You are on page 1of 8

International Journal of Psychology, 2013

Vol. 48, No. 3, 308315, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00207594.2011.645483

Individual and parental factors related to meaning


in life among Hungarian minority adolescents
from Romania

Laszlo Brassai1, Bettina F. Piko2, and Michael F. Steger3


1
Psychological Consulting Center, Saint George, Romania
2
Department of Behavioral Sciences, University of Szeged, Szeged, Hungary
3
Colorado State University, Department of Psychology, Fort Collins, CO, USA

U nderstanding how adolescents achieve meaning in life has important implications for their psychological
development. A social cognitive model of meaning development was tested by assessing psychological (self-
efficacy, self-regulation and social comparison) and parental (parental responsiveness, demandingness, and social
support) variables in a sample of 1944 adolescents (aged 1519 years; 47.8% males) from secondary schools of the
Middle Transylvanian Region, Romania. Both psychological and parental factors were significantly related to
meaning in life. For both boys and girls, self-efficacy, self-regulation, and maternal responsiveness related
positively with meaning in life, and paternal demandingness related inversely to meaning in life. However, social
comparison related positively to meaning only among boys, and paternal responsiveness related positively to
meaning only among girls. Results point to a possible meaning-supporting role played by social cognitive
variables, as well as parental autonomy support. The gender differences observed here suggest that existing
theories of meaning development may need to be elaborated to include family of origin and gender.

Keywords: Meaning in life; Adolescence; Psychological (socio-cognitive) factors; Parental factors.

C omprendre comment les adolescents donnent un sens a` la vie a des implications importantes pour leur
developpement psychologique. Un mode`le social cognitif du developpement du sens a ete teste en evaluant
des variables psychologiques (efficacite personnelle, autoregulation et comparaison sociale) et parentales (reponse
parentale, exigence parentale et soutien social). Letude fut realisee aupre`s dun echantillon de 1944 adolescents
(agees entre 15 et 19 ans; 47,8% de garcons) provenant decoles secondaires de la region centrale de la
Transylvanie en Roumanie. A` la fois les facteurs psychologiques et parentaux etaient significativement associes
au sens a` la vie. Pour les garcons et les filles, lefficacite personnelle, lautoregulation et la reponse maternelle
etaient positivement reliees au sens a` la vie, tandis que lexigence paternelle etait negativement reliee au sens a` la
vie. Cependant, la comparaison sociale etait positivement associee au sens a` la vie seulement chez les garcons,
alors que la reponse paternelle etait positivement reliee au sens a` la vie seulement chez les filles. Les resultats
reve`lent que les variables cognitives sociales, tout comme le soutien parental a` lautonomie, joueraient
possiblement un role sur les plans de la signification et du soutien. Les differences liees au genre observees dans
cette etude sugge`rent que les theories actuelles du developpement du sens peuvent necessiter davantage
delaboration dans le but dy inclure la famille dorigine et le genre.

L a comprension acerca de como los adolescentes alcanzan el significado de la vida tiene consecuencias
importantes para su desarrollo psicologico. Se puso a prueba un modelo sociocognitivo del desarrollo del
significado por medio de la evaluacion de variables psicologicas (autoeficacia, autorregulacion y comparacion
social) y parentales (capacidad de respuesta parental, demandas o exigencia y apoyo social) en una muestra de
1944 adolescentes (de entre 15 y 19 anos de edad; 47,8% varones) de los colegios secundarios de la Region
Central de Transilvania, Rumania. Tanto los factores psicologicos como los parentales se relacionaron
significativamente con el sentido de la vida. Para los adolescentes de ambos sexos, la autoeficacia, la
autorregulacion y la capacidad de respuesta materna se relaciono positivamente con el sentido de la vida, y la

Correspondence should be addressed to Laszlo Brassai, Psychological Consulting Center, 520023 Victor Babes street 15/C,
Kovaszna County, Saint George, Romania. (E-mail: brassail@yahoo.com).

2013 International Union of Psychological Science


FACTORS RELATED TO MEANING IN LIFE IN ADOLESCENCE 309

exigencia paterna se relaciono inversamente con el significado de la vida. Sin embargo, la comparacion social se
relaciono positivamente con el sentido de la vida solo en los varones, mientras que la respuesta paterna solamente
se relaciono positivamente con el sentido de la vida en las chicas. Los resultados apuntan a una posible funcion de
soporte para el sentido de la vida de parte de las variables sociocognitivas, como as tambien del apoyo a la
autonom a por parte de los padres. Las diferencias de genero observadas en este art culo sugieren que las teor as
existentes del desarrollo de significado necesitar an ser revisadas para incluir a la familia de origen y el genero.

According to Steger, Frazier, Oishi, and Kaler, in life, and substance abuse (Newcomb & Harlow,
(2006), meaning in life [is] the sense made of, and 1986).
significance felt regarding, the nature of ones Despite the importance of meaning in life to
being and existence (p. 81). Meaning in this sense adolescents, the question of how meaning develops
enables people to interpret and organize their is difficult to answer. Integrative models of how
experience, achieve a sense of their own worth and meaning in life develops over the lifespan have
place, identify the things that matter to them, and only recently been proposed (although ideas
effectively direct their energies. Thus in the present specific to purpose have been articulated; Damon
study, we focused on the overarching concept of et al., 2003). The purpose of the present study is to
meaning, which we understand to incorporate continue to elaborate, and empirically test, a social
both comprehension and purpose (Steger, 2009). cognitive model of meaning development. Steger,
(It is worth mentioning that in another view, Bundick, and Yeager (in press) argue that three
whereas meaning in life refers to all highly valued social-cognitive factors should play leading roles in
life goals, purpose refers only to a subset of life adolescent meaning development: cognitive devel-
goals that represent a concern with issues beyond opment, identity and self-understanding, and
the self: Bronk & Finch, 2010; Damon, Menon, & social learning. In this social cognitive perspective,
Bronk, 2003). meaning is seen to arise from adolescents abilities
to navigate the social world and develop self-
regulatory resources, and establish autonomy. To
MEANING FORMATION IN
further specify the discrete social cognitive vari-
ADOLESCENCE
ables suggested by this model, we draw upon
earlier work by Fry (1998).
The construction of meaning in life starts quite
Fry (1998) argued that adolescents must develop
early in development, and is a powerful motivating
both psychological (e.g., life skills for effective
force in the identity formation of adolescents
coping and adapting to societal expectancies) and
(Erikson, 1968). It is thought that adolescents
achieve identities by integrating the myriad, and social (i.e., peers and family) assets to attain
sometimes contradictory, information generated meaning in life. Using qualitative methods, Fry
by different sources in their environment in order found that meaning in life was connected to an
to create a comprehensive image of the world intrinsic need for environmental mastery, which
around them. Damon and colleagues (2003) have would be pursued through developing a sense of
suggested that successful moral development of self-efficacy and effective self-regulation. Further,
adolescents requires an additional development of young people seemed to express a need to maintain
valued life goals (which should ideally reflect a self-esteem through constructive self-enhancing
desire to make a positive impact on others). Thus, evaluations. This research provides ways of
identity and moral development should proceed expanding on the components identified by
through making sense of the world and finding Steger and colleagues (in press). We will first
purpose, dovetailing with theories of meaning in focus on connecting the primarily psychological
life. Empirical research generally supports a close variables Fry identified with the social cognitive
relationship between meaning in life and positive model. These include self-efficacy, self-regulation,
youth development (Beaumont, 2009). Meaning in and social comparison.
life is related to a wide range of indicators of Self-efficacy has previously been identified as a
positive psychological functioning (quality of life, need for meaning (Baumeister, 1991). Self-
psychological wellbeing, low psychosomatic symp- efficacy refers to an optimistic belief in ones own
toms) in adolescence (Brassai, Piko, & Steger, abilities to accomplish tasks and goals (Schwarzer
2011). Accordingly, adolescents who fail to & Jerusalem, 1995). The view that one can
develop meaning in life appear susceptible to accomplish goals is relevant to adolescents per-
negative experiences, such as a lost sense of control ceptions that they can, and ought to, pursue a
310 BRASSAI, PIKO, STEGER

valued life purpose. Although self-efficacy is meaning in life stress the individually constructed
typically assessed in terms of discrete domains nature of the variable, we focused on parental
such as school or work, global self-efficacy is more factors that impact autonomy development among
relevant because a life purpose is broad, complex, adolescents: parental support, and parental
and not constrained to a single domain of life demandingness and responsiveness (Baumrind,
(Skrabski, Kopp, Rozsa, Rethelyi, & Rahe, 2005). 1991). Parental support captures the extent to
Thus, global self-efficacy should help build a which an adolescent feels appreciated and encour-
foundation that gives an adolescent the confidence aged by his or her parents. Responsiveness refers to
to apply effort toward a life purpose. Whereas self- the extent to which parents are warm, accepting,
efficacy should provide the confidence to apply and involved. Demandingness refers to the extent
effort, self-regulation captures the controlled to which parents exert control, demand maturity,
deployment of that effort. Self-regulation refers and actively supervise their children. In the midst of
to intentionally direct effort toward particular identity formation, when instability and identity
objectives, whether by regulating time, attention, variability are at their peak, parental support and
or behavioral responses. For example, self-regulat- responsiveness may improve adolescents auton-
ing attention helps people attend to meaningful omy and authentic self-expression. On the other
environmental stimuli without being distracted by hand, demandingness may attenuate autonomy, as
irrelevant stimuli (Luszczynska, Diehl, Gutierrez- parents exert control and enforce their expectations
Dona, Kuusinen, & Schwarzer, 2004). By devel- about behavior, rather than accepting their childs
oping this ability, adolescents should be able to self-expression. Whereas parental support and
maintain attention on resources that help them responsiveness should foster meaning in life
progress toward their life purpose, and better resist (Lambert et al., 2010), demandingness should
distractions and pitfalls. Self-efficacy and self- inhibit positive psychological functioning (Deci &
regulation work reciprocally, and together help Ryan, 2000; Shek, 2007). Taken together, these
inform identity and self-understanding (Steger results lead us to hypothesize that the psychological
et al., in press). Thus, self-efficacy and self- assets of self-efficacy, self-regulation, and social
regulation dovetail both with Steger and collea- comparison practices, as well as parental
gues (in press) emphasis on identity and assets of support and responsiveness, would be
self-understanding and with Frys (1998) emphasis positively related to meaning in life, whereas
on environmental mastery. In addition, research demandingness would be negatively related to
supports the positive association of self-regulatory meaning in life.
variables (life control) and meaning in life in
adolescence (Newcomb & Harlow, 1986).
As several scholars have noted, meaning origi- GENDER AND CULTURE
nates in early interpersonal relations and social
transactions (Fry, 1998; Steger et al., in press). Gender and culture have been implicated in the
Much of this meaning-making is derived from basic processes of meaning in life (e.g., Steger,
internalized mental models of important others, Kawabata, Shimai, & Otake, 2008). Cross-cultural
and meaning-making is particularly fostered when research on meaning has focused on comparing
these models encourage engagement with the countries from different extremes of individualism
world (Tronick & Beeghly, 2011). One way and collectivism (e.g., Steger et al., 2008). In
adolescents make meaning is through social collectivistic societies, social contexts and roles
comparison, which arises from motivations to enforce more constraints than in individualistic
evaluate, improve, and enhance the self (e.g., societies. The primary literature on gender, par-
Gibbons & Buunk, 1999). Thus, through social enting, and meaning comes from a collectivistic
comparison, adolescents may increase their self- culture (i.e., Hong Kong). In this research,
understanding and engage in social learning parenting practices (particularly demandingness)
(Steger et al., in press). Social comparisons provide were found to more strongly impact girls psycho-
a critical link between the psychological and the logical functioning than boys (Shek, 2007). There
social assets that Fry identified. We next discuss is reason to believe that socially and morally
the social assets we focused on. correct behavior, transmitted through parental
Among potential social partners, theory practices, receives more emphasis for girls than
(Tronick & Beeghly, 2011) and research (Lambert boys, even beyond the specific cultural context of
et al., 2010) point toward the primary importance Sheks study (2004). This fits with an interpreta-
of parents. Parents influence their children in tion that boys are more likely to be supported in
multiple ways, of course. Because theories of fostering uniqueness, autonomy, and social
FACTORS RELATED TO MEANING IN LIFE IN ADOLESCENCE 311
TABLE 1
Descriptive statistics for meaning in life and related factors

Whole sample Boys Girls


Factors (N 1944) (N 929) (N 1015) Significancea Effect sizeb

Meaning in life: M (SD) (0) 8.7 (2.2) (.86) 13.8 (2.1) 13.9 (2.1) NS .05
Parental factors: M (SD) (0)
Mothers responsiveness 14.6 (4.1) (.96) 14.7 (3.5) 14.8 (3.3) NS .03
Mothers demandingness 7.5 (4.3) (.85) 6.6 (4.0) 7.7 (3.9) p 5 .001 .27
Fathers responsiveness 13.0 (5.2) (.98) 13.8 (4.2) 12.4 (3.9) p 5 .001 .35
Fathers demandingness 6.5 (4.8) (.95) 5.4 (4.3) 6.7 (5.1) NS .07
Satisfaction with social support from mother 13.6 (4.2) (.99) 13.5 (4.1) 13.9 (4.0) p 5 .05 .10
Satisfaction with social support from father 11.6 (5.2) (.99) 12.2 (4.8) 11.7 (4.7) p 5 .05 .11
Psychological factors: M (SD) (0)
Perceived self-efficacy 18.5 (5.5) (.94) 19.8 (5.9) 17.8 (4.1) p 5 .001 .40
Self-regulatory behavior 11.7 (4.2) (.85) 12.9 (3.9) 10.4 (3.6) p 5 .001 .40
Social comparison 22.3 (8.1) (.79) 23.4 (7.4) 21.2 (7.1) p 5 .001 .30
a
Students t-tests; beffect size was measured by Cohens d: .20 small effect, .50 medium effect, .80 large effect.

competence (Luszczynska et al., 2004), which may consisted of 1944 students who met age criteria
drive higher interest in social comparison for boys (1519 years of age; Mage 16.8 years, SD 1.0),
than for girls (Kemmelmeier & Oyserman, 2001). 52.2% of whom were female.
Thus the social cognitive variables discussed above
may have differential relations with meaning in life
depending on gender. Procedure
The purpose of the present study is to further
Parental permissions were obtained prior to the
elaborate, and test, a social cognitive model of
start of the study. Students were given a brief
meaning in life in adolescence. We assessed key
explanation of the objectives of the study and
psychological and parental variables in a large
instructions for filling out the questionnaire.
sample of collectivistic Eastern European adoles-
Participation in the study was voluntary, and
cents, hypothesizing that psychological assets that
confidentiality was ensured.
promote self-understanding and social learning
(i.e., self-efficacy, self-regulation, social compar-
ison) and parental assets that promoted autonomy Measures
development (i.e., high support, high responsive-
ness, low demandingness) would be related to Only scales that had been validated in
meaning in life. We further speculated that Hungarian speaking population samples were
parental support (which promotes autonomy used in the present study. Cronbachs alpha
development) might be particularly important for coefficients for all scales are presented in Table 1.
boys, and parental responsiveness and demand- Meaning in life was measured by the Life
ingness might be particularly important for girls Meaning (LM) subscale of the Brief Stress and
(cf. Shek, 2004). Coping Inventory (BSCI) developed by Rahe and
Tolles (2002). The LM is the only measure of
meaning in life that has been validated in a
METHOD representative Hungarian-speaking sample
(Skrabski et al., 2005). The eight items on the
Sample LM are consistent with those on other meaning in
life questionnaires, with most being centrally
Data were collected from students enrolled in the focused on prevalent notions of meaning (e.g.,
secondary schools of the Middle Transylvanian I feel my life is part of a larger plan; My life
(Hungarian ethnic majority) Region, Romania. has no direction and meaning; I doubt that my
Multistage sampling was used, first identifying life makes a difference). Items are rated from 1
towns, then high schools, and finally randomly (No) to 3 (Frequently); high scores indicate high
selecting classes, yielding a subject pool of 2152 life meaning.
students, of whom 1977 returned the questionnaire The General Perceived Self-Efficacy Scale (SE;
(response rate 91.9%). The final sample Schwarzer & Jerusalem, 1995) was used to measure
312 BRASSAI, PIKO, STEGER

global confidence in ones ability to deal with the deviation. There was no significant multi-
challenges (e.g., I can always manage to solve collinearity (variance inflation factor from 1.20 to
difficult problems if I try hard enough). Ten items 1.96), nor significant outliers (standard residuals
are rated from 1 (Not at all true) to 4 (Exactly from 1.89 to 2.73).
true). High scores indicate greater self-efficacy.
The Self-Regulation Scale (SRS; Luszczynska
et al., 2004) was used to measure self-regulation of Descriptive statistics
attention (e.g., I can control my thoughts from
Boys reported higher levels of self-efficacy, self-
distracting me from the task at hand). Seven
regulation, and social comparisons, as well as
items are rated from 1 (Not at all true) to 4
greater satisfaction with social support from
(Completely true). High scores indicate greater
father, more fathers responsiveness, less maternal
self-regulation.
demandingness, and satisfaction with maternal
The IowaNetherlands Comparison Orientation
social support (see Table 1).
Measure (INCOM; Gibbons & Buunk, 1999) was
used to measure the tendency to compare oneself
to others in different life areas (e.g., I always pay Multiple regressions
a lot of attention to how I do things compared
with how others do things). Eleven items were To assess predictors of meaning, parental factors
rated from 1 (I disagree strongly) to 5 (I agree were entered in Step 1 of a multiple regression,
strongly). High scores indicate greater tendencies followed by psychological factors in Step 2. Across
for social comparison. the full sample, all factors were significantly
The Measures of Perceived Social Support related to meaning with moderate effect sizes
(MPSS; Turner & Marino, 1994) was used to (|.16| to |.23|), except for maternal demandingness
assess parental social support. Satisfaction with (see Table 2), accounting for 38% of the variance
maternal and with paternal social support were in meaning in life scores. Only fathers demand-
assessed using six item scales (e.g. I feel very close ingness was negatively related to meaning in life;
to my mother/father; I often feel really appre- all other variables were positively related.
ciated by my mother/father). Items were rated When boys and girls were examined separately,
from 1 (Not at all like my experience) to 4 (Very two differences emerged. First, social comparisons
much like my experience). High scores indicate were positively related to meaning in life only
greater perceived maternal/paternal support. among boys. Second, paternal responsiveness,
The Authoritative Parenting Index (Jackson, andunlike our hypothesisparental support
Henriksen, & Foshee, 1998) was used to assess was more significant for girls meaning in life but
Baumrinds (1991) dimensions of parenting: paternal demandingness was not.
demandingness and responsiveness. Both maternal
and paternal social support was assessed using
20-item scales (e.g., maternal responsiveness: She DISCUSSION
likes me just the way I am; maternal demanding-
ness: She has rules that I must follow). Items Adolescence is a formative period for cultivating
were rated from 1 (Not like her/him) to 4 (Just like meaning in life (Beaumont, 2009; Erikson, 1968).
her/him). High scores indicate greater responsive- As recent social cognitive models have argued
ness or demandingness. (Steger, 2009; Steger et al., in press), to succeed in
this effort, adolescents need to find and pursue
valued goals, develop a coherent and authentic
RESULTS self-understanding, learn to direct effort and
attention toward important aims, and establish
Statistical analysis autonomy (Deci & Ryan, 2000). This study
represents the first effort to test key facets of a
Gender differences on scale means were computed coherent theoretical model for how meaning in life
using Students t-tests. Interrelationships between develops among adolescents. Previous research has
psychological factors, parental factors and mean- been typified by ad hoc collections of variables,
ing in life were assessed by multiple regression and reporting based on bivariate relations, requir-
analysis for whole sample, and for boys and girls ing integration both in theory and in empirical
separately. After analyzing the normality of testing. The present study builds on emerging
distribution (with Kolmogorov-Smirnov statistics) social cognitive models of meaning in life, and also
we applied logarithmic transformation to decrease integrates key indicators of psychological and
FACTORS RELATED TO MEANING IN LIFE IN ADOLESCENCE 313
TABLE 2
Regression analyses predicting meaning in life including parental and individual factors

Whole sample (N 1944) Boys (N 929) Girls (N 1015)

a SEb VIFc a SEb VIFc a SEb VIFc

Parental factors
Mothers responsiveness .17*** .01 1.0 .15* .02 1.4 .16** .03 1.5
Mothers demandingness .05 .06 2.1 .05 .04 2.6 .07 .01 1.2
Fathers responsiveness .16** .02 1.9 .08 .01 1.3 .18*** .04 1.2
Fathers demandingness .18*** .03 1.5 .14* .03 1.7 .14* .01 1.0
Satisfaction with social support from mother .20*** .02 1.3 .16** .03 1.3 .16** .03 1.2
Satisfaction with social support from father .20*** .03 1.0 .14* .02 1.0 .19*** .03 1.3
DR2 .18*** .14*** .23***
Psychological factors
Perceived self-efficacy .21*** .03 1.1 .18*** .04 1.7 .20*** .02 1.8
Self-regulatory behavior .23*** .02 1.3 .23*** .03 1.9 .16** .02 1.9
Social comparison .17*** .01 1.1 .17*** .03 1.6 .09 .05 2.9
DR2 .20*** .19*** .12***

Constant 26.910*** 17.714*** 20.017***


R2 .38*** .34*** .35***

*p 5 .05; **p 5 .01; ***p 5 .001. aStandardized regression coefficients; bstandard error; cvariance inflation factor (VIF): from 1 to
2 low multicollinearity, from 2 to 5 medium multicollinearity, 4 5 strong multicollinearity; changes in R2.

parental contributors to meaning in multivariate we speculated that parental support would be


analysis for the first time. more important for supporting boys meaning and
Results from a large sample of Romanian that parental responsiveness and demandingness
adolescents linked both sets of variables to would be more important for girls meaning. Our
adolescent meaning in life. As hypothesized, self- results provided only partial support for these
efficacy and self-regulation (and social comparison hypotheses. For example, paternal demandingness
among boys) were positively related to meaning in was negatively related to meaning in life for both
life. In addition, feeling that ones parents were boys and girls, and maternal demandingness was
supportive and responsive was positively related to unrelated to meaning in life. Another unexpected
meaning in life. The variables assessed in the result was that social comparison was related to
present study are linked conceptually to the two meaning only among boys.
major components of meaning in life: comprehen- These results should be interpreted carefully in
sion and purpose. The parental variables of light of the limited range of cultures in which
support and responsiveness and the psychological similar research has been carried out. One way of
variable, social comparison, should help adoles- interpreting the results is by focusing on the gender
cents build a positively valenced mental model of of the adolescent. Social comparison may empha-
the world around them, which supports compre- size ways in which boys are unique and self-
hension. The psychological variables of self- expressive, and may instead highlights points of
efficacy and self-regulation bear on adolescents deviation from others in girls (Gibbons & Buunk,
confidence in pursuing important goals and their 1999; Kemmelmeier & Oyserman, 2001). The
abilities to direct their attention and effort toward difference in emphasis may augment the benefits
those aims, which supports purpose. of positive social comparisons for boys and
We attempted to predict how these variables neutralize any benefits for girls.
would differentially impact boys and girls based on However, another way to look at our results is
the small amount of existing literature. In research in terms of the gender of parents. Self-determina-
on Hong Kong adolescents, Shek (2004, 2007) tion theory views autonomy development as a
found that parenting impacted girls more than critical factor in successful development and the
boys. Other research found that parents often achievement of wellbeing (Deci & Ryan, 2000).
placed more constraints on girls, and appeared to Parenting that inhibits autonomy development
provide more autonomy support for boys should be toxic to meaning, and parenting that
(Luszczynska et al., 2004). Based on this research, promotes autonomy should nourish meaning. In
314 BRASSAI, PIKO, STEGER

the present sample, one of the points of conver- REFERENCES


gence for both boys and girls was that paternal
demandingness was negatively related to meaning Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Meanings of Life. New York:
in life. For fathers, autonomy interference may Guilford.
Baumrind, D. (1991). Parenting styles and adolescent
detract from meaning for both sons and daughters. development. In R. Lerner, A. C. Petersen &
The other point of convergence was that maternal J. Brooks-Gunn (Eds.), The encyclopedia of adoles-
responsiveness was positively related to meaning cence (pp. 746758). New York, NY: Garland.
for both boys and girls. While mothers appear to Beaumont, S. L. (2009). Identity processing and per-
play a nurturing role in terms of adolescents sonal wisdom: An information-oriented identity style
predicts self-actualization and self-transcendence.
striving for autonomy, fathers may restrict it.
Identity, 9, 95115.
Correspondingly, maternal demandingness was Brassai, L., Piko, B. F., & Steger, M. F. (2011). Meaning
unrelated to meaning in life. However, in contrast in life: Is it a protective factor for adolescents
to this relatively rigid view, fathers responsiveness psychological health? International Journal of
was positive for girls. One possibility is that Behavior Medicine, 18, 4451.
nurturance is typically left to mothers and Bronk, K. C., & Finch, W. H. (2010). Adolescent
characteristics by type of long-term aim in life.
discipline is left to fathers in Romania. When Applied Developmental Science, 14, 110.
parents embrace their respective roles to an Damon, W., Menon, J., & Bronk, K. C. (2003). The
extreme degree, it may interfere with typical development of purpose during adolescence. Applied
adolescent development. When responsive fathers Developmental Science, 7, 119128.
allow autonomy development for daughters, it Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The what and
why of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-
goes against the cultural inclination and provides determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11,
benefits. The present findings further highlight the 227268.
impact of both mothers and fathers on child Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity: Youth and crisis.
wellbeing, especially for fathers and their daugh- NewYork, NY: Norton.
ters (see also Shek, 2004). More research on Fry, P. M. (1998). The development of personal mean-
ing and wisdom in adolescence: A reexamination of
parenting patterns in Eastern European contexts
moderating and consolidating factor and influences.
is needed to determine whether these results In P. T. P. Wong & P. M. Fry (Eds.), The human
generalize to other populations, and whether our quest for meaning: A handbook of psychological
interpretation of them is tenable. research and clinical application (pp. 91110).
These results must be interpreted in the light of Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
several limitations. The present research is limited Gibbons, F. X., & Buunk, B. P. (1999). Individual
differences in social comparison: Development of a
by the use of cross-sectional, self-report methods. scale of social comparison orientation. Journal of
In addition, the specific cultural context (Eastern Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 129142.
Europe) may limit the generalizability of the Jackson, C., Henriksen, L., & Foshee, V. A. (1998). The
present data. However, this may also be a strength, Authoritative Parenting Index: Predicting health-risk
as the present research supports previous findings behaviors among children and adolescents. Health
Education and Behavior, 25, 321339.
from other cultural contexts. Future research Kemmelmeier, M., & Oyserman, D. (2001). Gendered
should use longitudinal research designs, in other influence of downward social comparison on current
cultural contexts, to examine the temporal rela- and possible selves. Journal of Social Issues, 57,
tions among these psychological and parental 129148.
variables, taking account of gender differences. Lambert, N. M., Stillman, T. F., Baumeister, R. F.,
In addition, other important psychological vari- Fincham, F. D., Hicks, J. A., & Graham, S. M.
(2010). Family as a salient source of meaning in
ables should be included in future research, young adulthood. Journal of Positive Psychology, 5,
such as hope, and the role of nonparental mentor- 367376.
ing and peer relationships in meaning develop- Luszczynska, A., Diehl, M., Gutierrez-Dona, B.,
ment. Based on the present results, the social Kuusinen, P., & Schwarzer, R. (2004). Measuring
cognitive model of meaning development among one component of dispositional self-regulation:
Attention control in goal pursuit. Personality and
adolescents (Steger et al., in press) appears viable. Individual Differences, 37, 555566.
Future research should focus on identifying the Newcomb, M. D., & Harlow, L. L. (1986). Life events
causal mechanisms through which these psycho- and substance use among adolescents: Mediating
logical and parental factors contribute to meaning effects of perceived loss of control and meaningless-
in life. ness in life. Journal of Personality & Social
Psychology, 51, 564577.
Rahe, R. H., & Tolles, R. L. (2002). The Brief Stress and
Manuscript received November 2010
Coping Inventory: A useful stress management
Revised manuscript accepted August 2011
instrument. International Journal of Stress
First published online March 2012
Management, 9, 6170.
FACTORS RELATED TO MEANING IN LIFE IN ADOLESCENCE 315
Schwarzer, R., & Jerusalem, M. (1995). Generalized self- Steger, M. F., Bundick, M. J., & Yeager, D. (in press).
efficacy scale. In J. Weinman, S. Wright & Understanding and promoting meaning in life
M. Johnston (Eds.), Measures in health psychology: during adolescence. In R. J. R. Levesque (Ed.),
A users portfolio. Causal and control beliefs Encyclopedia of adolescence. New York, NY:
(pp. 3537). Windsor, UK: NFER-Nelson. Springer.
Shek, D. T. L. (2004). Family processes and develop- Steger, M. F., Kawabata, Y., Shimai, S., & Otake, K.
mental outcomes in Chinese adolescents. Hong Kong (2008). The meaningful life in Japan and the
Journal of Pediatrics, 9, 316324. United States: Levels and correlates of meaning
Shek, D. T. L. (2007). A longitudinal study of perceived in life. Journal of Research in Personality, 42,
parental psychological control and psychological 660678.
well-being in Chinese adolescents in Hong Kong. Steger, M. F., Frazier, P., Oishi, S., & Kaler, M. (2006).
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 63, 120. The Meaning in Life Questionnaire: Assessing the
Skrabski, A., Kopp, M., Rozsa, S., Rethelyi, J., & Rahe, presence of and search for meaning in life. Journal of
R. H. (2005). Life meaning: An important correlate Counseling Psychology, 53, 8093.
of health in the Hungarian population. International Tronick, E., & Beeghly, M. (2011). Infants meaning-
Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 12, 7885. making and the development of mental health
Steger, M. F. (2009). Meaning in life. problems. American Psychologist, 66, 107119.
In S. J. Lopez (Ed.), Oxford handbook of positive Turner, R. J., & Marino, F. (1994). Social support
psychology (2nd ed., pp. 679687). Oxford, UK: and social structure. Journal of Health and Social
Oxford University Press. Behavior, 35, 193212.

You might also like