez, and Furnham (Study 3) (2007) TEIQue .40 212 .42 6.06 8.88E-010 0 1 1
Petrides, Pita and Kokkinaki (2007) TEIQue .47 274 .51 8.25 7.78E-016 0 2 1
Queirs, Fernndez-Berrocal, Extremera, Carral, and e Queirs (2005) TMMSc .22 240 .22 3.44 2.91E-004 0 1 0
Repetto, Pena, and Lozano (2006) EII .22 392 .22 4.40 5.44E-006 0 2 1
Riley and Schutte (2003)a SEIS .38 141 .40 4.66 1.58E-006 0 1 0
Rossen and Kranzler (2009) ABILITY .25 150 .26 3.10 9.81E-004 0 1 1
Saklofske, Austin, and Minski (2003)a SEIS .38 344 .40 7.31 3.79E-013 0 1 1
Saklofske, Austin, Galloway, and Davidson (2007) SEIS .04 356 .04 .75 .226 0 1 1
Salovey, Stroud, Woolery, and Epel (2002)a TMMS .26 104 .27 2.68 .004 0 0 1
Snchez, Montas, Latorre, and Fernndez-Berrocal (2006) TMMSc .17 146 .17 2.06 .020 0 1 2
Schmidt and Andrykowski (2004)a TMMSc .35 210 .37 5.22 9.60E-008 2 1 2
Schutte et al. (1998)a SEIS .37 37 .39 2.29 0.011 0 1 0
Shulman and Hemenover (2006) MIXED .32 225 .33 4.91 4.64E-007 0 1 1
Slaski and Cartwright (2002)a EQ-i .49 221 .54 7.76 1.94E-014 0 1 2
A. Martins et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 49 (2010) 554564 557

Table 1 (continued)

Study Taskb Statistic (r) n Z Fisher Z p Genderg Ageh Origini


c f
Summerfeldt, Kloosterman, Antony, and Parker (2006) EQ-i .54 667 .60 15.15 1.60E-034 0 1 1
Trinidad and Johnson (2002)a ABILITY .19 205 .19 2.74 .003 0 2 1
Tsaousis and Nikolaou (Study 1) (2005)a TEIQ .43 365 .46 8.62 6.39E-017 0 1 0
Tsaousis and Nikolaou (Study 1) (2005)a TEIQ .29 212 .30 4.30 8.61E-006 0 1 2
Uva et al. (2010) TEIQue .44 36 .47 2.72 .003 0 1 2
Psychosomatic health
Augusto-Landa et al. (2006) TMMSc .18 180 .18 2.42 .008 0 1 2
Brown and Schutte (2006)a SEIS .36 167 .38 4.80 8.47E-007 0 1 1
Dawda and Hart (Men) 2000a EQ-i .30 117 .31 3.30 4.83E-004 1 0 1
Dawda and Hart (Women) 2000a EQ-i .32 122 .33 3.61 1.54E-004 2 0 1
Day et al. (2005)a EQ-i .42 115 .45 4.70 1.38E-006 0 1 1
Duran et al. (2006) TMMSc .22 373 .22 4.29 8.89E-006 0 1 1
Extremera and Fernndez-Berrocal (2006) TMMSc .14 169 .14 1.82 .034 0 1 1
Extremera & Fernndez-Berrocal (2002)a TMMS .20 99 .20 2.00 .023 2 1 1
Extremera et al. (2003) TMMS .15 183 .15 2.03 .021 0 1 2
Greven, Chamorro-Premuzic, Arteche, and Furnham (2008) TEIQue .50 1038 .55 17.27 2.74E-039 0 1 1
c
Johnson, Batey, and Holdsworth (2009) TEIQue .50 328 .55 9.69 4.85E-020 0 1 1
Latorre and Montas (2004) TMMSc .13 450 .13 2.76 .003 0 2 1
Mavroveli et al. (2007) TEIQue .40 282 .42 6.99 2.87E-012 0 2 1
Mikolajczak, Petrides and Hurry (2009) TEIQuec .31 490 .32 7.03 2.34E-012 0 2 1
Salovey et al. (2002)a TMMS .23 104 .23 2.36 .009 0 0 1
Shulman and Hemenover (2006) MIXED .22 225 .22 3.33 4.34E-004 0 1 1
Physical health
Austin et al. (2005)a SEISd .03 117 .03 .32 .37 0 1 0
Bauld and Brown (2009) SEIS .54 116 .60 6.28 2.34E-010 2 1 2
Donaldson-Feilder and Bond (2004)a TMMSc .16 290 .15 2.74 .003 0 1 2
Extremera and Fernndez-Berrocal (2002)a TMMS .02 99 .02 .20 .42 2 1 1
Freudenthaler, Neubauer, Gabler, Scherl, and Rindermann (2008) MIXED .34 150 .35 4.27 9.81E-006 0 1 1
Goldman et al. (1996)a TMMS .15 134 .15 1.74 .041 0 1 1
Mikolajczak, Luminet, and Menil (Study 1) (2006) TEIQue .46 80 .50 4.32 7.87E-006 0 1 1
Mikolajczak, Luminet, and Menil (Study 2) (2006) TEIQue .58 75 .66 5.49 2.24E-008 0 1 1
Queirs, Fernndez-Berrocal, Extremera, and Queirs (2006) TMMSc .26 400 .27 5.28 6.93E-008 0 1 2
Saklofske et al. (2007) SEIS .13 356 .13 2.46 .007 0 1 1
Tsaousis and Nikolaou (Study 1) (2005)a TEIQ .32 365 .33 6.27 2.58E-010 0 1 0
Tsaousis and Nikolaou (Study 2) (2005)a TEIQ .44 212 .47 6.73 1.51E-011 0 1 2
a
Studies included in the Schutte et al.s (2007) meta-analysis.
b
ABILITY = MSCEITMayer, Salovey, Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso, 2002) or MEISMultifactor Emotional Intelligence Scale; BRIEF = Brain
Resource Inventory for Emotional intelligence Factors (Kemp et al., 2005); EII = Emotional Intelligence Inventory (Tapia, 2001); EIQ = Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire
(Dulewicz et al., 2003); EISRS = Emotional Intelligence Self-Regulation Scale (Martinez-Pons, 1999-2000); EQ-i = Emotional Quotient Inventory (Bar-On, 1997); SEIS = Schutte
Emotional Intelligence Scale (Schutte et al., 1998); TEIQ = Traits Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (Tsaousis & Nikolaou, 2005); TEIQue = Trait Emotional Intelligence
Questionnaire (Petrides, Prez-Gonzlez & Furnham, 2007); TMMS = Trait Meta Mood Scale (Salovey et al., 1995); WLEIS = Wong and Law Emotional Intelligence Scale (Law,
Wong, & Song, 2004).
c
Short version.
d
Modied version of the SEIS (41 items).
e
Modied version of the EQ-i (15 additional items).
f
Given that the sample size of this study (n = 2629) was greater than the combined size of all the other EQ-i studies, we used the sample size equal to the next biggest
study (viz., 667), to avoid biasing the results (Hunter & Schmidt, 2004).
g
Male = 1, female = 2, both = 0.
h
Adults = 1, adolescents = 2, both = 0.
i
Students = 1, community = 2, both = 0.

ses tests included in this meta-analysis, along with the relevant Questionnaire; Petrides, Prez-Gonzlez & Furnham, 2007), repre-
statistical information). senting the different tests used to measure EI as a trait. The same
meta-analytic procedures were applied for health in general and
for the three types of health, in order to analyze their specic rela-
3. Results tionships with EI.

Hypothesis tests for the ability and trait tasks were separately
subjected to the following meta-analytic procedures: combination 3.1. Ability task
of signicance levels, and the combination and diffuse comparison
of effect sizes (for more details please refer to Rosenthal & Rubin, 3.2.1. Results of combinations of signicance levels and effect sizes
1986). The same was done for the diverse trait tasks, that were Effect sizes and signicance levels. For the k = 11 hypothesis tests
used more often: TMMS1 (Trait Meta Mood Scale; Salovey, Mayer, presented in Table 1, the EI effect was small (r = .172, Z Fish-
Goldman, Turvey, & Palfai, 1995), SEIS (Schutte Emotional Intelli- er = 0.174) but signicant (Z = 9.009, p = 4.690525E-018) for the
gence Scale; Schutte et al., 1998), EQ-i (Emotional Quotient Inven- ability task.
tory; Bar-on, 1997), and TEIQue (Trait Emotional Intelligence Fail-safe number. For the present meta-analysis, the fail-safe
1
Although the TMMS contains three scales that are usually not combined, they
number of 317 exceeds the threshold of 5(11 + 10) = 105. There-
were averaged to obtain a global index for the purpose of this study, as there is fore, it seems unlikely that the results reported above were ad-
precedence for this procedure (Schutte et al., 2007). versely affected by publication bias.
558 A. Martins et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 49 (2010) 554564

Results of the focused comparisons of the predictor variables. For 3.4. TMMS task
the ability task, none of the predictor variables hypothesized to
moderate the relationship between EI and Health, was signicant 3.4.1. Results of combinations of signicance levels and effect sizes
(see Table 3). Effect sizes and signicance levels. For the k = 25 hypothesis tests
presented in Table 1, the EI effect was moderate (r = .242, Z Fish-
3.3. Trait task er = 0.247) and signicant (Z = 17.060, p = 7.68916E-39) for the
TMMS task.
3.3.1. Results of combinations of signicance levels and effect sizes Fail-safe number. For the present meta-analysis, the fail-safe
Effect sizes and signicance levels. For the k = 72 hypothesis tests number of 2690 well exceeds the threshold of 5(25 + 10) = 175.
presented in Table 1, the EI effect was moderate (r = .341, Z Fish- Therefore, it seems unlikely that the results reported above were
er = 0.356) and signicant (Z = 41.451, p = 1,828692E-73) for the adversely affected by publication bias.
trait task. Results of the focused comparisons of the predictor variables. For
Fail-safe number. For the present meta-analysis, the fail-safe the TMMS task none of the predictor variables hypothesized to
number of 49,182 well exceeds the threshold of 5(72 + 10) = 410. moderate the relationship between EI and Health, was signicant
Therefore, it seems unlikely that the results reported above were
adversely affected by publication bias.
Results of the focused comparisons of the predictor variables. There 3.5. SEIS task
was only one signicant effect of participants characteristics in the
trait task Gender (Z = 3.223, p = .001) (see Table 3). Therefore, fe- 3.5.1. Results of combinations of signicance levels and effect sizes
male participants had higher EI in the trait task than male Effect sizes and signicance levels. For the k = 13 hypothesis tests
participants. presented in Table 1, the EI effect was moderate (r = .286, Z Fish-
er = 0.294) and signicant (Z = 13.052, p = 2,471925E-29) for the
SEIS task.
Table 2
Fail-safe number. For the present meta-analysis, the fail-safe
Summary statistics of the meta-analysis for the relationship between EI and health
indicators.
number of 1008 well exceeds the threshold of 5(13 + 10) = 115.
Therefore, it seems unlikely that the results reported above were
Health indicator n r Z Fisher Z p Fail-safe No. adversely affected by publication bias.
Ability task Results of the focused comparisons of the predictor variables. There
Mental 11 .17 .17 9.01 4.69E-18 317 was only one signicant effect of participants characteristics in the
Trait task SEIS task Gender (Z = 2.550, p = .005). Therefore, female partici-
Physical 12 .27 .28 12.40 1.24E-27 773 pants had higher EI in the SEIS task than male participants (see Ta-
Mental 67 .36 .38 33.31 1.05E-64 39,258
ble 3).
Psychosomatic 16 .33 .35 23.45 6.62E-51 2255
TMMS task
Mental 23 .25 .26 16.60 7.72E-38 2238
Psychosomatic 7 .17 .17 6.52 5.61E-11 109 3.6. EQ-i task
SEIS task
Mental 13 .28 .29 12.62 3.38E-28 972 3.6.1. Results of combinations of signicance levels and effect sizes
Effect sizes and signicance levels. For the k = 10 hypothesis tests
EQ-i task
Mental 10 .44 .48 21.23 4.48E-47 1790 presented in Table 1, the EI effect was moderate (r = .444, Z Fish-
er = 0.477) and signicant (Z = 21.233, p = 4,484285E-47) for the
TEIQue task
Mental 10 .53 .59 22.61 1.70E-49 1978 EQ-i task.
Fail-safe number. For the present meta-analysis, the fail-safe
Note: For the physical and psychosomatic health indicators there were no studies
number of 1790 well exceeds the threshold of 5(10 + 10) = 100.
available that used the ability task. Regarding the specic trait tasks, health indi-
cators that had fewer than ve studies with the same task were not included in the Therefore, it seems unlikely that the results reported above were
table. adversely affected by publication bias.
Results of the focused comparisons of the predictor variables. There
was only one signicant effect of participants characteristics in the
EQ-i task Gender. (Z = 2.228, p = .01). Therefore, female partici-
Table 3 pants had higher EI in the EQ-i task than male participants (see Ta-
Effect Sizes for the predictor variables included in the meta-analysis of the
relationship between EI and health.
ble 3).

Task Gendera Ageb Originc


d
ABILITY Z = .594 Z = .231 3.7. TEIQue task
P = .276 P = .408
TRAIT Z = 3.223 Z = 1.392 Z = 2.772
P = .001 P = .920 P = 1.115
3.7.1. Results of combinations of signicance levels and effect sizes
TMMS Z = .785 Z = .007 Z = 1.290 Effect sizes and signicance levels. For the k = 12 hypothesis tests
P = .216 P = .503 P = .099 presented in Table 1, the EI effect was moderate (r = .505, Z Fish-
SEIS Z = 2.550 Z = 1.481 Z = .636 er = 0.556) and signicant (Z = 27.817, p = 1,425827E-57) for the
P = .005 P = .069 P = .262
TEIQue task.
EQ-i Z = 2.228 Z = 4.515 Z = 2.521
P = .01 P = 4.325 P = 1.061 Fail-safe number. For the present meta-analysis, the fail-safe
TEIQue d
Z = .1743 Z = 1.498 number of 3553 well exceeds the threshold of 5(12 + 10) = 110.
P = .966 P = .094 Therefore, it seems unlikely that the results reported above were
a
Male = 1, female = 2.
adversely affected by publication bias.
b
Adults = 1, adolescents = 2. Results of the focused comparisons of the predictor variables. For
c
Students = 1, community = 2. the TEIQue task none of the predictor variables hypothesized to
d
Insufcient number of studies. moderate the relationship between EI and Health, was signicant
A. Martins et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 49 (2010) 554564 559

3.8. Health indicators fail-safe numbers exceeded the thresholds, it seems unlikely that
these results were adversely affected by publication bias. Regard-
Regarding the different health indicators, results were also ana- ing the other two types of health, there arent enough studies with-
lyzed as a function of the three categories considered: mental, psy- in each specic kind of measure, to allow for this sort of
chosomatic and physical. Since only mental health studies comparison.
included ability measures, the comparison between these three
categories regards just the trait tasks. As shown in Table 2, mental 3.9. Cumulative meta-analysis
health showed a stronger association with EI (r = .36), followed by
psychosomatic health (r = .33) and physical health (r = .27). In all To perform a cumulative meta-analysis a separate meta-
cases the fail-safe number exceeded the threshold. Therefore, it analysis is conducted each time a new study is added (wave)
seems unlikely that these results were adversely affected by pub- to the meta-analytic database in order to answer the questions
lication bias. of sufciency and stability (Mullen et al., 2001). Two different
Examining the specic measures of the trait approach, Table 2 indicators were proposed by Mullen et al. (2001) to address
also shows that the TEIQue and EQ-i have the strongest association these issues: the fail-safe ratio and the cumulative slope,
with mental health (r = .53 and r = .44, respectively). Since all the respectively.

Fig. 1. Cumulative meta-analysis for the ability task.

Fig. 2. Cumulative meta-analysis for the trait task.

Fig. 3. Cumulative meta-analysis for the TMMS task.


560 A. Martins et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 49 (2010) 554564

Sufciency has been obtained if the fail-safe ratio exceeds three hypothesis tests (see Fig. 4), the EQ-i task reached sufciency
Rosenthals threshold at 1.000, i.e., when the fail-safe number con- in the rst wave in 2000 after two hypothesis tests and (see Fig. 5)
sistently exceeds the 5k + 10 benchmark, indicating that there is no and the TEIQue task reached sufciency in the rst wave in 2006
need for additional research to establish the phenomenon (Mullen after two hypothesis tests (see Fig. 6).
et al., 2001). Therefore, Figs.1 and 2 indicate that the EI paradigm Stability has been obtained if the cumulative slope reaches
has reached sufciency across both ability and trait tasks. Speci- 0.000, i.e., when the slope of the best-tting regression line is as
cally, as seen in Fig. 1, the ability task reached sufciency by the small as possible, indicating that the effect has become stable
rst wave in 2002 after one hypothesis test. And, as seen in and that additional studies are unlikely to change the existing nd-
Fig. 2, the trait task reached sufciency by the second wave in ings (Mullen et al., 2001). Therefore, Figs. 1 and 2 indicate that the
1997 after two hypotheses tests. Within the trait paradigm, the EI paradigm has reached stability for both tasks. Specically, as
four tasks analyzed TMMS, SEIS, EQ-i and TEIQue have all seen in Fig. 1, the ability task reached stability by the third wave
reached sufciency. Specically, the TMMS reached sufciency in in 2004 after three hypotheses tests. And, as seen in Fig. 2, the trait
the second wave in 1997 after two hypotheses tests (see Fig. 3), task reached stability by the fth wave in 2000 after six hypotheses
the SEIS task reached sufciency in the third wave in 2002 after tests. Within the trait paradigm, the four tasks analyzed TMMS,

Fig. 4. Cumulative meta-analysis for the SEIS task.

Fig. 5. Cumulative meta-analysis for the EQ-i task. Note: For the EQ-i task the fail-safe ratio exceeded 100 from the rst wave onward.

Fig. 6. Cumulative meta-analysis for the TEIQue task. Note: For the TEIQue task the fail-safe ratio exceeded 110 from the rst wave onward.
A. Martins et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 49 (2010) 554564 561

SEIS, EQ-i and TEIQue have all reached stability. Specically, and compared studies that used only one gender with those that
TMMS reached stability in the third wave in 2002 after ve used both. Having a bigger pool of studies made possible these
hypotheses tests (see Fig. 3), the SEIS task reached stability in the more specic comparisons revealing important information about
second wave in 2001 after two hypotheses tests (see Fig. 4), the the sort of measure that produces these kinds of results and within
EQ-i task reached stability in the second wave in 2002 after three which gender group. Since neither meta-analysis found a signi-
hypotheses tests (see Fig. 5) and TEIQue reached stability in the cant effect of origin of participants (students vs. community), there
second wave in 2007 after seven hypotheses tests (see Fig. 6). seems to be no support for the skeptic idea that studies using sam-
ples of university students would not apply to clinical populations
(Hansen, Lloyd, & Stough, 2009).
4. Discussion Considering the three health indicators, the conclusions of the
present meta-analysis are consistent with those obtained by Schu-
The rst meta-analytic studies in the EI domain were mainly tte et al. (2007). Specically, as seen in Table 2, the pattern of the
concerned with construct (e.g., Van Rooy et al., 2005) and predic- effect sizes was similar, with mental and psychosomatic health
tive validity, particularly its relationship with performance (e.g., showing a stronger association with EI (r = .36 and r = .33, respec-
Van Rooy & Viswesvaran, 2004). Subsequently, Schutte et al. tively), followed by physical health (r = .27). For the mental health
(2007) decided to take a more in-depth look at the association be- indicator, the magnitude of the relationship (.36) was higher than
tween EI and health and systematically reviewed studies investi- the one found by Schutte et al. (.29), but there were 44 additional
gating this relationship. However, due to the widespread interest effect sizes in the present study. For the psychosomatic health indi-
in this area, numerous additional studies became available after- cator, although there were 10 additional effect sizes included, the
wards, including non-English ones. Moreover, no cumulative magnitude of the association was quite the same (.31 in their study
meta-analysis had yet been performed, at least to our knowledge, and .33 in the present one). For physical health, even if the studies
which would indicate if more investigation is required. In view of included in the present analysis were doubled, the results were
this, the present work aimed to complement and extend Schutte quite similar, although a bit higher (.22 in the original study and
et al.s (2007) ndings by including studies published after their re- .27 in the present study).Overall, these results suggest the same
view as well as non-English ones (both before and after their direction and strength.
study), and also by performing a cumulative meta-analysis. Considering the two main approaches to assess EI, we can see in
After the new literature search, an additional pool of 46 studies Table 2 that, unlike Schutte et al.s (2007) meta-analysis, the trait
and 63 effect sizes were assembled and analyzed together with the approach had signicant associations with all of the health indica-
ones included in the prior meta-analysis. The general combinations tors, following the same pattern indicated above for EI in general.
of the total 105 effect sizes based on the responses of 19,815 par- Also, unlike the original meta-analysis, for the ability approach
ticipants revealed a highly signicant, moderate, positive relation- there was a signicant association with mental health, but the
ship between EI and health. For the trait task this effect was higher magnitude was lower than for trait approach. Most likely, these
(r = .34) than for the ability task (r = .17) and no additional studies differences are due to the increased number of studies in both
are needed to establish the existence of the phenomenon in either cases.
case (the fail-safe numbers were all above the threshold). There- Taking into account the different measures of trait EI approach,
fore, comparing the two main approaches, EI measured as a trait we can see in Table 2 that TEIQue and EQ-i have the strongest asso-
is apparently a better health predictor. Overall the results are con- ciations with mental health (r = .53 and r = .44, respectively),
gruent with the previous study by Schutte et al. (2007). However, showing the same pattern as health in general. In Schutte et al.s
they could be a function of common method variance, i.e., using (2007) meta-analysis the EQ-i had the strongest correlations with
self-reports for both predictor and criterion variables could ac- both mental and psychosomatic health, but as noted before, the
count for their relationship, due to response dispositions. Never- TEIQue had not been included in that study. Overall, it seems that
theless, Mikolajczak, Petrides, Coumans and Luminet (2009) comprehensive measures (e.g.,TEIQue and EQ-i) are better health
conducted a series of experimental studies using self-reports of predictors than narrow measures (e.g., SEIS and TMMS), having
EI (TEIQue) and Affectivity (PANAS), and even after controlling also the advantage of providing dimensional scores, which could
for Social Desirability, the association between them remained sig- help identify decits in specic facets of trait EI related to a clinical
nicant, excluding potential response biases. condition or particular health problem. Mikolajczak, Petrides, Cou-
Looking at the effects of the different instruments used to mea- mans & Luminet (2009) found that the four factors of the TEIQue
sure EI within the trait paradigm, the TEIQue and EQ-i showed had different effects on mood deterioration after induced stress.
stronger relationships with health (r = .50 and r = .44, respectively), Future applications should take this into account.
while the SEIS and TMMS showed lower associations (r = .29 and The cumulative analysis included in this study showed suf-
r = .24, respectively). In Schutte et al.s (2007) meta-analysis the ciency and stability for the ability and trait approaches, as well
EQ-i had the strongest relationship with mental and psychoso- as for the four specic trait measures: TMMS, SEIS, EQ-i and TEI-
matic health (their study did not report the association with health Que. The other measures did not have enough waves of investiga-
in general) but with similar results to our study. As the TEIQue was tion (i.e., in more than two successive points in time) to make this
not included in previous meta-analyses, this is a new and impor- analysis possible, thus indicating that more research is needed to
tant nding. test their sufciency and stability.
From the three moderator variables analyzed gender, age, and
origin of the participants only gender produced signicant differ-
ences in the magnitude of the relationship between EI and health. 5. Conclusion
In fact, studies that used just females signicantly increased the ef-
fect of this relationship in the trait task and more specically in the The main ndings of the present meta-analysis represent an
SEIS and EQ-i tasks. Overall, these results are congruent with those extension of the results obtained by Schutte et al. (2007) and cor-
obtained by Schutte el al. (2007), since their study also did not nd roborate the overall tendencies already identied by their study,
moderating effects for participants age and origin. However, this reinforcing prior conclusions. First, as they already pointed out,
comparison is not straightforward, as they analyzed this modera- the effect sizes for the relationship between EI and the three types
tion in general terms, not looking at each measure separately of health found in the present research compare favorably to the
562 A. Martins et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 49 (2010) 554564

*Brown, R. F., & Schutte, N. S. (2006). Direct and indirect relationships between
association of .20 between EI and work performance reported by
emotional intelligence and subjective fatigue in university students. Journal of
Van Rooy and Viswesvaran (2004). Schutte et al. (2007) also men-
Psychosomatic Research, 60, 585593.
tioned that a similar association between the big ve personality *Ciarrochi, J., Chan, A. Y. C., & Bajgar, J. (2001). Measuring emotional intelligence in
dimensions and health was found in a recent meta-analysis adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences, 31, 11051119.
*Ciarrochi, J., Deane, F. P., & Anderson, S. (2002). Emotional intelligence moderates
(Malouff, Thorsteinsson, & Schutte, 2005).
the relationship between stress and mental health. Personality and Individual
However, given that in their latest meta-analysis Van Rooy et al. Differences, 32, 197209.
*Curbelo, M., Prez, I., Garca, L., & Fumero, A. (2006). Inteligencia emocional en
(2005) reported a .34 correlation between EI measured as a trait
and personality, the question remains as to the incremental valid- policas locales. Ansiedad y Estrs, 12(2/3), 463477.
*Dawda, D., & Hart, S. D. (2000). Assessing emotional intelligence. reliability and
ity of EI measures beyond other predictors, especially within the validity of the bar-on emotional quotient inventory (EQ-i) in university
trait EI approach. Future studies should concentrate on that ques- students. Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 797812.
*Day, A. L., Therrien, D. L., & Carroll, S. A. (2005). Predicting psychological health:
tion. A good example is the Mikolajczak, Petrides, Coumans and
assessing the incremental validity of emotional intelligence beyond personality,
Luminet (2009) series of experimental studies in which trait EI type A behaviour, and daily hassles. European Journal of Personality, 19,
showed incremental validity over and above the Big ve, Alexithy- 519536.
*Donaldson-Feilder, E. J., & Bond, F. W. (2004). The relative importance of
mia, Social Desirability and Resilience in mood deterioration
psychological acceptance and emotional intelligence to workplace well-being.
prediction. British Journal of Guidance and Counselling, 32, 187203.
The cumulative meta-analyses included in this study also added *Dulewicz, V., Higgs, M., & Slaski, M. (2003). Measuring emotional intelligence.
some important new data, namely that both approaches to EI (abil- content, construct and criterion related validity. Journal of Managerial
Psychology, 18, 405420.
ity and trait) have already reached sufciency and stability, as well
Durn, A., Extremera, N., Rey, L., Fernndez-Berrocal, P., Moltalbn F. M. (2006).
as four specic trait EI questionnaires: TMMS, SEIS, EQ-i and TEI- Predicting academic burnout and engagement in educational settings:
Que. Therefore, this reinforces the idea that future research should Assessing the incremental validity of perceived emotional intelligence beyond
perceived stress and general self-efcacy. Psicothema, 18(suppl.), 158164.
not concentrate on establishing the importance of the relationship *Extremera, N., & Fernndez-Berrocal, P. (2002). Relation of perceived emotional
between EI and health, but should instead focus on incremental intelligence and health-related quality of life of middle-aged women.
validity issues. Another interesting topic for future research would Psychological Reports, 91, 4759.
*Extremera, N., & Fernndez-Berrocal, P. (2005). Perceived emotional intelligence
be to investigate the relationships between EI and specic types of
and life satisfaction: Predictive and incremental validity using the trait meta-
health condition (mental, psychosomatic and physical), such as mood scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 937948.
personality disorders and addictions. *Extremera, N., & Fernndez-Berrocal, P. (2006). Emotional intelligence as predictor

Overall, the results seem encouraging regarding the value of EI of mental, social, and physical health in university students. The Spanish Journal
of Psychology, 9(1), 4551.
as a possible health predictor. Naturally, it is important to be aware *Extremera, N., Fernndez-Berrocal, P., & Durn, A. (2003). Inteligencia emocional y
that these analyses are based on correlational studies, so caution burnout en profesores. Encuentros en Psicologa Social, 1, 260265.
*Extremera, N., Fernndez-Berrocal, P., Ruiz-Aranda, D., & Cabello, R. (2006).
should be taken in inferring causal relationships.
Inteligencia emocional, estilos de respuesta y depresin. Ansiedad y Estrs,
12(2/3), 191205.
*Fernndez-Berrocal, P., Alcaide, R., Extremera, N., & Pizarro, D. (2006). The role of
Acknowledgements emotional intelligence in anxiety and depression among adolescents. Individual
Differences Research., 4(1), 1627.
*Fernndez-Berrocal, P., Extremera, N., & Ramos, N. (2004). Validity and reliability
This research was supported in part by the Ph.D scholarship
of the Spanish modied version of the trait meta-mood scale. Psychological
granted by the FCT Foundation for Science and Technology to Reports, 94, 751755.
*Fernandez-Berrocal, P., Salovey, P., Vera, A., Extremera, N., & Ramos, N. (2005).
the rst author. The authors wish to express their gratitude for
Prof. Tirza Leader (University of Kent) helpful comments and sug- Cultural inuences on the relation between perceived emotional
intelligence and depression. International Review of Social Psychology,
gestions on a previous version of this paper, as well as for making 18(1), 91107.
available her software program that supported the analysis of the *Fleming, C., & Snell JR, W. (2008). Emotional intelligence in adults with ADHD. Psi

results presented in this study. Chi Journal of Undergraduate Research, 13(2), 8695.
*Freudenthaler, H. H., Neubauer, A. C., Gabler, P., Scherl, W. G., & Rindermann, H.
(2008). Testing and validating the trait emotional intelligence questionnaire
(TEIQue) in a German-speaking sample. Personality and Individual Differences,
References 45, 673678.
*Fritson, K., Wadkins, T., Gerdes, P., & Hof, D. (2007). The impact of neurotherapy on
college students cognitive abilities and emotions. Journal of Neurotherapy,
References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the 11(4), 19.
*Gardner, K., & Qualter, P. (2009). Emotional intelligence and borderline personality
metaanalysis
disorder. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 9498.
*Ghorbani, N., Bing, M. N., Watson, P. J., Davison, H. K., & Mack, D. A. (2002). Self-
*Ali, F., Amorim, I. S., & Chamorro-Premuzic, T. (2009). Empathy decits and trait
reported emotional intelligence. Construct similarity and functional
emotional intelligence in psychopathy and Machiavellianism. Personality and
dissimilarity of higher order processing in Iran and the United States.
Individual Differences, 47, 758762.
*Augusto-Landa, J. M., Berrios-Martos, M. P., Lpez-Zafra, E., & Aguilar-Luzn, M. C. International Journal of Psychology, 37, 297308.
*Gohm, C. L., Corser, G. C., & Dalsky, D. J. (2005). Emotional intelligence under stress:
(2006). Relacin entre burnout e inteligencia emocional y su impacto en la
useful, unnecessary, or irrelevant? Personality and Individual Differences, 39,
salud mental, bienestar y satisfaccin laboral en profesionales de enfermera.
10171028.
Ansiedad y Estrs, 12(23), 479493. *Goldenberg, I., Matheson, K., & Mantler, J. (2006). The assessment of emotional
*Austin, E. J., Saklofske, D. H., & Egan, V. (2005). Personality, well-being and health
intelligence. A comparison of performance-based and self-report
correlates of trait emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences,
methodologies. Journal of Personality Assessment, 86(1), 3345.
38, 547558. *Goldman, S. L., Kramer, D. T., & Salovey, P. (1996). Beliefs about mood moderate the
Bar-On, R. (1997). Emotional quotient inventory: Technical manual. Toronto: Multi-
relationship of stress to illness and symptom reporting. Journal of Psychosomatic
Health Systems.
*Bastian, V. A., Burns, N. R., & Nettelbeck, T. (2005). Emotional intelligence predicts Research, 41, 115128.
*Greven, C., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., Arteche, A., & Furnham, A. (2008). A
life skills, but not as well as personality and cognitive abilities. Personality and
hierarchical integration of dispositional determinants of general health in
Individual Differences, 39, 11351145.
*Bauld, R., & Brown, R. (2009). Stress, psychological distress, psychosocial factors, students: The big ve, trait emotional intelligence, and humour styles.
Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 15621573.
menopause symptoms and physical health in women. Maturitas, 62(2), *Grisham, J., Steketee, G., Frost, R. (2008). Interpersonal problems and emotional
160165.
*Brackett, M. A., & Mayer, J. D. (2003). Convergent, discriminant, and incremental intelligence in compulsive hoarding. Depression and Anxiety (10914269), 25(9),
E63E71.
validity of competing measures of emotional intelligence. Personality and Social
Hansen, K., Lloyd, J., & Stough, C. (2009). Emotional intelligence and clinical
Psychology Bulletin, 29, 11471158.
*Brackett, M. A., Mayer, J. D., & Warner, R. M. (2004). Emotional intelligence and its disorders. In C. Stough, D. Saklofske, & J. Parker (Eds.), Assessing
emotional intelligence. Theory, research, and applications (pp. 219237).
relation to everyday behaviour. Personality and Individual Differences, 36,
US: Springer.
13871402.
A. Martins et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 49 (2010) 554564 563

*Hemmati, T., Mills, J. F., & Kroner, D. G. (2004). The validity of the Bar-On emotional nursing students. Journal of Psychiatric and Mental Health Nursing, 14(2),
intelligence quotient in an offender population. Personality and Individual 163171.
Differences, 37, 695706. Mullen, B., Muellerleile, P., & Bryant, B. (2001). Cumulative meta-analysis: A
*Hervs, G., & Vzquez, C. (2006). Explorando el origen emocional de las respuestas consideration of indicators of sufciency and stability. Personality and Social
rumiativas: El papel de la complejidad emocional y la inteligencia emocional. Psychology Bulletin, 27, 14501462.
Ansiedad y Estrs, 12(23), 279292. *Nikolaou, I., & Tsaousis, I. (19932002). Emotional intelligence in the workplace.
Humpel, N., Caputi, P., & Martin, C. (2001). The relationship between emotions and Exploring its effects on occupational stress and organizational commitment.
stress among mental health nurses. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Mental International Journal of Organizational Analysis, 10(4), 327.
Health Nursing, 10, 5560. *Noguera, J., & Dez, I. (2006). Relaciones entre inteligencia emocional percibida y
Hunter, J. E., & Schmidt, F. L. (2004). Methods of meta-analysis: Correcting error and emocionalidad negativa. Ansiedad y Estrs, 12(2/3), 207221.
bias in research ndings (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. *Oginska-Bulik, N. (2005). Emotional intelligence in the workplace. Exploring its
*Jain, A. K., & Sinha, A. K. (2005). General health in organizations: relative relevance effects on occupational stress and health outcomes in human service workers.
of emotional intelligence, trust, and organizational support. International Journal International Journal of Occupational Medicine and Environmental Health, 18,
of Stress Management, 12, 257273. 167175.
*Johnson, S. J., Batey, M., & Holdsworth, L. (2009). Personality and health: The *Parker, J. D. A., Taylor, R. N., Eastabrook, J. M., Schell, S. L., & Wood, L. M. (2008).
mediating role of trait emotional intelligence and work locus of control. Problem gambling in adolescence. Relationships with internet misuse, gaming
Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 470475. abuse, and emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 45,
Johnson, B. T., Mullen, B., & Salas, E. (1995). Comparison of three major meta- 174180.
analytic approaches. Journal of Applied Psychology, 80(1), 94106. *Pau, A. K. H., & Croucher, R. (2003). Emotional intelligence and perceived stress in
Johnson, B. T., Scott-Sheldon, L. A. J., Snyder, L. B., Noar, S. M., & Huedo-Medina, T. B. dental undergraduates. Journal of Dental Education, 67, 10231028.
(2008). Contemporary approaches to meta-analysis in communication research. *Petrides, K. V., Prez-Gonzlez, J. C., & Furnham, A. (2007). On the criterion and
In A. F. *Hayes, M. Slater, & L. B. Snyder (Eds.), Advanced data analysis methods for incremental validity of trait emotional intelligence. Cognition and Emotion,
communication research (pp. 311347). Sage Publications inc.. 21(1), 2655.
*Johnson, H. M., & Spector, P. E. (2007). Service with a smile: Do emotional *Petrides, K. V., Pita, R., & Kokkinaki, F. (2007). The location of trait emotional
intelligence, gender, and autonomy moderate the emotional labor process? intelligence in personality factor space. British Journal of Psychology, 98,
Journal of Occupational and Health Psychology, 12(4), 319333. 273289.
*Jorge, M., Garca, D., & Mrida, J. (2008). Relaciones entre inteligencia emocional y *Queirs, M. M., Fernndez-Berrocal, P., Extremera, N., Carral, J. M. C., & e Queirs, P.
ajuste psicolgico en alumnos de enseanza secundaria. Ansiedad y Estrs, 14(1), S. (2005). Validao e abilidade da verso portuguesa modicada da Trait
3141. Meta-Mood Scale. Revista de Psicologia, Educao e Cultura, 9, 199216.
*Kaur, I., Schutte, N. S., & Thorsteinsson, E. B. (2006). Gambling control self-efcacy *Queirs, M., Fernndez-Berrocal, P., Extremera, N., & Queirs, P. (2006). Actividad
as a mediator of the effects of low emotional intelligence on problem gambling. fsica en la tercera edad: Anlisis de sus relaciones con la inteligencia emocional
Journal of Gambling Studies, 22, 405411. percibida y los estilos de respuesta a la depresin. Ansiedad y Estrs, 12(23),
*Kee, K., Horan, W., Salovey, P., Kern, R., Sergi, M., Fiske, A., et al. (2009). Emotional 293303.
intelligence in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 107(1), 6168. *Repetto, E., Pena, M., & Lozano, S. (2006). Adaptacin y validacin espaola del
*Kemp, A., Stephan, B., Hopkinson, P., Sumich, A., Paul, R., Richard Clark, C., et al. emotional intelligence inventory (EII): Estudio piloto. Ansiedad y Estrs., 12(2
(2005). Toward an integrated prole of depression: Evidence from the brain 3), 181189.
resource international database. Journal of Integrative Neuroscience, 4(1), *Riley, H., & Schutte, N. (2003). Low emotional intelligence as a predictor of
95106. substance-use problems. Journal of Drug Education, 33, 391398.
*Latorre, J. M., & Montas, J. (2004). Ansiedad, inteligencia emocional y salud en la Rosenthal, R., & DiMatteo, M. R. (2001). Meta-analysis: Recent developments in
adolescencia. Ansiedad y Estrs, 10(1), 111125. quantitative methods for literature reviews. Annual Review of Psychology, 52,
Law, K. S., Wong, C. S., & Song, L. J. (2004). The construct and criterion validity of 5982.
emotional intelligence and its potential utility for management studies. Journal Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1986). Meta-analytic procedures for combining studies
of Applied Psychology, 89(3), 483496. with multiple effect sizes. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 400406.
*Leible, T. L., & Snell, W. E. (2004). Borderline personality disorder and multiple *Rossen, E., & Kranzler, J. H. (2009). Incremental validity of the Mayer
aspects of emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 37, SaloveyCaruso Emotional Intelligence Test Version 2.0 (MSCEIT) after
393404. controlling for personality and intelligence. Journal of Research in
*Limonero, J. T., Toms-Sbado, J., Fernndez-Castro, J. (2006). Perceived emotional Personality, 43, 6065.
intelligence and its relation to tobacco and cannabis use among university *Saklofske, D. H., Austin, E. J., Galloway, J., & Davidson, K. (2007). Individual
students. Psicothema, 18(suppl.), 95100. difference correlates of health-related behaviours: Preliminary evidence for
Malouff, J. M., Thorsteinsson, E., & Schutte, N. S. (2005). The relationship between links between emotional intelligence and coping. Personality and Individual
the ve-factor model of personality and clinical disorders: a meta-analysis. Differences, 42, 491502.
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 27, 101114. *Saklofske, D. H., Austin, E. J., & Minski, P. S. (2003). Factor structure and validity of a
*Martinez-Pons, M. (19971998). The relation of emotional intelligence with trait emotional intelligence measure. Personality and Individual Differences, 34,
selected areas of personal functioning. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 17, 707721.
313. Salovey, P., Mayer, J. D., Goldman, S., Turvey, C., & Palfai, T. (1995). Emotional
*Martinez-Pons, M. (19992000). Emotional intelligence as a self-regulatory attention, clarity, and repair: Exploring emotional intelligence using the trait
process: a social cognitive view. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 19, meta-mood scale. In J. W. Pennebaker (Ed.), Emotion, disclosure, and health
331350. (pp. 125154). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
*Matthews, G., Emo, A., Funke, G., Zeidner, M., Roberts, R., Costa, P. Jr.,, et al. (2006). *Salovey, P., Stroud, L. R., Woolery, A., & Epel, E. S. (2002). Perceived emotional
Emotional intelligence, personality, and task-induced stress. Journal of intelligence, stress reactivity, and symptom reports: Further explorations using
Experimental Psychology: Applied, 12(2), 96107. the trait meta-mood scale. Psychology and Health, 17, 611627.
*Mavroveli, S., Petrides, K. V., Rieffe, C., & Bakker, F. (2007). Trait emotional *Snchez, M., Montas, J., Latorre, J., & Fernndez-Berrocal, P. (2006). Anlisis de
intelligence, psychological well-being and peer-rated social competence in las relaciones entre la inteligencia emocional percibida y la salud mental en la
adolescence. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25, 263275. pareja. Ansiedad y Estrs, 12(23), 343353.
Mayer, J. D., & Salovey, P. (1997). What is emotional intelligence? In P. Salovey & D. *Schmidt, J. E., & Andrykowski, M. A. (2004). The role of social and dispositional
Sluyter (Eds.), Emotional development and emotional intelligence. Educational variables associated with emotional processing in adjustment to breast cancer:
implications (pp. 331). New York: Basic Books. An internet-based study. Health Psychology, 23, 259266.
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (2002). Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., Thorsteinsson, E. B., Bhullar, N. Rooke, S. E. (2007). A
Intelligence Test (MSCEIT) users manual. Toronto, ON: Multi-Health Systems. meta-analytic investigation of the relationship between emotional intelligence
*Mikolajczak, M., Luminet, O., Leroy, C., & Roy, E. (2007). Psychometric properties of and health Personality and Individual Differences, 42(6), 921933.
the trait emotional intelligence questionnaire: Factor structure, reliability, *Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., Hall, L. E., Haggerty, D. J., Cooper, J. T., Golden, C. J.,
construct, and incremental validity in a French-speaking population. Journal of et al. (1998). Development and validation of a measure of emotional
Personality Assessment, 88(3), 338353. intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 25, 167177.
*Mikolajczak, M., Luminet, O., & Menil, C. (2006). Predicting resistance to stress: *Shulman, T., & Hemenover, S. (2006). Is dispositional emotional intelligence
Incremental validity of trait emotional intelligence over alexithymia and synonymous with personality? Self and Identity, 5(2), 147171.
optimism. Psicothema, 18, 7988. *Slaski, M., & Cartwright, S. (2002). Health, performance and emotional intelligence.
Mikolajczak, M., Petrides, K. V., Coumans, N., & Luminet, O. (2009a). The moderating An exploratory study of retail managers. Stress and Health, 18, 6368.
effect of trait emotional intelligence on mood deterioration following *Summerfeldt, L. J., Kloosterman, P. H., Antony, M. M., & Parker, J. D. A. (2006). Social
laboratory-induced stress. International Journal of Clinical and Health anxiety, emotional intelligence, and interpersonal adjustment. Journal of
Psychology, 9, 455477. Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 28, 5768.
*Mikolajczak, M., Petrides, K. V., & Hurry, J. (2009b). Adolescents choosing self-harm Tapia, M. (2001). Measuring emotional intelligence. Psychological Reports, 88,
as an emotion regulation strategy: The protective role of trait emotional 353364.
intelligence. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 48, 181193. *Trinidad, D. R., & Johnson, C. A. (2002). The association between emotional
*Montes-Berges, B., & Augusto-Landa, J. (2007). Exploring the relationship between intelligence and early adolescent tobacco and alcohol use. Personality and
perceived emotional intelligence, coping, social support and mental health in Individual Differences, 32, 95105.
564 A. Martins et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 49 (2010) 554564

*Tsaousis, I., & Nikolaou, I. (2005). Exploring the relationship of emotional intelligence Van Rooy, D. L., & Viswesvaran, C. (2004). Emotional intelligence. A meta-analytic
with physical and psychological health functioning. Stress and Health, 21, 7786. investigation of predictive validity and nomological net. Journal of Vocational
*Uva, M. C. D., de Timary, P., Cortesi, M., Mikolajczak, M., de Blicquy, P. D., & Luminet, Behavior, 65, 7195.
O. (2010). Moderating effect of emotional intelligence on the role of negative Van Rooy, D. L., Viswesvaran, C., & Pluta, P. (2005). An evaluation of construct
affect in the motivation to drink in alcohol-dependent subjects undergoing validity: What is this thing called emotional intelligence? Human Performance,
protracted withdrawal. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 1621. 18(4), 445462.

You might also like