You are on page 1of 11

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/222185234

Defining Place Attachment: A Tripartite


Organizing Framework

Article in Journal of Environmental Psychology March 2010


DOI: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.09.006

CITATIONS READS

297 2,969

2 authors:

Leila Scannell Robert Gifford


Royal Roads University University of Victoria
13 PUBLICATIONS 602 CITATIONS 164 PUBLICATIONS 4,321 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Leila Scannell on 07 April 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. All in-text references underlined in blue are added to the original document
and are linked to publications on ResearchGate, letting you access and read them immediately.
Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 110

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Environmental Psychology


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jep

Dening place attachment: A tripartite organizing framework


Leila Scannell, Robert Gifford*
Department of Psychology, University of Victoria, Victoria, British Columbia, V8W 3P5 Canada

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Place attachment has been researched quite broadly, and so has been dened in a variety of ways. The
Available online 22 September 2009 various denitions of the concept are reviewed and synthesized into a three-dimensional, person
processplace organizing framework. The person dimension of place attachment refers to its individually
Keywords: or collectively determined meanings. The psychological dimension includes the affective, cognitive, and
Place attachment behavioral components of attachment. The place dimension emphasizes the place characteristics of
Framework
attachment, including spatial level, specicity, and the prominence of social or physical elements. In
Model
addition, potential functions of place attachment are reviewed. The framework organizes related place
attachment concepts and thus claries the term. The framework may also be used to stimulate new
research, investigate multidimensionality, create operational denitions for quantitative studies, guide
semi-structured interviews for qualitative studies, and assist in conict resolution for successful land-use
management.
2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Place attachment, the bonding that occurs between individuals activity, trafc, etc.) on ones block and less fear of neighborhood
and their meaningful environments, has gained much scientic crime (Brown, Perkins, & Brown, 2003). Finally, because of its
attention in recent years (e.g., Giuliani, 2003; Low & Altman, 1992). associations with environmental risk perception, and place-
Part of this interest stems from the awareness that personplace protective attitudes (e.g., Kyle, Graefe, Manning, & Bacon, 2004;
bonds have become fragile as globalization, increased mobility, and Nordenstam, 1994; Stedman, 2002; Vorkinn & Riese, 2001), place
encroaching environmental problems threaten the existence of, attachment contributes to the understanding of pro-environmental
and our connections to, places important to us (Relph, 1976; behavior, although the research on this topic is limited and the
Sanders, Bowie, & Bowie, 2003; Sennett, 2000). ndings are inconsistent (e.g., Uzzell, Pol, & Badenas, 2002; Vaske &
Place attachment is also worthy of study because of its relevance Kobrin, 2001).
to many important processes. For instance, the examination of Because of the application of place attachment to many
place attachment as an emotional bond has shed light on the perspectives, a plenitude of denitions has accumulated. For the
distress and grief expressed by those who are forced to relocate most part, researchers portray place attachment as a multifaceted
(e.g., Fried, 1963; Fullilove, 1996). Place attachment has thus been concept that characterizes the bonding between individuals and
applied to disaster psychology (e.g., Brown & Perkins, 1992), their important places (e.g., Giuliani, 2003; Low & Altman, 1992).
immigration (e.g., Ng, 1998), and mobility (e.g., Giuliani, Ferrara, & However, variations in this denition are vast. Humanistic geog-
Barabotti, 2003; Gustafson, 2001). Other research has shown that raphers argue that a bond with a meaningful space, or sense of
place meaning and attachment can be used to plan and encourage place is a universal affective tie that fullls fundamental human
the use of public spaces, such as national parks (e.g., Kyle, Graefe, & needs (e.g., Relph, 1976; Tuan, 1974), Some authors suggest that
Manning, 2005; Moore & Graefe, 1994; Williams & Stewart, 1998). sense of place encompasses the sub-concepts of place identity,
Place attachment is also relevant to the study of environmental place attachment, and place dependence (e.g., Jorgensen & Sted-
perception. Attached individuals experience a heightened sense of man, 2001), or that it includes ancestral ties, feeling like an
safety, even when their place is situated in a war zone (e.g., Billig, insider, and a desire to stay in the place (Hay, 1998). In the
2006). On a smaller scale, attachment to ones neighborhood is immigration and refugee literature, however, the emphasis is
associated with fewer perceived incivilities (e.g., drug dealing, gang typically on displacement, or diaspora, such that attachment is
dened by the intensity of longing for places that are lost (Deutsch,
2005). Urban sociologists and community scientists locate attach-
* Corresponding author. Tel.: 1 250 721 7532. ment at the city, home, and neighborhood levels (e.g., Kasarda &
E-mail address: rgifford@uvic.ca (R. Gifford). Janowitz, 1974). Even within disciplines, models diverge in their

0272-4944/$ see front matter 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.09.006
2 L. Scannell, R. Gifford / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 110

denitions of place attachment; for example, place attachment has involves the personal connections one has to a place. For example,
been said to rely on social features (e.g., Woldoff, 2002), physical place attachment is stronger for settings that evoke personal
features (e.g., Stokols & Shumaker, 1981), or both (e.g., Riger & memories, and this type of place attachment is thought to
Lavrakas, 1981). contribute to a stable sense of self (Twigger-Ross & Uzzell, 1996).
This denitional diversity reects the growing interest in place Similarly, places become meaningful from personally important
attachment, and can be seen as progress in the concepts theoretical experiences, such as realizations, milestones (e.g., where I rst
development. Researchers have highlighted different processes, met my signicant other), and experiences of personal growth, as
places, and people involved in personplace bonding, but these Manzo (2005) notes in her study of the experiences and places
denitions remain scattered in the literature, and thus the theo- that create place meaning. She comments, it is not simply the
retical development of the concept has not yet been acknowledged, places themselves that are signicant, but rather what can be
nor has a more general denition of place attachment been agreed called experience-in-place that creates meaning (p. 74).
upon. By exploring the commonalities across the different Although other theorists argue that place characteristics are
permutations of the concept, we can begin to shape, then structure, integral in the construction of place meaning, the argument that
a coherent understanding of it. individual experiences may form the basis for the attachment is
convincing.
1. Understanding place attachment: a tripartite organizing At the group level, attachment is comprised of the symbolic
framework meanings of a place that are shared among members (Low, 1992).
Group-framed place attachment has been examined in different
We propose a three-dimensional framework of place attach- cultures, genders, and religions. For example, attachment has
ment that usefully structures the varied denitions in the literature. been described as a community process in which groups become
This framework proposes that place attachment is a multidimen- attached to areas wherein they may practice, and thus preserve,
sional concept with person, psychological process, and place their cultures (e.g., Fried, 1963; Gans, 1962; Michelson, 1976).
dimensions (see Fig. 1). The rst dimension is the actor: who is Culture links members to place through shared historical expe-
attached? To what extent is the attachment based on individually riences, values, and symbols. In a study of landscape perception,
and collectively held meanings? The second dimension is the forests were perceived to be more threatening for Hispanic
psychological process: how are affect, cognition, and behavior Americans, African Americans, and women, and less threatening
manifested in the attachment? The third dimension is the object of for European Americans and men (Virden & Walker, 1999). The
the attachment, including place characteristics: what is the authors speculate that different meanings arise from historical
attachment to, and what is the nature of, this place? This three- events, religion, and other experiences common to group
dimensional framework of place attachment organizes the main members, and that these meanings are transmitted to subsequent
denitions in the literature and, as knowledge grows about the generations.
specic levels within each of these dimensions, a comprehensive In addition, place attachment may be religiously based. Through
understanding of place attachment will be reached. religion, the meanings of certain places become elevated to the
status of sacred (Mazumdar & Mazumdar, 2004). Revered places
2. The person dimension: individual and collective place such as Mecca or Jerusalem or, on a smaller scale, churches,
attachment temples, shrines, burial sites, or divine places in nature, are central
to many religions, and their scared meanings are shared among
Place attachment occurs at both the individual and group worshippers. Not only do such places seem to bring worshippers
levels, and although denitions of the term tend to emphasize closer to their gods, but reverence for, and protection of, these
one over the other, the two may overlap. At the individual level, it places essentially reects ones cultural fealty. Although religions

Fig. 1. The tripartite model of place attachment.


L. Scannell, R. Gifford / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 110 3

often designate which places are important, Mazumdar and maintain closeness to a place is an attempt to experience the
Mazumdar note that religious connections to place can also be positive emotions that a place may evoke (Giuliani, 2003).
individual: a place may gain spiritual signicance through personal
experiences (e.g., an epiphany). Therefore, the cultural and indi- 3.2. Place attachment as cognition
vidual levels of place attachment are not entirely independent.
Cultural place meanings and values inuence the extent of indi- Personplace bonds also include cognitive elements. The
vidual place attachment, and individual experiences within a place, memories, beliefs, meaning, and knowledge that individuals asso-
if positive, can maintain and possibly strengthen cultural place ciate with their central settings make them personally important.
attachment. Place attachment as cognition involves the construction of, and
bonding to, place meaning, as well as the cognitions that facilitate
3. The psychological process dimension of place attachment closeness to a place. Through memory, people create place meaning
and connect it to the self. As noted earlier, one can grow attached to
The second dimension of place attachment concerns the way the settings where memorable eras or important events occurred
that individuals and groups relate to a place, and the nature of the (Hay, 1998; Hunter, 1974; Manzo, 2005; Rubenstein & Parmelee,
psychological interactions that occur in the environments that are 1992; Twigger-Ross & Uzzell, 1996). Hunter (1974) describes these
important to them. The three psychological aspects of place as symbolic communities, because the attachment is based on the
attachment or, according to some authors, sense of place, (e.g., representations of the past that the setting contains.
Jorgensen & Stedman, 2001) typically highlighted in its various Individuals structure social information so that it is maximally
theoretical and operational denitions are affect, cognition, and coherent and easy to process (Sears, Freedman, & Peplau, 1985).
behavior. Some denitions include all three of these components, This information is organized into sets of cognitions, or schemas
and others emphasize only one or two of them. This organization of (Bartlett, 1932), which include knowledge and beliefs about
place attachment is common to other social psychological concepts particular objects, or the self (Markus, 1977). Schemas may be
such as attitudes and prejudice, which are also characterized by applied to place attachment. For example, Fullilove (1996) views
affective, cognitive, and behavioral components (e.g., Aronson, familiarity as the cognitive component of place attachment; to be
Wilson, Akert, & Fehr, 2005). attached is to know and organize the details of the environment.
Feldmans (1990) notion of settlement identity suggests that
3.1. Place attachment as affect individuals are attached to certain types or categories of places
(e.g., cottages in rural settings, suburban single-family dwellings,
Personplace bonding undoubtedly involves an emotional or downtowns). Stokols and Shumaker (1981) describe a similar
connection to a particular place (e.g., Cuba & Hummon, 1993; concept, generic place dependence (p. 481). For these attach-
Fullilove, 1996; Giuliani, 2003; Hidalgo & Hernandez, 2001; Manzo, ments, the schema contains information about the features
2003, 2005; Mesch & Manor, 1998; Riley, 1992). Humanistic geog- common to the types of places to which one may become
raphers describe place belongingness in emotional terms. Tuan attached. A favorite place may be a kind of place schema of place-
(1974), for example, coined the word topophilia or love of place, related knowledge and beliefs, which ultimately represents the
for this connection, and Relph (1976) dened place attachment as special character of the place and ones personal connections to it.
the authentic and emotional bond with an environment that In turn, these cognitions can become incorporated into ones self-
satises a fundamental human need. Environmental psychologists concept.
similarly assert the central role of affect in personplace bonding. Proshansky and colleagues (e.g., Proshansky, 1978; Proshansky
Most often, their denitions portray place attachment in affective & Fabian, 1987; Proshansky, Ittelson, & Rivlin, 1970; Proshansky,
terms, such as an emotional investment in a place (Hummon, 1992), Fabian, & Kaminoff, 1983) coined the term place identity, to
or feelings of pride .and a general sense of well-being (Brown describe the physical world socialization of the self (Proshansky
et al., 2003). et al., 1983. p. 57), or the self-denitions that are derived from
Further evidence that attachment to a place is grounded in places. This occurs when individuals draw similarities between self
emotion comes from the literature on displacement, when indi- and place, and incorporate cognitions about the physical environ-
viduals must leave their places such as in the event of a natural ment (memories, thoughts, values, preferences, categorizations)
disaster or war, immigration, or relocation. In his classic study on into their self-denitions. Salient features of a place that make it
the effects of displacement, Fried (1963) investigated a neighbor- unique (e.g., architecture, historical monuments, a cultural
hood redevelopment project in the West End of Boston. The community) can be attached to ones self-concept, a process that
improvements planned for the neighborhood caused the resi- Twigger-Ross and Uzzell (1996) call place-related distinctiveness.
dents to lose familiar structures and social settings, and many of This process is comparable to the development of social identity
them were forced to move. Essentially, this reconstruction meant as described by optimal distinctiveness theory (Brewer, 1991),
the collapse of a tight-knit community. After the fact, residents which asserts that social identity forms when a person seeks
mourned and displayed symptoms of grief. Fried concluded that a balance of similarity to in-group members, and distinctiveness
grief is not limited to the death of a loved one, but can emerge from out-groups. Place also provides information about ones
following the loss of an important place. Fullilove (1996) also found distinctiveness or similarity, information that may be based on
that displacement results in feelings of sadness and longing, and so physical or social features. Similarity would represent a sense of
concluded that attachment is primarily based in affect. belonging to a place, and could be attained in a neighborhood, for
Relationships with place can represent an array of emotions example, from comparisons of the physical appearance of ones
from love and contentment to fear, hatred, and ambivalence house to the houses of proximal others. Differentiation in place
(Manzo, 2005). For example, one can experience a childhood home identity would depend on distinguishing features such as climate
as a signicant place, but that does not necessarily mean the bond or landscape and their relevant connotations (e.g., we are island
is positive. Rather, unhappy or traumatic experiences in a place may people). In general, individuals may connect to a place in the
create negative feelings or even aversion toward it. Although strong sense that it comes to represent who they are. Connections to place
negatively valenced bonds can form with important places, may be cognitive, and can sometimes be incorporated, at the most
attachment usually is dened in positive terms; the desire to personal level, into ones self-denition.
4 L. Scannell, R. Gifford / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 110

3.3. Place attachment as behavior territorial behaviors include marking, personalization, aggression,
and territorial defense, whereas place attachment behaviors
The third aspect of the psychological process dimension of place include pilgrimages, social support, and place restoration.
attachment is the behavioral level, in which attachment is expressed The behavioral level of place attachment, therefore, is founded
through actions. Like interpersonal attachment, place attachment is on the desire to remain close to a place, and can be expressed in
typied by proximity-maintaining behaviors and is a positive, part, by proximity-maintaining in concert with journeys away,
affective bond between an individual and a specic place, the main place reconstruction, and relocation to similar places.
characteristic of which is to maintain closeness to such a place
(Hidalgo & Hernandez, 2001, p. 274). The idea of place attachment as 4. The place dimension of place attachment
proximity-maintaining behavior is supported in studies that relate
place attachment to length of residence (Hay, 1998; Kasarda & Perhaps the most important dimension of place attachment is
Janowitz, 1974) and efforts to return. The literature on homesickness the place itself. What is it about the place to which we connect?
shows that some individuals who have been absent from their This dimension has been examined at various geographic scales
homes for an extended period of time express a great desire to (e.g., a room in a house, a city, or the world; Cuba & Hummon, 1993;
return to or visit the place, and at times, the return can involve much Low & Altman, 1992), and has typically been divided into two
effort or cost (e.g., Riemer, 2004). A religious pilgrimage is another levels: social and physical place attachment (e.g., Riger & Lavrakas,
behavior that exemplies efforts made to be close to ones signi- 1981). Hidalgo and Hernandez (2001) measured the social and
cant place (Low, 1992; Mazumdar & Mazumdar, 2004). physical levels of place attachment at three different spatial levels
Nevertheless, maintaining proximity to ones place is not (home, neighborhood, and city). They found that the strength of the
constantly enacted by the securely attached individual; one may be attachment differed depending on the level of analysis: greater
highly attached to a place, and yet depart from it regularly. In fact, place attachment emerged for the home and city levels than for the
place attachment can even become dysfunctional when an indi- neighborhood level, and the social dimension of place attachment
vidual with a rigid bond to home is reluctant to leave it (Fried, was stronger than the physical dimension. Nevertheless, the
2000). For example, these individuals may miss important oppor- Hidalgo and Hernandez stress that physical and social attachments
tunities, or even put themselves in mortal danger for the sake of both inuence the overall bond, and that spatial level should be
remaining in the place. Furthermore, the dialectical process of considered when measuring place attachment. Riger and Lavrakas
being at home versus being away from home aids in the develop- (1981) suggested that social attachment, or bondedness consists
ment of place meaning (Case, 1996; Dovey, 1985). Through journeys of social ties, belongingness to the neighborhood, and familiarity
away, in combination with proximity-maintaining, individuals are with fellow residents and neighborhood children, and that physical
better able to understand and appreciate routine aspects of their attachment, or rootedness is predicted by length of residence,
place (Case, 1996). Together, these behaviors contribute to the ownership, and plans to stay. Others have also described
construction and expression of the personplace bond. a combined physical-social place attachment (e.g., Mazumdar &
Another interesting behavioral expression of place attachment Mazumdar, 2004; Mesch & Manor, 1998; Uzzell et al., 2002).
is the reconstruction of place, as has been observed in post-disaster Nevertheless, much of the research on place attachment (and
cities. Francaviglia (1978) documented the rebuilding of Xenia, related concepts) has focused on its social aspect; people are
Ohio, a town that was devastated by a tornado in 1974. The attached to places that facilitate social relationships and group
destruction gave planners the opportunity to rectify planning identity. Frieds (1963) study was conducted in a neighborhood that
problems that had existed prior to the disaster, such as the decline was quite dilapidated, but demonstrated that the strong neigh-
of downtown and increased suburbanization. Nevertheless, local borhood bonds can stem from interpersonal interactions. In
residents and businesses used their power to override the post- another study, attachment in a London neighborhood was based on
disaster zoning maps that had been proposed, and ultimately, the the ability to frequently interact with relatives (Young & Willmott,
new Xenia looked much like it had before the disaster. Recreating 1962). Certain physical features, such as density, proximity, and the
a familiar town proved more important than addressing planning presence of amenities and other social arenas inuence these
aws. Francaviglia discussed the role of place meaning, nostalgia, interactions (e.g., Fried, 2000), but when the attachment is directed
and the desire to restore meaningful areas to which residents were toward others who live in the place rather than to aspects of the
attached. Similar acts of reconstruction have been observed, such as place itself, it is considered to be a socially based place bond.
those following the 1976 earthquake in Friuli, Italy (Geipel, 1982), According to urban sociologists, place attachment is necessarily
and the 1964 earthquake in Anchorage, Alaska (Kates, Eriksen, social (e.g., Hunter, 1974, 1978; Kasarda & Janowitz, 1974; Gans,
Pijawka, & Bowden, 1977). Familiarity and use took precedence over 1962), and as such it is sometimes compared to (e.g., McMillan &
planners wishes; residents manifested their attachments by rec- Chavis, 1986), or conated with (e.g., Perkins & Long, 2002; Pretty,
reating the city to which they were bonded, even if it was awed. Chipuer, & Bramston, 2003) sense of community. Community is
Another form of place reconstruction occurs when individuals a complex system of friendship and kinship networks and formal
must relocate to a new place. Some choose to preserve the bond by and informal associational ties rooted in family life and on-going
selecting locations that are as similar as possible to the old place socialization processes (Kasarda & Janowitz, 1974, p. 329). Two
(e.g., Michelson, 1976). This was observed among Boston West types of community have been distinguished: community of
Enders who, when forced to relocate, selected areas of Boston interest, where members are connected through lifestyle and
reminiscent of their previous neighborhood (Gans, 1962). common interests, and community of place, where members are
Place attachment behaviors are not necessarily territorial, connected through geographical location (McMillan & Chavis, 1986;
although the two may overlap, given that place use is an element of Nasar & Julian, 1995). Communities of interest are not always place-
both (e.g., Altman, 1975; Williams, Patterson, Roggenbuck, & Wat- bound, such as in the case of online, professional, or religious
son 1992). Territoriality is based on ownership, control of space, groups that are connected without reference to a place, and so this
and the regulation of access to self (Altman, 1975), but attachment term is not specic to place attachment. Community of place,
to places is an affective, proximity-maintaining bond that can be however, is more relevant to our framework because it describes
expressed without an underlying purpose of control, especially for social ties rooted in place, such as neighborhoods, coffee shops, or
public spaces such as parks or cafes, or sacred spaces. Further, other spaces that support social interaction.
L. Scannell, R. Gifford / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 110 5

According to Hunter (1974), local sentiments and ties are created underdeveloped area may symbolize wilderness. The physical
by the broader social system of mass society, which dictates status aspects constrain the possible meanings a place may adopt, and
based on class, race, and religion. Individuals of similar status and therefore, physically based place attachment rests in these
life-stage select the location and type of dwelling according to their symbolic meanings. One physical feature that inuences place
lifestyles and economic constraints. As a result, pockets of relatively attachment is climate, especially when it resembles the climate of
homogeneous communities emerge, and within these neighbor- ones childhood (Knez, 2005). The meaning-mediated model might
hoods, interpersonal attachments and networks develop. Kasarda explain this nding through symbolic associations: one is attached
and Janowitz (1974) investigated whether local community senti- to the physical climate because it represents ones past.
ments can persist in mass society (i.e., the systemic model) or erode In sum, three dimensions of place attachment are postulated:
as population size and density increases (i.e., the linear model). person, psychological process, and place. Given the complexity of
Support was found for the systemic model: length of residence was personplace bonding, many threads tie individuals to their
associated with greater social ties, including the number of local important places. Some are stronger or more salient than others,
acquaintances, friends, and relatives. These social bonds, in turn, several are twisted together and seem inseparable, and few are
predicted local community attitudes and sentiments. apparent to outside observers. The tapestry that describes the
Similarly, other community attachment researchers (e.g., nature of ones relationship to a place is unique for each individual.
Woldoff, 2002) assume that attachment to a place means attach-
ment to those who live there and to the social interactions that the 5. The functions of place attachment
place affords them. Lalli (1992) notes that spatial bonds become
important largely because they symbolize social bonds. Thus, part According to our personprocessplace (PPP) framework, place
of social place bonding involves attachment to the others with attachment is a bond between an individual or group and a place
whom individuals interact in their place, and part of it involves that can vary in terms of spatial level, degree of specicity, and
attachment to the social group that the place represents. This latter social or physical features of the place, and is manifested through
type of attachment, and recognition that the place symbolizes ones affective, cognitive, and behavioral psychological processes.
social group, is closely aligned with place identity (Twigger-Ross & However, the question of why people develop such enduring
Uzzell, 1996); one is attached to the place because it facilitates psychological bonds with place remains. Most likely, place attach-
distinctiveness from other places, or afrms the specialness of ment bonds exist because they serve several functions. Of those
ones group. Civic place attachment is an instance of group- speculated upon in the literature, the most common include
symbolic place attachment that occurs at the city level (e.g., Hidalgo survival and security, goal support, and temporal or personal
& Hernandez, 2001; Vorkinn & Riese, 2001). Nationalism is another continuity. Implicit in each of these proposed functions is a partic-
example of attachment to, and identication with, a place repre- ular denition of place attachment, which will be discussed in
sentative of ones group, but on a broader scale (Bonaiuto, Break- relation to the PPP framework.
well, & Cano, 1996). These denitions suggest that social place
attachment can sometimes center upon the place as an arena for 5.1. Place attachment for survival and security
social interactions, or as a symbol for ones social group.
However, attachment obviously can also rest on the physical One perspective is that place attachment arises because certain
features of the place. The denition of place dependence, for places offer survival advantages. Using the framework, we can
instance, highlights the physical characteristics of a place as central classify this perspective as one that emphasizes the physical
to attachment because it provides amenities or resources to aspects of the place (i.e., resources), and focuses on the behavioral
support ones goals (Stokols & Shumaker, 1981). The types of places and cognitive expressions of the attachment. The behavioral bond
that individuals nd meaningful represent a broad range of phys- is expressed by maintaining proximity to places that supply the
ical settings, from built environments such as houses, streets, necessities of food, water, shelter, and other resources (e.g., Shu-
certain buildings, and non-residential indoor settings, to natural maker & Taylor, 1983), and the cognitive bond is the knowledge and
environments such as lakes, parks, trails, forests, and mountains familiarity of how these resources may be extracted or used within
(Manzo, 2003, 2005). the place (Turnbull, 1987). Discussions of place attachment as
The level of specicity of the physical attachment is important. survival typically occur at the individual level of analysis.
For example, one study examined whether attachment to a specic The view that place attachment supports physiological need
place, namely the Rattlesnake Wilderness in Montanas Lolo satisfaction is comparable to Freuds (1940, p. 188) claim that
National Forest, differs from attachment to a class of places, such as interpersonal attachment develops because the mother satises
wilderness (Williams et al., 1992). Those with greater place- the infants physiological needs; he writes, love has its origin in
focused attachment were less willing to substitute their place for attachment to the satised need for nourishment. Bowlby (1969),
another. Those whose attachment was more wilderness-focused however, rejected Freuds theory, and argued that attachment
were more likely to have visited other wilderness areas, and belong functions to provide a sense of security, as shown in Harlows
to a wilderness organization. (1961) study of infant rhesus monkeys that were separated from
A related concept, environmental identity (Clayton, 2003), their mothers at birth, and given replacement mothers made
refers to the inclusion of nature into ones self-concept. Clayton from either wire or cloth. The monkeys exhibited attachment
notes that self-denitions aligned with parts of the natural world behaviors (e.g., clinging when frightened) towards the cloth
may stem from a general attachment to nature. Again, this mothers even when the wire monkeys provided milk. Bowlby
emphasizes that place attachment may be directed toward a phys- suggested, therefore, that attachment forms not because it satises
ical aspect of the place in this case nature. physiological needs, but rather, because it offers a sense of security
How do physical features come to affect the formation of the and comfort.
place attachment? The meaning-mediated model of place attach- Similar security-seeking motives may explain place attachment;
ment (Stedman, 2003) proposes that individuals do not become indeed, several authors have alluded to safety and security as
directly attached to the physical features of a place, but rather to the a function of place attachment (e.g., Chatterjee, 2005; Fried, 2000;
meaning that those features represent. Stedman showed how Fullilove, 1996; Giuliani, 2003). Security perspectives on place
a developed area may symbolize community, or an attachment describe the bond as one of positive affect, cognitions of
6 L. Scannell, R. Gifford / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 110

reduced risk, and proximity-maintaining behaviors. Fried (2000) supplied from an individual, cognitively based attachment through
argued that individuals maintain proximity to their signicant which individuals identify with a place according to the extent it
place because it offers protection and a sense of security, which in seems to t the self. That is, individuals are more often attached
turn, increases condence and allows for exploration. That is, once to environments that they feel match their personal values, and
closeness enhances feelings of safety, individuals are more willing thus seem to appropriately represent them, an experience which
to venture from their places. In the event of a personal threat, Twigger-Ross and Uzzell called place-congruent continuity.
individuals may exhibit a sort of clinging behavior by reducing Place bonds also provide continuity over time (e.g., Twigger-
their range and remaining close to home. Furthermore, just as Ross & Uzzell, 1996). This function stems from individual attach-
attachment behaviors are stronger among vulnerable individuals ment to a place that is symbolically meaningful through memories
(e.g., someone who is pregnant or sick), Fried suggested that place and connections to the past. For instance, we might be attached to
bonds are often more intense among vulnerable populations (e.g., our childhood homes, or to places that seem to link us to those
immigrants). Thus, the strong emotional bonds of place attach- people we have lost. The place serves as a physical representation of
ment, and its proximity-maintaining behaviors as well as con- important events, and seems to contain their essence, or preserve
trasting journeys away (e.g., Case, 1996), result from the sense of them. Twigger-Ross and Uzzell (1996) observe that places create
safety and security that a place imparts. continuity across time by reminding individuals of episodes that
occurred there in the past, or by allowing individuals to compare
5.2. Place attachment for goal support and self-regulation their present and past selves (place-referent continuity). In this
sense, a setting represents part of an individuals personal history.
Safety alone, however, does not fully explain place attachment. The continuity function can also exist at the cultural or religious
Individuals also become attached to places that support the pursuit level (e.g., Low, 1990). Places where important cultural events have
of their goals. This perspective suggests that the positive affective transpired become meaningful for that group. Behaviors at this
content of the bond results from successful goal pursuit, the level include pilgrimages to a sacred place, designations that the
cognitions consist of expectations of goal attainment based on past place is an historic site, or stories and myths that convey the
experiences, the behavior expressed is repeated place use, and the signicance of the place.
place focus is social or physical, depending on the particular goals
sought (e.g., Kyle, Mowen, & Tarrant, 2004; Proshansky et al., 1983). 5.4. Other functions
This can lead to place dependence, a type of attachment in which
individuals value a place for the specic activities that it supports or This list of the functions of place attachment is by no means
facilitates (Jorgensen & Stedman, 2001; Moore & Graefe, 1994). exhaustive. An additional function is that place attachment
Stokols and Shumaker (1981) found that attachment forms when provides individuals with a sense of belongingness (Giuliani, 2003)
a place provides the resources required for goal attainment, and the one that is mirrored in the interpersonal attachment literature
use of those resources is frequent. For example, a kite surfer who (e.g., La Guardia, Ryan, Couchman, & Deci, 2000). Another is that
visits a particular lake every weekend may be attached to the place attachment to places with distinct social or physical qualities
for its ideal wind conditions. enhance identity and bolster self-esteem (Twigger-Ross & Uzzell,
Others argue that the main function of place attachment is to 1996). Some have suggested that place attachment is a precursor
indirectly support ones goals by facilitating the self-regulatory for place friendship in children (Chatterjee, 2005).
processes necessary for goal attainment (Korpela, 1989). Self- Further research is needed to fully understand the functions of
regulation is the process in which current behavior is compared to place attachment and the particular needs that it fullls. The PPP
ones greater goals and standards in order to evaluate progress framework may be used for this purpose, that is, to explore the
towards the goal (Carver & Scheier, 2001). Emotions provide feed- types of functions at each of the dimensions. For instance, does
back about the level of success, and self-control keeps the behavior attachment to place also support goals at the group level? Or, do
focused toward the goal. However, self-control is a limited resource bonds at varying spatial levels serve different purposes? For
that cannot function optimally under cognitive load (Wegner, example, attachment to ones neighborhood may meet different
1994). Because place attachment enhances positive emotions and needs than attachment to ones city. Finally, research could use the
allows for cognitive freedom, emotions can be regulated and framework to name different forms of personplace bonds and
cognitive load can be reduced. In particular, place attachment examine the functions unique to each type of bond.
serves self-regulation because favorite places have restorative
qualities (Korpela, 1989). In several studies of childrens favorite 6. Testing the multidimensionality of place attachment
places, reported experiences of place visits tended to have
a restorative theme (Korpela, Hartig, Kaiser, & Fuhrer, 2001; Kor- The PPP framework assumes that the dimensions and levels of
pela, Kytta, & Hartig, 2002). Restoration within a favorite place place attachment may overlap, but may also be separable. There-
improves self-regulatory processes by providing a secure, fore, validation of the framework will require evidence that the
comfortable environment conducive to self-reection, problem- components of place attachment are as proposed. One approach
solving, and stress relief. A favorite place is a safe haven, where would be to develop a place attachment measurement instrument,
individuals can plan for the implementation of their goals, and can and explore the factors that emerge. Another would be to
evaluate their progress. These ndings reect studies which show demonstrate place attachments multidimensionality by showing
that interpersonal attachment supports self-regulatory processes that its effects differ depending on the type or level of the attach-
(Izard & Kobak, 1991). ment (Kyle, Graefe, Manning, & Bacon, 2004; Stedman, 2002;
Vorkinn & Riese, 2001). For example, Vorkinn and Riese (2001)
5.3. Place attachment for continuity showed that Norwegian residents who were strongly attached to
specic areas of a municipality tended to express more opposition
Another function of place attachment is that it provides conti- to a proposed hydropower plant development, but that those who
nuity. Self-continuity is a stable sense of self, or an awareness of the were especially attached to the municipality as a whole were in
self as continuous, such that past and future behaviors are linked favor of the development. This supports the view that a simple
(Hallowell, 1955; Robinson & Freeman, 1954). This function is partly measure of place attachment does not always predict attitudes or
L. Scannell, R. Gifford / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 110 7

willingness to oppose threats, and that within the place dimension, by their unique effects on other constructs). Another difference lies
the type and level of the attachment must be considered. in the organization of the frameworks. Following trends and
In another study, two levels within the psychological dimension debates in the literature, the PPP framework divides the place
of place attachment were shown to exert opposite effects on atti- dimension into social and physical levels (e.g., Hidalgo & Hernan-
tudes toward social and environmental conditions along the dez, 2001; Stedman, 2003). Most importantly, the PPP framework is
Appalachian Trail (Kyle, Graefe, Manning, & Bacon, 2004). Partici- based on a review of both classic and recent studies, and so it is
pants with a greater sense of place identity viewed problems along grounded in newer insights and ndings.
the trail (such as crowding, litter, or noise) to be more important, Although most denitions of place attachment and related
but those with a greater sense of place dependence perceived concepts t well within the tripartite framework, earlier frame-
problems to be less important. Although few such studies exist, works and denitions tend to represent pieces of the whole. By
they support the multidimensionality of place attachment by connecting these disparate denitions, the PPP framework provides
demonstrating that the specic dimensions can have unique effects a view of place attachment that is more integrative and inclusive.
on other concepts.
8. Uses of the PPP framework
7. PPP in relation to other frameworks
The proposed personprocessplace framework of place
The PPP framework structures the plethora of place attachment attachment builds upon previous frameworks and models by
denitions into a simple, three-dimensional framework. Previous incorporating classic and recent empirical ndings from a variety of
frameworks and models have also attempted to dene place theoretical perspectives. It thus serves as a portrait of place
attachment, but these denitions are limited. For instance, Full- attachment research to date. As Blalock (1969) advised, the rst
iloves (1996) conceptualization is exclusive to the psychological step toward construction of testable theories is to scrutinize the
process dimension. Woldoff (2002) and Hunters (1978) models current literature, identify key variables, and, when there are many,
depict place attachment as a mainly social phenomenon, in which reduce them into an organized classication system. The PPP
place facilitates social ties, and is expressed through community framework endeavors to do just that. It connects and integrates the
action or informal neighboring, but other conceptualizations show many constructs within its three dimensions. Therefore, it presents
that place attachment is often directed toward physical features of an encompassing, yet simplied view of personplace bonding that
the place, and that it can be expressed through a variety of is both accessible to those new to the place attachment concept and
behavioral modalities. Sense of community (e.g., McMillan & Cha- comprehensive to those already familiar with it. It should stimulate
vis, 1986) is another concept that details the nature of social new research by identifying gaps in previous studies, aid in the
attachments, but given that sense of community is not always development of assessment tools, and categorize types of place
place-bound (i.e., for communities of interest), it departs from attachment for planning purposes and related conict resolution
a model that focuses on personenvironment transactions. Place strategies.
identity is also relevant to place attachment, because it is a cogni-
tive-emotional connection of self to place (e.g., Proshansky, 1978), 8.1. Organization and accessibility
but it does not detail place aspects or behavioral expressions of
personplace bonding. Those new to the study of place attachment have likely strug-
Some models of place attachment have been more inclusive, gled with its multitude of terms and varying constructs; as some
such as that of Shumaker and Taylor (1983), which dened place have warned, denitional inconsistencies can hinder progress in
attachment as a multidimensional bond between persons and their the eld (e.g., Giuliani & Feldman, 1993). The PPP framework,
residential environments. Like the PPP framework, it placed however, showcases the diversity of denitions and structures
importance on the physical and social aspects of place, and them into an organized, coherent framework that is based on
emphasized affect as a key psychological expression of the bond. empirical works from different disciplines. Thus, the concept of
However, in the twenty-ve years since their model was intro- place attachment becomes more coherent, broad, and accessible.
duced, a collection of studies has accrued which indicate that it too For this reason, the framework could also serve as a teaching tool
narrowly denes place attachment (e.g., Clayton, 2003; Mazumdar for introducing place attachment, or it could further clarify the
& Mazumdar, 2004). Place attachment is not simply a residential construct for those who are already familiar with aspects of place
concept, but applies to a variety of places, and the PPP framework attachment.
allows for this diversity, connecting the different types of bonds
into a single overarching concept. Although Shumaker and Taylor 8.2. Denitional and methodological uses
emphasized that affect is an important part of the attachment, they
did not focus on its behavioral and cognitive components, the latter Some denitions of place attachment, such as proximity-
of which described residents evaluations of place-worth relative to maintaining behavior (e.g., Hidalgo & Hernandez, 2001), or social
alternatives. In contrast, the cognitive level in the PPP framework bondedness and physical rootedness (Riger & Lavrakas, 1981)
includes memories, schemas, meaning, and knowledge. Finally, emphasize particular aspects of the attachment, and have
Shumaker and Taylor incorporated social and physical place contributed to the diversity of the construct. However, relating such
features into their model, but recent studies have shown that these relatively narrow denitions to the broader construct of place
may be further distinguished into social-symbolic and social attachment, and understanding how these terms t together, can
interaction levels, as well as natural and built levels, divisions be difcult. Given that the PPP framework is multidimensional and
which are highlighted in the PPP framework. comprehensive, researchers should be able to use it to situate their
A second important multidimensional model of place attach- topic-specic denitions of place attachment within the tradition
ment is that proposed by Low and Altman (1992). Like the PPP of place attachment research. For instance, future denitions of
framework, it suggests that place attachment consists of different place attachment could refer to the framework to specify the
places, actors, and psychological processes. However, Low and relative importance placed on each of the dimensions: is the
Altman suggest that these levels are inseparable, but the PPP attachment individually or collectively based? Is it experienced
framework presents these levels related, yet distinct (as evidenced affectively, cognitively, and behaviorally? What is the place
8 L. Scannell, R. Gifford / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 110

component of the attachment? As researchers begin to investigate received, and so a clear understanding of individuals attachment is
place attachment according to its dimensions, knowledge about the an important part of successful planning (e.g., Manzo & Perkins,
construct will be more easily organized and synthesized. Research 2006). Furthermore, conicts can arise from planning when places
may be located within a common framework, rather than con- mean different things to different people. Manzo and Perkins
ducted idiosyncratically in each study. In addition, this may guide proposed a consensus-building approach to conict resolution in
the development of quantitative instruments. Researchers could this domain. The end goals are to create shared values and social
include items, for instance, from one of each of the psychological capital among the factions, but the unique place meanings held by
process levels or justify why some levels are of interest to the study each must rst be unearthed. Manzo and Perkins did not, however,
but the others are not. For qualitative studies, the framework could propose a method to reveal these meanings and characterize each
be used to develop semi-structured interviews, in which question groups attachment. The PPP framework will complement this, and
categories are based on each of the frameworks three dimensions. other planning situations that benet from analyses of person
place bonds. Specically, open-ended questions based on the
8.3. Heuristic value framework could be used to facilitate discussion and arrive at an
understanding of the particular nature of individuals attachments.
Importantly, the PPP framework should stimulate new research. If other, less comprehensive place attachment frameworks were
After simplifying and organizing the constructs, a next step is to used, key elements of the bond could be overlooked.
further simplify the framework such as by exploring the frequen-
cies of different combinations of the levels (Blalock, 1969).
Combinations that are incompatible, or low in frequency, may be 9. Conclusion
excluded from a resulting typology (Blalock, 1969). This endeavor
would be of particular interest to the study of place attachment: This review of the various denitions of personplace bonding
which combinations of the levels are most and least common for organizes them into a clear three-dimensional framework. This
which individuals, groups, or places? Furthermore, interactions synthesis of the place attachment construct can be used in theo-
between the proposed dimensions and levels could be examined. retical explorations and in practical domains. This is not to say that
For instance, would individually based place attachment differ from all levels of the place attachment concept must be examined in
collectively based place attachment in terms of the prevalence and each study, but that denitions of place attachment should be
content of related behaviors, cognitions, and emotions? specied according to their relative emphasis within each of the
The PPP framework will also contribute to other levels of person, process, and place dimensions. Future research on the
theorizing, including inventories of causes and effects (e.g., Blalock, unique effects and functions of the dimensions, and the different
1969), which can then be used to identify gaps in past research. levels within them, will lend further support for the frameworks
Inventories about causes would help to clarify the inuence of the structure. The renement and use of the PPP framework will clarify
different levels of place attachment on other concepts. Discrepant understanding of place attachments relations to other concepts,
ndings caused by denitional differences would become more and provide consistency in place attachment research. Ultimately,
apparent, and the unique effects of the various dimensions on other the PPP framework portrays the diversity of place attachment
concepts could begin to be assessed. Isolation of the levels most denitions that have accrued thus far, and offers a coherent
important to such concepts has applied value, for example, in structural alternative to the related concepts that previously
understanding the effects of place attachment on pro-environ- remained scattered in the literature.
mental behavior. One possibility is that civic and natural attach-
ment might inuence pro-environmental behavior in different
Acknowledgements
ways. That is, the levels of place attachment that are conceptually
related to a specic behavior may predict that behavior much
The authors would like to thank Dr. Frederick Grouzet for his
better than a general measure of place attachment.
helpful feedback on earlier versions of this manuscript. The authors
Inventories of effects would help to explore the inuence of
are also grateful to the anonymous reviewers for their informed
various predictors on place attachment. Research might compare
comments and insightful suggestions.
the effects of personality types, cultures, and environments with
objectively different properties on the different levels or typologies
of place attachment. For instance, does the frequency of the levels References
within the framework differ among collectivistic and individualistic
cultures? How does the psychological content of the bond differ for Altman, I. (1975). The environment and social behavior. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.
those who are attached to natural versus non-natural places? Aronson, E., Wilson, T. D., Akert, R. M., & Fehr, B. (2005). Social psychology. Engle-
wood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Several studies have described the development of place attach- Bartlett, D. C. (1932). Remembering. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
ment (e.g., Fried, 2000; Hay, 1998; Low, 1992), but these processes Billig, M. (2006). Is my home my castle? Place attachment, risk perception, and
may not be relevant to each of its dimensions. Future research religious faith. Environment and Behavior, 38, 248265.
Blalock, H. M. (1969). Theory construction: From verbal to mathematical formulations.
could, therefore, investigate whether the development of place Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
attachment in general is applicable to all of the dimensions, and if Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss. In: Attachment, Vol. 1. New York: Basic Books.
not, explain which differences exist. For example, what are the Bonaiuto, M., Breakwell, G. M., & Cano, I. (1996). Identity processes and environ-
mental threat: the effects of nationalism and local identity upon perception of
determinants of place attachments that are more physically or beach pollution. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 6, 157175.
socially based? This knowledge would be useful for those who wish Brewer, M. B. (1991). The social self: on being same and different at the same time.
to encourage the development of certain types of place attachment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 475482.
Brown, B. B., & Perkins, D. D. (1992). Disruptions in place attachment. Human
Behavior & Environment: Advances in Theory & Research, 12, 279304.
8.4. Applied uses Brown, B., Perkins, D. D., & Brown, G. (2003). Place attachment in a revitalizing
neighborhood: individual and block levels of analysis. Journal of Environmental
The framework is also relevant to applied settings. One example Psychology, 23, 259271.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2001). Optimism, pessimism, and self-regulation. In
is in planning and land-use decisions. Plans that incorporate or E. C. Chang (Ed.), Optimism and pessimism: Implications for theory, research, and
enhance elements central to the meaning of the place are better- practice (pp. 3152). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
L. Scannell, R. Gifford / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 110 9

Case, D. (1996). Contributions of journeys away to the denition of home: an La Guardia, J. G., Ryan, R. M., Couchman, C. E., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Within-person
empirical study of a dialectical process. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 16, variation in security of attachment: a self-determination theory perspective on
115. attachment, need fulllment, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social
Chatterjee, S. (2005). Childrens friendship with place: a conceptual inquiry. Chil- Psychology, 79, 367384.
dren, Youth and Environments, 15, 126. Lalli, M. (1992). Urban-related identity: theory, measurement and empirical nd-
Clayton, S. (2003). Environmental identity: conceptual and operational denition. ings. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 12, 285303.
In S. Clayton, & S. Opotow (Eds.), Identity and the natural environment: The Low, S. M. (1990). Cross-cultural place attachment: a preliminary typology. In
psychological signicance of nature (pp. 4565). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Y. Yoshitake, R. B. Bechtel, T. Takahashi, & M. Asai (Eds.), Current issues in
Cuba, L., & Hummon, D. M. (1993). A place to call home: identication with environment-behavior research. Tokyo: University of Tokyo.
dwelling, community, and region. Sociological Quarterly, 34, 111131. Low, S. M. (1992). Symbolic ties that bind. In I. Altman, & S. M. Low (Eds.), Place
Deutsch, N. (2005). A second home. In B. J. Hirsch (Ed.), A place to call home: After- attachment (pp. 165185). New York: Plenum Press.
school programs for urban youth (pp. 4156). Washington, DC: American Low, S. M., & Altman, I. (1992). Place attachment: a conceptual inquiry. In I. Altman,
Psychological Association. & S. M. Low (Eds.), Place attachment (pp. 112). New York: Plenum Press.
Dovey, K. (1985). Home and homeless. In I. Altman, & C. M. Werner (Eds.), Home Manzo, L. C. (2005). For better or worse: exploring multiple dimensions of place
environments (pp. 3364). New York: Plenum Press. meaning. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 25, 6786.
Feldman, R. M. (1990). Settlement identity: psychological bonds with home places Manzo, L. C. (2003). Beyond house and haven: toward a revisioning of
in a mobile society. Environment and Behavior, 22, 183229. emotional relationships with places. Journal of Environmental Psychology,
Francaviglia, R. V. (1978). Xenia rebuilds: effects of pre-disaster conditioning on 23, 4761.
post-disaster redevelopment. Journal of the American Institute of Planners, 44, Manzo, L. C., & Perkins, D. D. (2006). Neighborhoods as common ground: the
1324. importance of place attachment to community participation and development.
Freud, S. (1940). In J. Strachey (Ed.), The standard edition of the complete psycho- Journal of Planning Literature, 20, 335350.
logical works of Sigmund Freud, 23. London: Hogarth Press. Markus, H. (1977). Self-schemata and processing information about the self. Journal
Fried, M. (1963). Grieving for a lost home. In L. J. Duhl (Ed.), The urban condition: of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 6378.
People and policy in the metropolis (pp. 124152). New York: Simon & Schuster. Mazumdar, S., & Mazumdar, S. (2004). Religion and place attachment: a study of
Fried, M. (2000). Continuities and discontinuities of place. Journal of Environmental sacred places. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 24, 385397.
Psychology, 20, 193205. McMillan, D. W., & Chavis, D. M. (1986). Sense of community: a denition and
Fullilove, M. T. (1996). Psychiatric implications of displacement: contributions from theory. Journal of Community Psychology, 14, 623.
the psychology of place. American Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 15161523. Mesch, G. S., & Manor, O. (1998). Social ties, environmental perception, and local
Gans, H. (1962). The urban villagers: Group and class in the life of Italian-Americans. attachment. Environment and Behavior, 30, 227245.
New York: Free Press of Glencoe. Michelson, W. (1976). Man and his urban environment: A sociological approach, with
Geipel, R. (1982). Disasters and reconstruction: The Friuli earthquakes of 1976. revisions. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
London: George Allen & Unwin. Moore, R. L., & Graefe, A. R. (1994). Attachments to recreation settings. Leisure
Giuliani, M. V. (2003). Theory of attachment and place attachment. In M. Bonnes, Sciences, 16, 1731.
T. Lee, & M. Bonaiuto (Eds.), Psychological theories for environmental issues (pp. Nasar, J. L., & Julian, D. A. (1995). The psychological sense of community in the
137170). Aldershot: Ashgate. neighborhood. Journal of American Planning Association, 61, 178184.
Giuliani, M. V., & Feldman, R. (1993). Place attachment in a developmental and Ng, C. (1998). Canada as a new place: the immigrants experience. Journal of Envi-
cultural context. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 13, 267274. ronmental Psychology, 18, 5567.
Giuliani, M., Ferrara, F., & Barabotti, S. (2003). One attachment or more? Ashland, OH: Nordenstam, B. J. (1994 March). When communities say NIMBY to their LULUS:
Hogrefe & Huber. Factors inuencing environmental and social impact perception, Paper pre-
Gustafson, P. (2001). Roots and routes: exploring the relationship between place sented at the 14th Annual Meeting of the International Association for Impact
attachment and mobility. Environment and Behavior, 33, 667686. Assessment, Quebec, Canada.
Hallowell, A. I. (1955). Culture and experience. Philadelphia: University of Pennsyl- Perkins, D. D., & Long, D. A. (2002). Neighborhood sense of community and social
vania Press. capital: a multi-level analysis. In A. Fisher, C. Sonn, & B. Bishop (Eds.), Psycho-
Harlow, H. (1961). The development of affectional patterns in infant monkeys. In logical sense of community: Research, applications, and implications (pp. 291
B. M. Foss (Ed.), Determinants of infant behavior, Vol. 1. New York: Wiley. 318). New York: Plenum.
Hay, R. (1998). Sense of place in developmental context. Journal of Environmental Pretty, G. H., Chipuer, H., & Bramston, P. (2003). Sense of place amongst adolescents
Psychology, 18, 529. and adults in two rural Australian towns: the discriminating features of place
Hidalgo, M. C., & Hernandez, B. (2001). Place attachment: conceptual and empirical attachment, sense of community and place dependence in relation to place
questions. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 21, 273281. identity. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 23, 273287.
Hummon, D. M. (1992). Community attachment: local sentiment and sense of place. Proshansky, H. M. (1978). The city and self-identity. Environment and Behavior, 10,
In I. Altman, & S. M. Low (Eds.), Place attachment (pp. 253278). New York: 147169.
Plenum Press. Proshansky, H. M., & Fabian, A. K. (1987). The development of place identity in the
Hunter, A. (1974). Symbolic communities. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. child. In C. S. Weinstein, & T. G. David (Eds.), Spaces for children (pp. 2140). New
Hunter, A. (1978). Persistence of local sentiments in mans society. In C. Street (Ed.), York: Plenum.
Handbook of contemporary urban life (pp. 133162). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Proshansky, H. M., Fabian, A. K., & Kaminoff, R. (1983). Place-identity. Journal of
Izard, C. E., & Kobak, R. R. (1991). Emotions system functioning and emotion Environmental Psychology, 3, 5783.
regulation. In J. Garber, & K. A. Dodge (Eds.), The development of emotion regu- Proshansky, H. M., Ittelson, W. H., & Rivlin, L. G. (Eds.). (1970). Environmental
lation and dysregulation (pp. 303321). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University psychology: Man and his physical setting. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Press. Relph, E. (1976). Place and placelessness. London: Pion Limited.
Jorgensen, B. S., & Stedman, R. C. (2001). Sense of place as an attitude: lakeshore Riemer, J. W. (2004). Job relocation, sources of stress, and sense of home.
owners attitudes toward their properties. Journal of Environmental Psychology, Community, Work & Family, 2, 205217.
21, 233248. Riger, S., & Lavrakas, P. J. (1981). Community ties: patterns of attachment and social
Kates, R. W., Eriksen, N. J., Pijawka, D., & Bowden, M. J. (1977). Alternative pasts and interaction in urban neighborhoods. American Journal of Community Psychology,
futures. In J. E. Haas, R. W. Kates, & M. J. Bowden (Eds.), Reconstruction following 9, 5566.
disaster (pp. 207259). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Riley, R. B. (1992). Attachment to the ordinary landscape. In I. Altman, & S. M. Low
Kasarda, J. D., & Janowitz, M. (1974). Community attachment in mass society. (Eds.), Place attachment (pp. 1335). New York: Plenum.
American Sociological Review, 39, 328339. Robinson, M. P., & Freeman, W. (1954). Psychosurgery and the self. New York: Grune
Knez, I. (2005). Attachment and identity as related to a place and its perceived & Stratton.
climate. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 25, 207218. Rubenstein, R. L., & Parmelee, P. A. (1992). Attachment to place and the represen-
Korpela, K. M. (1989). Place-identity as a product of environmental self-regulation. tation of the life course by the elderly. In I. Altman, & S. M. Low (Eds.), Place
Journal of Environmental Psychology, 9, 241256. attachment (pp. 139163). New York: Plenum.
Korpela, K., Hartig, T., Kaiser, F. G., & Fuhrer, U. (2001). Restorative experience and Sanders, S., Bowie, S. L., & Bowie, Y. D. (2003). Lessons learned on forced relocation
self-regulation in favorite places. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 16, of older adults: the impact of hurricane Andrew on health, mental health, and
221233. social support of public housing residents. Journal of Gerontological Social Work,
Korpela, K., Kytta, M., & Hartig, T. (2002). Childrens favorite places: restorative 40, 2335.
experience, self-regulation and childrens place preferences. Journal of Envi- Sears, D. O., Freedman, J. L., & Peplau, L. A. (1985). In Social psychology (5th ed.).
ronmental Psychology, 22, 387398. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Kyle, G. T., Graefe, A., & Manning, R. E. (2005). Testing the dimensionality of place Sennett, R. (2000). New capitalism, new isolation: A exible city of strangers. Le
attachment in recreational settings. Environment and Behavior, 37, 153177. Monde Diplomatique. (English Edition).
Kyle, G. T., Graefe, A., Manning, R. E., & Bacon, J. (2004). Effect of involvement and Shumaker, S. A., & Taylor, R. B. (1983). Toward a clarication of people place rela-
place attachment on recreationists perceptions of setting density. Journal of tionships: a model of attachment to place. In N. R. Feimer, & E. S. Geller (Eds.),
Leisure Research, 36, 209231. Environmental psychology: Directions and perspectives (pp. 219251). New York:
Kyle, G. T., Mowen, A. J., & Tarrant, M. (2004). Linking place preferences with place Praeger.
meaning: an examination of the relationship between place motivation and Stedman, R. (2002). Toward a social psychology of place. Environment and Behavior,
place attachment. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 24, 439454. 34, 561581.
10 L. Scannell, R. Gifford / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 110

Stedman, R. (2003). Is it really just a social construction? The contribution of the Virden, R. J., & Walker, G. J. (1999). Ethnic/racial and gender variations among meanings
physical environment to sense of place. Society and Natural Resources, 16, 671685. given to, and preferences for, the natural environment. Leisure Sciences, 21, 219239.
Stokols, D., & Shumaker, S. A. (1981). People in places: a transactional view of Vorkinn, M., & Riese, H. (2001). Environmental concern in a local context: the
settings. In J. Harvey (Ed.), Cognition, social behavior, and the environment (pp. signicance of place attachment. Environment and Behavior, 33, 249263.
441488). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101,
Tuan, Y. (1974). Topophilia: A study of environmental perception, attitudes, and values. 3452.
Englewood Cliffs, N.J: Prentice Hall. Williams, D. R., Patterson, M. E., Roggenbuck, J. W., & Watson, A. E. (1992). Beyond
Turnbull, C. M. (1987). The forest people. New York: Touchstone. the commodity metaphor: Examining emotional and symbolic attachment to
Twigger-Ross, C. L., & Uzzell, D. L. (1996). Place and identity processes. Journal of place. Leisure Sciences, 14, 2946.
Environmental Psychology, 16, 205220. Williams, D. R., & Stewart, S. I. (1998). Sense of place. Journal of Forestry, 98,
Uzzell, D., Pol, E., & Badenas, D. (2002). Place identication, social cohesion, and 1823.
environmental sustainability. Environment and Behavior, 34, 2653. Woldoff, R. A. (2002). The effects of local stressors on neighborhood attachment.
Vaske, J. J., & Kobrin, K. C. (2001). Place attachment and environmentally respon- Social Forces, 81, 87116.
sible behavior. Journal of Environmental Education, 32, 1621. Young, M., & Willmott, P. (1962). Family and kinship in East London. London: Pelican.

You might also like