You are on page 1of 8

Psychiatry Research 216 (2014) 325332

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Psychiatry Research
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/psychres

Empathy, depressive symptoms, and social functioning among


individuals with schizophrenia
Amy C. Abramowitz, Emily J. Ginger, Jackie K. Gollan, Matthew J. Smith n
Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, 446 E. Ontario, Suite 7-100, Chicago, IL 60611, USA

art ic l e i nf o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Empathy decits have been associated with schizophrenia and depression. We compared whether
Received 17 July 2013 individuals with schizophrenia with and without co-occurring depressive symptoms differed on self-
Received in revised form reported and performance-based measures of empathy and social functioning. We also examined the
13 February 2014
relationships among depressive symptoms, empathy, clinical symptoms, and social functioning. Twenty-
Accepted 19 February 2014
Available online 28 February 2014
eight individuals with schizophrenia and depressive symptoms, 32 individuals with schizophrenia
without depressive symptoms, and 44 control subjects were compared on assessments of depressive
Keywords: symptoms, empathy, global neurocognition, clinical symptoms, and social functioning. Both groups of
Schizophrenia individuals with schizophrenia scored higher than controls on the Interpersonal Reactivity Index
Empathy
personal distress subscale. Individuals with schizophrenia and co-occurring depressive symptoms scored
Depressive symptoms
signicantly higher than individuals with schizophrenia without depressive symptoms on the personal
Social functioning
Personal distress distress subscale. Personal distress and depressive symptoms were signicantly correlated among
individuals with schizophrenia and co-occurring depressive symptoms, while both measures negatively
correlated with social functioning. Emotional empathy was related to clinical symptoms in both groups
of individuals with schizophrenia. Personal distress partially mediated the relationship between co-
occurring depressive symptoms and social functioning. Personal distress may be an important implica-
tion for social functioning among individuals with schizophrenia and co-occurring depressive symptoms,
and should be examined further as a potential treatment target.
& 2014 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction and the selfother distinction that underlie cognitive empathy


(Shamay-Tsoory, 2011).
Empathy involves the ability to process emotional cues (verbal Individuals with schizophrenia are characterized by decits in
and nonverbal) displayed by others and consists of both emotional empathy across a range of methods, including self-report (Achim
and cognitive components (Shamay-Tsoory, 2011; Zaki and et al., 2011; Haker et al., 2012) and performance-based measures
Ochsner, 2011). The emotional component involves sharing and (Langdon et al., 2006; Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2007; Derntl et al.,
detecting the emotions displayed by others, and regulating the 2009; Smith et al., in press). Although the literature appears to be
emotional response to others, while the cognitive component mixed regarding the presence of emotional empathy decits in
involves understanding the emotional perspective of others and this group (Sparks et al., 2010; Achim et al., 2011; Smith et al.,
distinguishing between the feelings experienced by others from 2012), studies have consistently observed decits in cognitive
one's own (Decety, 2011). Cognitive empathy is similar to theory-of- empathy (Derntl et al., 2009; Sparks et al., 2010; Achim et al.,
mind (ToM), which can be dened as inferring the cognitive mental 2011; Smith et al., 2012). Moreover, recent studies demonstrated
states (i.e., beliefs and intentions) of others and using this informa- that cognitive empathy, but not emotional empathy, explained
tion to predict future behavior (Brune and Brune-Cohrs, 2006). unique variance in social functioning after accounting for neuro-
Although these constructs share some underlying neural mechan- cognitive decits and clinical symptoms (Smith et al., 2012; Smith
isms, they can be differentiated by the emotional understanding et al., in press).
Given that empathy explains unique variation in social function-
ing among individuals with schizophrenia (Smith et al., 2012; Smith
n
et al., in press), it is important to investigate clinical factors that
Correspondence to: Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine,
might exacerbate these empathy decits. One such factor could be
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Abbott Hall 13th Floor, Chicago,
IL 60611, USA. Tel.: 1 312 503 2542; fax: 1 312 5030 527. the presence of depressive symptoms, which occur more frequently
E-mail address: matthewsmith@northwestern.edu (M.J. Smith). among individuals with schizophrenia compared to the general

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2014.02.028
0165-1781 & 2014 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
326 A.C. Abramowitz et al. / Psychiatry Research 216 (2014) 325332

population (Lanon et al., 2001; Kessler et al., 2003; Buckley et al., The internal consistency of the IRI was generally acceptable but not strong for the
control subjects and individuals with schizophrenia (across both groups): fantasy
2009) and are associated with reduced functioning (Fervaha et al.,
( 0.76, 0.70, respectively), perspective-taking ( 0.73, 0.49, respec-
2013; Kasckow et al., 2010; Simonsen et al., 2010). Moreover, tively), empathic concern ( 0.79, 0.70, respectively), and personal distress
individuals with a non-psychotic major depressive disorder have ( 0.67, 0.63, respectively).
impairments in both emotional and cognitive empathy (Cusi et al.,
2011; Schreiter et al., 2013). Based on ndings of heightened 2.2.2. Performance-based empathy
depression among individuals with schizophrenia, depressive symp- Two computerized tasks from an adapted version of the Derntl paradigm
toms in this population could be related to exacerbated impairments (Derntl et al., 2009) assessed cognitive empathy with emotional perspective-taking
in empathy (Baez et al., 2013). and emotional empathy with affective responsiveness (Smith et al., in press). The
developers translated the directions and tasks from German into English and
In the current study, we hypothesized that (1) individuals with
worked with the principal investigator (MJS) to edit the text for readability. Each
schizophrenia and depressive symptoms (SCZDEP) would have task used a two alternative, forced-choice response format with standardized face
exacerbated impairments in the emotional and cognitive compo- stimuli (Gur et al., 2002) and took approximately 20 min to complete. Accuracy (i.e.,
nents of empathy as compared to individuals with schizophrenia percent correct) and response times (RT) were recorded for each task. Sample
and no depressive symptoms (SCZ); (2) SCZ DEP would stimuli from the two tasks can be found here (Smith et al., in press).

have poorer social functioning than SCZ; (3) among SCZ DEP,
(a) greater depressive symptoms would be related to impaired 2.2.2.1. Affective responsiveness. Participants judged how they would feel in various
empathy and lower social functioning; (b) impaired empathy would emotional scenarios presented as 150 brief sentences describing emotional (i.e.,
fear, anger, sadness, disgust, happiness, neutrality) and neutral situations (25 sti-
be related to poorer social functioning; (c) impaired empathy would
muli per emotion). The sentences were presented for 6 s and a response slide di-
mediate the relationship between depressive symptoms and social splaying two emotional faces was presented for a maximum of 4 s. Participants
functioning. We tested these hypotheses using a multimodal selected the emotional expression that reected how they would feel in the sce-
approach with self-report and performance-based measures of both nario; one face correctly depicted how most people would feel in the scenario and
empathy and social functioning. Lastly, we explored the relationship the other face was a randomized option.

between depressive symptoms and the clinical symptoms of schizo-


phrenia, duration of illness (years since onset of psychotic symp- 2.2.2.2. Emotional perspective-taking. Participants were shown 60 scenes displaying
toms), and antipsychotic medication treatment. two actors engaged in social interactions depicting happiness, sadness, anger, fear,
disgust and neutrality (10 scenes each). The face of one actor was masked and
participants were prompted to select which of the two facial expressions would
best reect how the masked character would feel in each interaction. Each social
2. Methods
interaction scene was displayed for 4 s and followed by a response slide that pre-
sented two faces for a maximum of 4 s. The emotional empathy task was completed
2.1. Participants prior to the cognitive empathy task.

Participants included 28 SCZ DEP, 32 SCZ, and 44 healthy controls (CON)


2.2.3. Social functioning measures
between 18 and 55 years of age. The Northwestern University Schizophrenia
Social competence was assessed using the Social Skills Performance Assess-
Research Group recruited all participants through outpatient mental health
ment, a video-recorded test comprised of two role-play scenes that involved
services, advertisements in surrounding neighborhoods, the National Alliance for
meeting a new neighbor and making a request from a landlord. Based on the work
Mental Illness, and online. The Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM-IV (SCID)
of Patterson et al. (2001), each scene was rated on a ve-point scale across eight
(First et al., 2002) was administered by MS and PhD-level research staff and used to
criteria for the rst scene and nine criteria for the second scene. A nal score was
determine a diagnosis of schizophrenia, other Axis-I disorders, and pharmacologi-
calculated by averaging the two role-play scores (ICC 0.97 for two blinded raters
cal treatment. The diagnoses were validated by a semi-structured interview
on 25% of the videos). Social attainment was assessed using the total score from a
performed by a research psychiatrist and by a review of available medical records.
participant interview version of the Specic Levels of Functioning (SLOF) scales,
Depressive symptoms were rated using the Montgomery Asberg Depression
which asks participants to consider their typical level of functioning in the areas of
Rating Scale (MADRS) (Montgomery and Asberg, 1979; Bondol et al., 2010) and
interpersonal relationships, social acceptability, activities of daily living, and work
based on observations during the SCID. Ten items were rated on a seven-point
skills (Schneider and Struening, 1983). The SSPA and SLOF have been previously
Likert scale with a possible range of 060. Using MADRS cutoff scores, 06 reected
validated as measures of functioning in individuals with schizophrenia (Patterson
a normal range of minor or absent depressive feelings, while scores of 719
et al., 2001; Harvey et al., 2007).
reected mild depressive symptoms, 2034 reected moderate depressive symp-
toms, and 35 reected severe depressive symptoms. A total MADRS score and
individual ratings for apparent sadness, total sadness, inner tension, reduced sleep, 2.2.4. Global neurocognition
reduced appetite, concentration difculties, lassitude, inability to feel, pessimistic We used a neuropsychological test battery to approximate the six non-social
thoughts, and suicidal thoughts were evaluated. The alpha reliability among cognitive domains in the Measurement and Treatment Initiative to Improve
SCZ DEP was acceptable ( 0.63). Reliabilities for CON and SCZ were Cognition in Schizophrenia battery (Marder and Fenton, 2004). Speed of processing
unacceptable (both o 0.30), but this nding is expected given that few subjects included Trail Making Test Part A (Reitan and Wolfson, 1985), category uency
in these two scored above 0 on this measure. All individuals with schizophrenia (animals) (Benton et al., 1976), and the Digit-Symbol Coding subtest from the
were clinically stable prior to study participation and treated with antipsychotic Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Third Edition (WAIS-III) (Wechsler, 1997a).
medications. Attention included the mean across the two, three, and four-item d0 scores from
Exclusion criteria for CON included having (1) a lifetime history of any Axis I a continuous performance task (Barch et al., 2004). Verbal Working Memory (WM)
psychiatric disorder according to SCID criteria or (2) a rst-degree relative with a included performance on Letter-Number Sequencing and Digit Span subtests from
psychotic disorder (including bipolar disorder). Additional exclusion criteria for the Wechsler Memory Scale-Third Edition (WMS-III) (Wechsler, 1997b). Non-verbal
both groups were: DSM-IV criteria for substance abuse or dependence within the WM included the Spatial Span subtest from the WMS-III (Wechsler, 1997b). Verbal
past 6 months or a documented neurological injury or disorder. Northwestern learning included the total score of trials 15 on the California Verbal Learning
University Feinberg School of Medicine's Institutional Review Board approved the Test-Second Edition (Delis et al., 1983). Reasoning and problem solving included
study protocol and all participants provided informed consent. The completion of scores from the Matrix Reasoning subtest of the WAIS-III (Wechsler, 1997a) and the
all study measures required approximately 56 h over the course of 23 visits. Trail Making Test Part B (Reitan and Wolfson, 1985). Each domain was computed by
standardizing raw or scaled scores from individual subtests using z-score transfor-
mations with the current sample, and then averaging these scores within the
2.2. Measures
domains. Global neurocognition was computed as an average across the six
domains scores.
2.2.1. Self-report empathy
The 28-item Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) (Davis, 1983) was used to
measure four areas theorized to reect empathy. Cognitive empathy was measured 2.2.5. Clinical measures
with fantasy (i.e., shifting one's feelings to ctional characters) and perspective- We used the global ratings from the Scale for the Assessment of Positive
taking (i.e., understanding the perspective of others), while emotional empathy Symptoms (Andreasen, 1983b) and the Scale for the Assessment of Negative
was measured with empathic concern (i.e., generating affective concern for others) Symptoms (Andreasen, 1983a) to provide us with positive, negative, and disorga-
and personal distress (i.e., becoming upset when others are in difcult situations). nized symptoms.
A.C. Abramowitz et al. / Psychiatry Research 216 (2014) 325332 327

2.3. Data analysis (all P40.10). Furthermore, they did not differ with respect to the
specic type of rst or second generation antipsychotic medication
We used an analysis of variance (ANOVAs) to examine between-group differences they were taking (all P40.10). The frequency of individuals treated
related to demographics, global neurocognition, clinical symptoms for continuous with antidepressant medication did not differ between SCZ and
variables and chi-square analyses for categorical variables. Between-group differences
were evaluated with ANOVA's for depressive symptoms, self-reported empathy (i.e., IRI
SCZDEP (P40.10).
empathic concern, IRI personal distress, IRI perspective-taking, IRI fantasy),
performance-based empathy (i.e., affective responsiveness, emotional perspective-tak-
ing), and social functioning (i.e., social competence, social attainment). We examined the
3.2. Depressive and clinical symptoms
relationships among the empathy, depressive symptoms, and social functioning
measures using Pearson correlations. Based on the hypothesized associations between Means and standard deviations for depressive and clinical symp-
empathy, depressive symptoms, and social functioning (Kasckow et al., 2010; Simonsen toms are presented in Table 2. There was a signicant effect of group
et al., 2010; Smith et al., 2012), we used the Preacher and Hayes (2004) bootstrapping
on the depressive symptoms total score (Po0.001) and each indivi-
technique to examine whether empathy mediated the relationship between depressive
symptoms and social functioning. This is a robust approach given the potential of small dually rated item (all Po0.01). MADRS total scores for SCZDEP were
samples to deviate from normality (Preacher & Hayes, 2004). Lastly, we also explored in a mild-to-moderate range (from 7 to 34) and were signicantly
the correlations between depressive symptoms and the clinical symptoms of schizo- higher than CON (scores ranged from 0 to 5) (all Po0.01) and SCZ
phrenia, duration of illness, and antipsychotic medication treatment, as well as between (scores ranged from 0 to 6) (all Po0.05). The distribution of depressive
empathy and clinical symptoms.
symptoms among SCZDEP was skewed to the left. Thus, we used a
square root transformation of this variable for correlation analyses.
Remaining study variables were normally distributed.
3. Results
SCZDEP had higher clinical symptom ratings than SCZ for
hallucinations (Po0.05), delusions (Po0.01), bizarre behavior
3.1. Participant characteristics
(Po0.01), avolition (Po0.05) and positive formal thought disorder
(P0.06) at the trend-level. The two groups did not differ with respect
Demographic, neurocognitive, and clinical characteristics are
to affective attening, alogia, anhedonia, and attention (all P40.10).
presented in Table 1. The groups did not signicantly differ in
terms of age, gender, socioeconomic status, and race (all P4 0.10).
There was a signicant effect of group on global neurocognitive 3.3. Self-reported empathy
function (F2,104 25.1, P o0.001). SCZ and SCZ DEP demonstrated
poorer global neurocognition than CON (both P o0.001), while Means and standard deviations for self-reported empathy are
SCZ and SCZ DEP did not differ (P 40.10). SCZ and SCZ DEP did presented in Table 3 and their scatterplots are displayed in
not signicantly differ with respect to duration of illness and Supplementary Fig. 1. There was a signicant effect of group on
treatment with rst or second-generation antipsychotic medications fantasy (F1,104 4.8, Po0.01), with CON and SCZ DEP having higher

Table 1
Demographic, Clinical, and cognitive characteristics of study sample.

CON (n 44) SCZ (n 32) SCZ DEP (n 28) F Statistic


Mean (S.D.) Mean (S.D.) Mean (S.D.)

Mean Age (S.D.) 32.8 (8.8) 35.8 (10.3) 34.9 (7.5) 1.1
Mean years since onset of psychotic symptoms (S.D.) 16.2 (10.2) 12.4 (7.7) 2.6
Mean Parental SES (S.D.) 26.9 (11.1) 22.0 (13.3) 26.9 (10.2) 1.9
Dose Years 1st Generation APa 0.2 (1.1) 0.5 (2.2)  0.5
Dose Years 2nd Generation AP 4.1 (3.3) 5.6 (4.1)  1.6
Global Cognitive Function 0.46 (0.55)  0.37 (0.61)  0.26 (0.51) 25.1nnnb

% % % 2 Statistic

Gender (% male) 52.3 68.8 57.1 2.1


Race % Caucasian 50.0 34.4 53.6 2.8
% African-American 36.4 49.6 35.7
% other 13.6 18.8 10.7
% with lifetime MDD diagnosis 0.0 12.5 39.3 21.5nnn
% Treated with AD Medication 31.3 50.0 2.2
1st Generation AP treatmenta,c 9.3 10.7 o 0.1
% Haloperidol 3.1 0.0
% Fluphenazine 3.1 0.0
% Perphenazine 0.0 3.6
2nd Generation AP treatmentc 93.8 96.4 0.2
% Risperidone 28.1 39.3
% Olanzapine 28.1 35.7
% Aripiprazole 25.0 21.4
% Quetiapine 18.8 17.9
% Clozapine 9.4 7.1
% Ziprasidone 6.3 7.1
% Paliperidone 3.1 7.1
% Asenapine 3.1 0.0

Abbreviations.: individuals with schizophrenia with no depressive symptoms (SCZ), individuals with schizophrenia and depressive symptoms (SCZ DEP), healthy controls
(CON), standard deviation (S.D.), socioeconomic status (SES), major depressive disorder (MDD), anti-depressant (AD), antipsychotic (AP).
nnn
Po 0.001.
a
n 2 SCZ and n 1 SCZ DEP were treated with FGA only.
b
CON4 SCZ, SCZ DEP.
c
Total percentages do not sum to 100% as some subjects were treated with more than one medication.
328 A.C. Abramowitz et al. / Psychiatry Research 216 (2014) 325332

Table 2
Mean (S.D.) comparison of clinical and depressive symptoms.

CON (n 45) SCZ (n 32) SCZ DEP (n 28) F-Statistic

Clinical symptoms
Hallucinations 2.5 (2.0) 3.6 (1.9) 4.4n
Delusions 2.4 (1.9) 3.9 (1.7) 10.8nn
Bizarre Behavior 1.0 (1.6) 2.3 (2.0) 7.5nn
Positive Formal Thought Disorder 1.6 (1.6) 2.4 (1.6) 3.7
Affective Flattening 3.2 (1.3) 3.4 (1.6) 0.5
Alogia 2.2 (1.8) 2.0 (1.7) 0.1
Avolition 3.0 (1.4) 3.8 (1.3) 4.8n
Anhedonia 3.1 (1.6) 3.3 (1.5) 0.2
Attention 2.1 (1.7) 1.7 (1.8) 0.8

Depressive Symptoms
MADRS Total Score 0.9 (1.4) 3.3 (2.5) 16.9 (8.0) 117.6nnna
Apparent Sadness 0.1 (0.4) 0.6 (1.0) 1.4 (1.3) 16.2nnna
Total Sadness 0.1 (0.4) 0.7 (1.1) 2.1 (1.7) 28.0nnna
Inner Tension 0.2 (0.6) 0.2 (0.6) 2.4 (1.8) 43.4nnnb
Reduced Sleep 0.2 (0.7) 0.2 (0.7) 1.0 (1.8) 5.9nnb
Reduced Appetite o 0.1 (0.3) 0.0 (0.0) 0.4 (0.9) 5.7nnb
Concentration Issues 0.1 (0.5) 0.9 (1.3) 2.3 (1.7) 28.9nnna
Lassitude 0.1 (0.3) 0.4 (0.8) 2.8 (1.6) 77.0nnnb
Inability to Feel 0.0 (0.0) 0.2 (0.6) 1.8 (2.2) 21.5nnnb
Pessimistic Thoughts o 0.1 (0.3) 0.2 (0.5) 2.0 (1.9) 32.7nnnb
Suicidal Thoughts o 0.1 (0.0) 0.1 (0.4) 0.9 (1.5) 10.8nnnb

Abbreviations: individuals with schizophrenia with no depressive symptoms (SCZ), individuals with schizophrenia and depressive symptoms (SCZDEP), healthy controls (CON).

P o 0.10.
n
Po 0.05.
nn
Po 0.01.
nnn
Po 0.001.
a
SCZ Dep 4SCZ4CON.
b
SCZ Dep4SCZ, CON.

Table 3
Mean (S.D.) comparison of empathy and social functioning measures.

CON (n 45) SCZ (n 32) SCZ DEP (n 28) F-Statistic

Self-reported empathy
Fantasy 16.4 (6.1) 12.5 (4.7) 16.4 (6.6) 4.8nna
Perspective-Taking 21.2 (4.5) 16.3 (4.4) 18.2 (4.5) 11.5nnnb
Empathic Concern 21.5 (4.6) 18.3 (4.7) 20.3 (5.4) 4.0nc
Personal Distress 8.6 (4.4) 11.7 (5.2) 14.9 (4.8) 15.2nnnd

Performance-based Empathy
Affective responsiveness (ACC) 0.88 (0.1) 0.78 (0.1) 0.79 (0.1) 15.7nnnb
(RT) 1.3 (0.3) 1.7 (0.5) 1.4 (0.3) 11.7nnne
Emotional perspective-taking (ACC) 0.85 (0.1) 0.74 (0.1) 0.73 (0.1) 17.7nnnb
(RT) 1.4 (0.3) 1.7 (0.4) 1.6 (0.3) 9.3nnne

Social functioning
Social competence 4.5 (0.6) 3.3 (0.8) 3.5 (0.8) 21.6nnnb
Social attainment 142.5 (11.1) 126.7 (14.4) 124.6 (10.2) 37.4nnnb

Abbreviations: individuals with schizophrenia with no depressive symptoms (SCZ), individuals with schizophrenia and depressive symptoms (SCZ DEP), healthy controls
(CON), task accuracy (ACC), task response time (RT).
n
Po 0.05.
nn
Po 0.01.
nnn
Po 0.001.
a
CON and SCZ DEP 4SCZ.
b
CON4SCZ and SCZ DEP.
c
CON4SCZ.
d
CONo SCZo SCZ DEP.
e
SCZ 4SCZ DEP and CON.

levels of fantasy than SCZ (Po0.01, Po0.05, respectively), while CON signicant effect of group on personal distress (F1,104 15.2, Po0.001)
and SCZ DEP did not differ (P40.10). There was a signicant effect of with SCZDEP and SCZ having higher personal distress than CON
group on IRI perspective-taking (F1,104 11.5, Po0.001) with SCZ and (Po0.001 and Po 0.01, respectively), while SCZDEP had higher
SCZDEP reporting lower IRI perspective-taking than controls personal distress than SCZ (Po0.01).
(Po0.001 and Po0.01, respectively), while the differences between
SCZ and SCZDEP did not differ (P40.10). There was a signicant 3.4. Performance-based empathy
effect of group on empathic concern (F1,104 4.0, Po0.05). SCZ had
lower levels of empathic concern than CON (Po0.01), while SCZDEP Means and standard deviations for performance-based empa-
did not differ from CON (P40.10) or SCZ (P40.10). There was a thy are presented in Table 3. There was a signicant effect of group
A.C. Abramowitz et al. / Psychiatry Research 216 (2014) 325332 329

on the accuracy (F2,104 15.7, P o0.001) and response time (RT) but did not correlate with the IRI subscales (all P 40.10). The IRI
(F2,104 11.7, P o0.001) for the affective responsiveness task. SCZ subscales did not correlate with social competence for either SCZ
and SCZ DEP had lower accuracies (both Po 0.001) and slower group (all P 40.10).
RTs (Po 0.001 and P 0.07 (trend), respectively) than CON. SCZ
and SCZ DEP did not differ with respect to accuracy (P4 0.10), 3.7. Mediation analysis
while SCZ had a longer RT than SCZ DEP (P o0.01).
There was a signicant effect of group on the accuracy Fig. 1 displays the mediation results. There was a direct effect of
(F2,104 17.7, P o0.001) and response time (RT) (F2,104 9.3, depressive symptoms on personal distress (F1,27 4.3, P o0.05)
P o0.001) for the emotional perspective-taking task. SCZ and and social attainment (F1,27 6.3, Po0.05). A single model using
SCZ DEP had lower accuracies (both Po 0.001) and slower RTs 5000 bootstrap resamples revealed that depressive symptoms had
(P o0.001 and P o0.05, respectively) than CON. SCZ and SCZ DEP an indirect effect on social attainment through personal distress
did not differ with respect to accuracy (P4 0.10), while SCZ had a (B  1.54, SE 0.93; 95% CI LL  4.00, UL  0.28). The direct
longer RT than SCZ DEP at the trend-level (P 0.06). effect of depressive symptoms on social attainment was reduced
to a trend-level of signicance (F1,24 3.3, P 0.08), which suggests
3.5. Social functioning personal distress partially mediated the relationship between
depressive symptoms and social attainment. Alternatively, we
Means and standard deviations for social functioning are examined whether personal distress had a signicant indirect
presented in Table 3. There was a signicant effect of group on effect on social attainment through depressive symptoms, but this
social competence (F2,84 21.6, P o0.001), with SCZ and SCZ DEP effect did not attain signicance (B  0.29, SE 0.22; 95%
demonstrating lower competencies than controls (both P o0.001), CI LL  0.86, UL 0.01).
but no difference between SCZ and SCZ DEP (P 40.10). There was
also a signicant effect of group on social attainment (F2,103 37.4, 3.8. Exploratory Correlations
P o0.001), with SCZ and SCZ DEP demonstrating lower social
attainment levels than CON (both P o0.001), but no difference Among SCZ DEP, we observed that greater depressive symp-
between SCZ and SCZ DEP (P 40.10). toms were correlated with ratings of hallucinations (R 0.45,
Po 0.05), at affect (R0.45, P o0.05), avolition (R 0.38,
3.6. Correlations Po 0.05), and delusions at the trend level (R 0.36, P 0.06).
Depressive symptoms did not correlate with the remaining clinical
Among SCZ DEP, depressive symptoms signicantly correlated symptom ratings, duration of illness, and antipsychotic treatment
with personal distress (R0.38, Po0.05), but no other empathy (all P 40.10). Among SCZ DEP, lower affective responsiveness
measure (all P40.10). Depressive symptoms correlated with social task accuracy was related to higher bizarre behavior (R  0.59,
attainment (R  0.44, Po0.05), but not social competence (P40.10). Po 0.001) and alogia (R  0.46, Po 0.05) ratings (see
The IRI subscales personal distress and perspective-taking were Supplementary Table 1). Among SCZ, we observed that lower
correlated with social attainment (R  0.50, Po0.01 and R 0.39, affective responsiveness task accuracy was related to more severe
Po0.05, respectively), while fantasy and empathic concern were not delusions (R  0.48, P o0.01) and anhedonia (R  0.42,
(both P40.10). We observed an a bivariate outlier that was removed Po 0.05), while higher IRI fantasy scores were related to higher
from the personal distress and social attainment correlation using a affective attening (R 0.40, P o0.05) and avolition (R 0.44,
standard procedure (Fieller, 2005; Tabachnick and Fidell, 2012). Po 0.05) ratings (see Supplementary Table 2). There were no
Performance-based empathy accuracies did not correlate with other signicant correlations between symptoms and the IRI data
social competence or social attainment (Both P 40.10). The or with emotional perspective-taking task accuracy for both
performance-based measures of empathy were correlated with groups.
each other (R 0.46, P o0.05), but not with the IRI subscales
(all P4 0.10). See Supplementary Fig. 2 for scatterplots of correla-
tions among personal distress, depressive symptoms, and social 4. Discussion
attainment.
Among SCZ, IRI perspective-taking (R 0.45, P o0.01) and The current study demonstrated that SCZ DEP were charac-
empathic concern (R0.46, P o0.01) were correlated with social terized by heightened levels of self-reported personal distress
attainment, but personal distress and fantasy were not (P4 0.10). beyond the elevated level of personal distress typically associated
Performance-based cognitive empathy was correlated with social with schizophrenia (Achim et al., 2011). We did not observe any
competence (R 0.44, P o0.05), while this relationship was at the differences between SCZ and SCZ DEP with respect to
trend level for emotional empathy (R0.37, P 0.08). Neither performance-based empathy. Measures of empathy, social func-
performance-based empathy accuracies correlated with social tioning, and clinical symptoms were normally distributed within
attainment (both P 40.10). The two performance-based measures each group. We found that personal distress and depressive
of empathy were correlated with each other (R 0.63, P o0.001), symptoms were both correlated with poorer social attainment

Fig. 1. The Preacher and Hayes bootstrapping approach indicates the indirect effect of depressive symptoms on social attainment through personal distress was signicant
(95% C.I. LL  4.00, UL  0.28), while the direct effect of depressive symptoms on social attainment was reduced to a trend level of signicance (highlighted by the dashed
line). P 0.08, nP o 0.05, nnP o 0.01.
330 A.C. Abramowitz et al. / Psychiatry Research 216 (2014) 325332

among SCZ DEP, and that personal distress partially mediated the or emotional empathy. These ndings add to the small literature
relationship between depressive symptoms and social attainment. suggesting that depression may not be related to additional
Both SCZ and SCZ DEP demonstrated self-reported and impairments in performance-based measures of empathy
performance-based decits in empathy; however, these methods (Thoma et al., 2011; Derntl et al., 2012). Future research could
were not related to one another. These ndings are consistent with examine the empathic abilities of individuals with schizophrenia
prior work in individuals with schizophrenia (Derntl et al., 2009; experiencing greater severity in depressive symptoms since the
Lee et al., 2011; Smith et al., in press) and in healthy subjects (Ickes present sample was characterized by mild-to-moderate ratings of
et al., 2000; Zaki et al., 2008). These results suggest that one's own depressive symptoms that were skewed towards being mild. The
self-perceived empathic ability may not be related to the actual negative core beliefs typically associated with depression may
ability to understand and be responsive to others' emotions. have contributed to the depressed group providing negatively
Surprisingly, we did not nd that SCZ DEP were characterized biased responses on the measures of personal distress (Beck,
by more severe impairments in social functioning. Perhaps the 1996).
social functioning measures used in the study were not sensitive Our exploratory results suggest that SCZ DEP were character-
to social impairments beyond the measurable decits typically ized by elevated positive symptoms that were associated with
associated with schizophrenia and exacerbated by co-occurring greater severity in depressive symptoms, which is consistent with
depressive symptoms. prior studies (Sax et al., 1996; Cohen et al., in press). Based on
Research suggests that regulating one's emotional responsive- these ndings, one could hypothesize that these individuals could
ness is a key contributor to empathy (Decety, 2011). Moreover, be feeling depressed due to persistently hearing voices or having
abnormalities in the regulation of one's emotional responsiveness delusional beliefs. However, future research using a longitudinal
can contribute to greater feelings of personal distress (Decety and design will be needed to evaluate this potential relationship. Thus,
Jackson, 2004; Eisenberg and Eggum, 2009). Prior research sug- a potential clinical implication of these results is that clinicians
gests that the IRI subscale of personal distress may be a proxy for might consider monitoring their patients with schizophrenia for
assessing emotional regulation rather than a measure of empathy increased depressive symptoms when an increased severity in
(Reniers et al., 2011). Our ndings suggested that SCZ and positive symptoms is observed.
SCZ DEP had elevated levels of personal distress (i.e., emotional The ndings should be interpreted in the context of some
dysregulation) as compared to CON, which is consistent with prior limitations. First, our samples were small and have limited
studies of personal distress in schizophrenia (Achim et al., 2011). explanatory power so a larger sample could help clarify the
Moreover, we found that personal distress was signicantly trend-level correlations or differences in task response time that
higher in SCZ DEP as compared to SCZ. This nding provides appear to be counterintuitive. Second, the cross-sectional nature of
some support to our hypothesis that SCZ DEP may have more this study prevents us from drawing any causal conclusions. Third,
severe empathic impairments. The association between depressive we did not assess several factors that may have inuenced
symptoms and heightened personal distress is also consistent with empathy or the level of depressive symptoms. For instance, we
previous studies of the empathic abilities of individuals with major did not assess one's motivation to empathize, which may inuence
depressive disorder (O'Connor et al., 2002; Cusi et al., 2011; Thoma one's ability to empathize with others (Tucker et al., 2005; Morelli
et al., 2011; Derntl et al., 2012; Schreiter et al., 2013). Thus, the et al., 2014). Hence, elevated depressive symptoms could be
presence of mild-to-moderate depressive symptoms could be associated with reduced empathic motivation rather than impact-
exacerbating the elevated personal distress already associated ing one's empathic ability. Also, we did not evaluate the presence
with schizophrenia. However, longitudinal data is needed to assess of Axis II disorders (e.g., schizotypal, narcissistic, and borderline
the direction of this relationship. personality disorders), which have been associated with impair-
Among SCZ DEP, we found that personal distress was nega- ments in empathy (Ritter et al., 2011; Roepke et al., 2012; Ripoll
tively correlated with social attainment. Prior studies have not et al., 2013). Social anxiety is another factor we did not assess that
reported a relationship between personal distress and social has been associated with reduced empathy (Achim et al., 2011).
functioning measures among individuals with schizophrenia or Furthermore, we did not assess the extent that our participants
depression (Cusi et al., 2011; Smith et al., 2012, in press). The experienced extrapyramidal symptoms or were demoralized by
observed mediating properties of personal distress as well as its their mental health status, which have both been associated with
relationship with social attainment in this study suggests that increased severity of depressive symptoms (Birchwood et al.,
personal distress could be a novel treatment target with direct 2005; Aguilar and Siris, 2007; Shahar et al., 2010). Hence, these
implications for improving social functioning. Regarding clinical factors should be considered when interpreting the results and
implications, interventions could be developed to remediate the may be important areas for future research. Fourth, the affective
heightened personal distress (i.e., emotion dysregulation) linked responsiveness task required participants to identify how they
to depressive symptoms experienced by this group. For instance, would feel in response to certain emotional situations. These
cognitive behavioral therapy has demonstrated preliminary ef- responses represent cognitively determined hypothetical emo-
cacy at improving emotion regulation in early psychosis (Khoury tional states rather than in-the-moment affective responses, and
et al., 2013), while dialectical behavior therapy has demonstrated must be considered as such when interpreting the ndings. Lastly,
promise for improving emotion regulation among individuals with the performance measures used to assess empathy may not have
cognitive disabilities (Brown et al., 2013). Hence, these interven- been able to capture all domains of empathy affected by depres-
tions could potentially be adapted for use with individuals with sion. For example, emotional contagion is a key empathic process
schizophrenia who are experiencing depressive symptoms. (Preston and de Waal, 2002), but was not well captured by the
Interestingly, we found that measures of emotional empathy self-reported or performance-based methods used in this study.
were associated with social attainment in the SCZ DEP group. In conclusion, our ndings suggest that individuals with
This nding diverges from our prior work that emotional empathy schizophrenia and mild-to-moderate depressive symptoms have
may be unrelated to social functioning in individuals with schizo- elevated levels of personal distress that potentially mediate the
phrenia (Smith et al., 2012, in press). Thus, the presence of association between depressive symptoms and poor social func-
depressive symptoms could be disturbing this relationship. Con- tioning. Hence, individuals with schizophrenia and co-occurring
trary to our hypothesis, SCZ DEP did not demonstrate exacer- depressive symptoms may benet from receiving services
bated decits in performance-based measures of either cognitive designed to address their heightened personal distress. Future
A.C. Abramowitz et al. / Psychiatry Research 216 (2014) 325332 331

research is needed to identify specic factors that contribute to Derntl, B., Finkelmeyer, A., Toygar, T.K., Hulsmann, A., Schneider, F., Falkenberg, D.I.,
elevations in personal distress that have been associated with Habel, U., 2009. Generalized decit in all core components of empathy in
schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 108 (13), 197206.
schizophrenia and depressive symptoms. Derntl, B., Seidel, E.M., Schneider, F., Habel, U., 2012. How specic are emotional
decits? A comparison of empathic abilities in schizophrenia, bipolar and
depressed patients. Schizophrenia Research 142 (1-3), 5864.
Eisenberg, N., Eggum, N.D., 2009. Empathic Responding: Sympathy and Personal
Acknowledgment Distress. In: Decety, J., Ickes, W. (Eds.), The Social Neuroscience of Empathy. The
MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 7184.
Fervaha, G., Agid, O., Takeuchi, H., Foussias, G., Remington, G., 2013. Clinical
We would like to acknowledge the research staff at the North- determinants of life satisfaction in chronic schizophrenia: data from the CATIE
western University Schizophrenia Research Group for study coor- study. Schizophrenia Research 151 (13), 203208.
dination and data collection, and our participants for volunteering Fieller, N., 2005. Multivariate outliers. In: Everitt, B.S., Howell, D.C. (Eds.), Encyclo-
their time. This work was funded by the Department of Psychiatry pedia of Statistics in Behavioral Science, vol. 3. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd,
Chichester, pp. 13791384.
and Behavioral Sciences at Northwestern University Feinberg First, M.B., Spitzer, R.L., Miriam, G., Williams, J.B.W., 2002. Structured clinical
School of Medicine. Additional support for this study was provided interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Non-patient
by the Warren Wright Adolescent Center at Northwestern Mem- Edition. Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York,
NY.
orial Hospital's Stone Institute of Psychiatry. Gur, R.C., Sara, R., Hagendoorn, M., Marom, O., Hughett, P., Macy, L., Turner, T.,
Bajcsy, R., Posner, A., Gur, R.E., 2002. A method for obtaining 3-dimensional
facial expressions and its standardization for use in neurocognitive studies.
Journal of Neuroscience Methods 115 (2), 137143.
Appendix A. Supplementary material Haker, H., Schimansky, J., Jann, S., Rssler, W., 2012. Self-reported empathic abilities
in schizophrenia: a longitudinal perspective. Psychiatry Research 200 (23),
Supplementary data associated with this article can be found in 10281031.
Harvey, P.D., Velligan, D.I., Bellack, A.S., 2007. Performance-based measures of
the online version at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2014.02.028. functional skills: usefulness in clinical treatment studies. Schizophrenia Bulle-
tin 33 (5), 11381148.
Ickes, W., Buysse, A.H.P., Rivers, K., Erickson, J., Hankock, M., Kelleher, J., Gesn, P.R.,
References 2000. On the difculty of distinguishing "good" and "poor" perceivers: a social
relations analysis of empathic accuracy data. Personal Relationships 7, 219234.
Kasckow, J., Lanouette, N., Patterson, T., Fellows, I., Golshan, S., Solorzano, E., Zisook, S.,
Achim, A.M., Ouellet, R., Roy, M.A., Jackson, P.L., 2011. Assessment of empathy in 2010. Treatment of subsyndromal depressive symptoms in middle-aged and older
rst-episode psychosis and meta-analytic comparison with previous studies in adults with schizophrenia: effect on functioning. International Journal of Geriatric
schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research 190 (1), 38. Psychiatry 25 (2), 183190.
Aguilar, E.J., Siris, S.G., 2007. Do antipsychotic drugs inuence suicidal behavior in Kessler, R.C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Koretz, D., Merikangas, K.R., Rush, A.J.,
schizophrenia? Psychopharmacology Bulletin 40 (3), 128142. Walters, E.E., Wang, P.S., 2003. The epidemiology of major depressive disorder:
Andreasen, N.C., 1983a. The Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms. The results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R). Journal of
University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA. the American Medical Association 289 (23), 30953105.
Andreasen, N.C., 1983b. The Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms. The Khoury, B., Lecomte, T., Comtois, G., Nicole, L., 2013. Third-wave strategies for
University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA. emotion regulation in early psychosis: a pilot study. Early Intervention in
Baez, S., Herrera, E., Villarin, L., Theil, D., Gonzalez-Gadea, M.L., Gomez, P., Psychiatry.
Mosquera, M., Huepe, D., Strejilevich, S., Vigliecca, N.S., Matthaus, F., Decety, Lanon, C., Auquier, P., Reine, G., Bernard, D., Addington, D., 2001. Relationships
J., Manes, F., Ibanez, A.M., 2013. Contextual social cognition impairments in
between depression and psychotic symptoms of schizophrenia during an acute
schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. PLoS One 8 (3), e57664.
episode and stable period. Schizophrenia Research 47 (23), 135140.
Barch, D.M., Mitropoulou, V., Harvey, P.D., New, A.S., Silverman, J.M., Siever, L.J.,
Langdon, R., Coltheart, M., Ward, P.B., 2006. Empathetic perspective-taking is
2004. Context-processing decits in schizotypal personality disorder. Journal of
impaired in schizophrenia: evidence from a study of emotion attribution and
Abnormal Psychology 113 (4), 556568.
theory of mind. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 11 (2), 133155.
Beck, A., 1996. The past and future of cognitive therapy. Journal of Psychotherapy
Lee, J., Zaki, J., Harvey, P.O., Ochsner, K., Green, M.F., 2011. Schizophrenia patients
Practice and Research 6 (4), 276284.
are impaired in empathic accuracy. Psychological Medicine 41 (11), 18.
Benton, A.L., Hamsher, K., Sivan, A.B., 1976. Multilingual Aphasia Examination. AJA
Marder, S.R., Fenton, W., 2004. Measurement and treatment research to improve
Associates, Iowa City.
cognition in schizophrenia: NIMH MATRICS initiative to support the develop-
Birchwood, M., Iqbal, Z., Upthegrove, R., 2005. Psychological pathways to depres-
ment of agents for improving cognition in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia
sion in schizophrenia: studies in acute psychosis, post psychotic depression and
Research 72 (1), 59.
auditory hallucinations. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neu-
Montgomery, S.A., Asberg, M., 1979. A new depression scale designed to be
roscience 255, 202212.
Bondol, G., Jermann, F., Rouget, B.W., Gex-Fabry, M., McQuillan, A., Dupont- sensitive to change. British Journal of Psychiatry 134, 382389.
Willemin, A., Aubry, J.M., Nguyen, C., 2010. Self- and clinician-rated Montgom- Morelli, S.A., Rameson, L.T., Lieberman, M.D., 2014. The neural components of
eryAsberg Depression Rating Scale: evaluation in clinical practice. Journal of empathy: predicting daily prosocial behavior. Social and Cognitive Affective
Affective Disorders 121 (3), 268272. Neuroscience 9 (1), 3947.
Brown, J.F., Brown, M.Z., Dibiasio, P., 2013. Treating Individuals with intellectual O'Connor, L.E., Berry, J.W., Weiss, J., Gilbert, P., 2002. Guilt, fear, submission, and
disabilities and challenging behaviors with adapted dialectical behavior ther- empathy in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders 71 (13), 1927.
apy. Journal of Mental Health Research in Intellectual Disabilities 6 (4), Patterson, T.L., Moscona, S., McKibbin, C.L., Davidson, K., Jeste, D.V., 2001. Social
280303. skills performance assessment among older patients with schizophrenia.
Brune, M., Brune-Cohrs, U., 2006. Theory of mindevolution, ontogeny, brain Schizophrenia Research 48 (23), 351360.
mechanisms and psychopathology. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews Preacher, K.J., Hayes, A.F., 2004. SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect
30 (4), 437455. effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, &
Buckley, P.F., Miller, B.J., Lehrer, D.S., Castle, D.J., 2009. Psychiatric comorbidities and Computers: A Journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc. 36 (4), 717731.
schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 35 (2), 383402. Preston, S.D., de Waal, F.B., 2002. Empathy: its ultimate and proximate bases.
Cohen, C.I., Izediuno, I., Yadack, A.M., Ghosh, B., Garrett, M., 2013. Characteristics of Behavioral and Brain sciences 25 (1), 2071 (120; discussion).
auditory hallucinations and associated factors in older adults with schizophre- Reitan, R., Wolfson, D., 1985. The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsycholoigcal Test Battery:
nia, American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, electronic publication 7 Septem- Theory and Clinical Interpretation. Neuropsychology Press, Tucson, AZ.
ber; http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jagp.2013.07.001. Reniers, R.L., Corcoran, R., Drake, R., Shryane, N.M., Vollm, B.A., 2011. The QCAE: a
Cusi, A.M., Macqueen, G.M., Spreng, R.N., McKinnon, M.C., 2011. Altered empathic Questionnaire of Cognitive and Affective Empathy. Journal of Personality
responding in major depressive disorder: relation to symptom severity, illness Assessments 93 (1), 8495.
burden, and psychosocial outcome. Psychiatry Research 188 (2), 231236. Ripoll, L.H., Zaki, J., Perez-Rodriguez, M.M., Snyder, R., Strike, K.S., Boussi, A., Bartz, J.A.,
Davis, M.H., 1983. Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a Ochsner, K.N., Siever, L.J., New, A.S., 2013. Empathic accuracy and cognition in
multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 44 schizotypal personality disorder. Psychiatry Research 210, 232241.
(1), 113126. Ritter, K., Dziobek, I., Preissler, S., Rter, A., Vater, A., Fydrich, T., Lammers, C.H.,
Decety, J., 2011. Dissecting the neural mechanisms mediating empathy. Emotion Heekeren, H.R., Roepke, S., 2011. Lack of empathy in patients with narcissistic
Review 3 (1), 92108. personality disorder. Psychiatry Research 187, 241247.
Decety, J., Jackson, P.L., 2004. The functional architecture of human empathy. Roepke, S., Vater, A., Preiler, S., Heekeren, H.R., Dziobek, I., 2012. Social cognition
Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews 3 (2), 71100. in borderline personality disorder. Frontiers in Neuroscience 6, 195.
Delis, D., Kramer, J.H., Kaplan, E., Ober, B., 1983. California Verbal Learning Test, Sax, K.W., Strakowski, S.M., Keck , P.E., Upadhyaya, V.H., West, S.A., McElroy, S.L.,
Research Edition. The Psychological Corporation, Cleveland. 1996. Relationships among negative, positive, and depressive symptoms in
332 A.C. Abramowitz et al. / Psychiatry Research 216 (2014) 325332

schizophrenia and psychotic depression. British Journal of Psychiatry 168 (1), Smith, M.J., Horan, W.P., Karpouzian, T.M., Abram, S.V., Cobia, D.J., Csernansky, J.G.,
6871. 2012. Self-reported empathy decits are uniquely associated with poor func-
Schneider, L.C., Struening, E.L., 1983. SLOF: a behavioral rating scale for assessing tioning in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 137 (13), 196202.
the mentally ill. Social Work Research Abstracts 19 (3), 921. Sparks, A., McDonald, S., Lino, B., O'Donnell, M., Green, M.J., 2010. Social cognition,
Schreiter, S., Pijnenborg, G.H., Aan He tRot, M., 2013. Empathy in adults with clinical empathy and functional outcome in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 122
or subclinical depressive symptoms. Journal of Affective Disorders 150 (1), (13), 172178.
116. Tabachnick, B.G., Fidell, L.S., 2012. Using multivariate statistics. In: Tabachnick, B.G.,
Shahar, G., Weinberg, D., McGlashan, T.H., Davidson, L., 2010. Illness related stress Fidell, L.S. (Eds.), Using Multivariate Statistics, 6th ed. Allyn and Bacon, Boston
interacts with perception of the self as ill to predict depression in psychosis. Thoma, P., Zalewski, I., von Reventlow, H.G., Norra, C., Juckel, G., Daum, I., 2011.
International Journal Cognitive Therapy 3 (2), 202210. Cognitive and affective empathy in depression linked to executive control.
Shamay-Tsoory, S.G., 2011. The neural bases for empathy. Neuroscientist 17 (1), Psychiatry Research 189 (3), 373378.
1824. Tucker, D.M., Luu, P., Derryberry, D., 2005. Love hurts: the evolution of empathic
Shamay-Tsoory, S.G., Shur, S., Harari, H., Levkovitz, Y., 2007. Neurocognitive basis of concern through the encephalization of nociceptive capacity. Development and
impaired empathy in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology 21 (4), 431438. Psychopathology 17 (3), 699713.
Simonsen, C., Sundet, K., Vaskinn, A., Ueland, T., Romm, K.L., Hellvin, T., Melle, I., Wechsler, D., 1997a. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Third Edition The Psycho-
Friis, S., Andreassen, O.A., 2010. Psychosocial function in schizophrenia and logical Corporation, San Antonio.
bipolar disorder: relationship to neurocognition and clinical symptoms. Journal Wechsler, D., 1997b. Wechsler Memory Scale, Third Edition The Psychological
of International Neuropsycholology Society 16 (5), 771783. Corporation, San Antonio.
Smith, M.J., Horan, W.P., Cobia, D.J., Karpouzian, T.M., Fox, J.M., Reilly, J.L., Breiter, H.C., Zaki, J., Bolger, N., Ochsner, K., 2008. It takes two: the interpersonal nature of
2013. Performance-based empathy mediates the inuence of working memory empathic accuracy. Psychological Science 19 (4), 399404.
on social competence in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, electronic pub- Zaki, J., Ochsner, K., 2011. Reintegrating the study of accuracy into social cognition
lication 31 December; http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbt084. research. Psychological Inquiry 22 (3), 159182.

You might also like