You are on page 1of 17

Aging Clinical and Experimental Research

REVIEW ARTICLE

Death and dying from old peoples point of view.


A literature review*
Ingalill Rahm Hallberg
The Swedish Institute for Health Sciences, Lund University, Lund, Sweden

ABSTRACT. Providing high-quality end-of-life care INTRODUCTION


to older people is a requirement especially for coun- Death and dying belong to the natural course of life. Old
tries with a high proportion of old and very old and especially very old people who are close to the end of
people. This calls for an understanding of older their lives may ponder and reflect over their life and forth-
peoples view of death and dying, and one way for- coming death and dying process (1). Their views of death
ward is to investigate the current knowledge base. and dying are of the utmost importance for health care staff
This study aimed at reviewing the literature of em- to know, since they may form the basis for how to ap-
pirical studies about older peoples view of death proach people at the end of life and release them from
and dying, whether in a terminal phase of life or death anxiety. Research findings from younger people
not. A total of 33 publications were included, iden- cannot easily be generalized to older people, since death
tified in a stepwise literature search done in Med- and dying most likely take on a different meaning earlier in
line, CINAHL and PsychInfo, using the terms life. Also, the view of death and dying may differ between
death, attitude to death, death and dying in younger old, old and very old people, because they are at
combination with aged. Very few studies focused different points in their life span. Thom (2) reported from
solely on the oldest old. The designs were mainly a study of quality of life (QoL) in old people (75-99 years of
cross-sectional, quantitative or qualitative, using age) with cancer that those between 75 and 79 years of age
personal interviews. Some common themes of im- had the lowest QoL, and that thereafter the differences be-
portance for further research were revealed, such as tween those with cancer and those without faded away. It
older peoples readiness to talk about death and dy- may well be that the aging process includes adapting to the
ing, conceptions of death, after-death and dying, fact that life will come to an end.
and were seemingly related to anxiety about death, The knowledge of how older people relate to their
the impact on and of those close by, having both death and dying should preferably be from an inside per-
negative and positive connotations, especially re- spective, e.g., derived from their narrations and not only
lated to balancing closeness, being a burden and de- at the end-phase of life but also before that phase. The pro-
pendency, death anxiety and its possible an- cess of dying may be short or take place over a long pe-
tecedents, the fine line between natural sadness riod. In most cases, the older person is in contact with pro-
and suffering from depression, and worry about fessionals in long-term care in hospital, or living in special
the end-of-life phase. The lack of studies dealing accommodation or receiving home care (3). Although
with older peoples view of death and dying, and the there seems to have been a change in that not so many
heterogeneity with regard to research questions older people die in hospital as earlier on, still most deaths
and samples implies that findings may serve main- take place in hospitals or nursing homes, i.e., in profes-
ly as inspiration for further research. sional care settings (4). A literature review of place of
(Aging Clin Exp Res 2004; 16: 87-103) death and access to home care in the end-phase of life ac-
2004, Editrice Kurtis tually indicated inequalities, in that those in poor eco-

*Review article based on the 2nd Andrus Viidik Lecture in Gerontology at the 17th Nordic Congress of Gerontology, Stockholm, Sweden,
May 23-26, 2004. The Nordic Gerontological Federation (NGF) initiated these lectures when Professor Viidik retired from the NGF, after
being its secretary 1974-1988 and chairman 1988-2002.
Key words: Aged, attitude to death, death, dying, end-of-life care, older people, systematic review.
Correspondence: Prof. I.R. Hallberg, The Swedish Institute for Health Sciences, PO Box 187, Lund University, SE 221 00 Lund, Sweden.
E-mail: Ingalill.rahm_hallberg@omv.lu.se
Received September 12, 2003; accepted in revised form December 9, 2003.

Aging Clin Exp Res, Vol. 16, No. 2 87


I.R. Hallberg

nomic conditions and those with poor access to informal baum (10), that death anxiety decreases rather than
caregiving were less likely to die at home (5). The re- increases with age. He recognized the fact that longi-
view, however, did not focus on old or very old people, tudinal studies are required to determine whether death
who may have even poorer access to informal caregivers anxiety increases or decreases with age and that such
and thus fewer options to die at home. In any case, a per- studies are not yet available. Older peoples explanations
son in the end-phase of life is likely to stay in contact with for their low scores were that they viewed death in the
professionals of various kinds, and their understanding of light of their entire life and the accomplishments made
the older persons end-of-life phase is part of the psycho- during their lifetime, and death could not take that
social milieu of older people and their dying process. A re- away (p. 368). Similarly, Aiken (13) reported that cross-
cent review showed that physicians predictions of survival sectional surveys show lower fear of death in older
in terminal cancer patients were highly correlated to actual adults than in middle-aged people, recognizing possible
survival, although mostly overestimated (6). Understanding cohort effects in these studies. He suggested that the el-
of older peoples view of death and dying is fundamental derly are more likely to see themselves as having had
to providing them with high-quality care at the end of life. their day and to view death in old age as only fair (p.
However, knowledge of older peoples view of their own 371) and that older people are better able to cope
forthcoming death and the dying process is sparse (7, 8). with the forthcoming death, partly because of their life
A systematic review of the literature on the doctor-pa- experiences, their failing health and the loss of people
tient relationship initiated by the Swedish Council on close to them. Others have likewise reported that the at-
Technology Assessment in Health Care located few em- titude towards death seems to change over the life
pirical studies focusing on people 65 years or older and span (14) and emphasized that it is in the light of a re-
their view of how to approach matters related to death view of life that death is regarded. The life span per-
and dying (9). They showed that doctors hesitate to spective is perhaps best understood within the frame-
bring up these matters, whilst the patients appreciate it work of narrative gerontology suggesting, among other
if they do and want to talk about it, although they some- things, that the meaning and nature of time is con-
times think it is emotionally upsetting. Kastenbaum (10) nected to our lives as a story, and that the story is
stated that stereotypes, negative or avoiding societal at- open to be changed, i.e., our lives are continuously
titudes towards death and dying, especially in old people, re-storied (15). Supposing that death and dying is re-
influence those in touch with older people, not to say the viewed from a life span perspective, agony may differ in
older persons themselves. Perhaps these societal attitudes those who reach old age with a certain bitterness from
may explain findings along the lines that doctors tend not those who look back at their life with satisfaction, but
to talk about death to older people, although they are open to be re-storied.
willing to discuss their own death and the end-phase of From previous research it seems important to separate
life, and this aggravates their end of life. Also, it has been older peoples view of death (the very last moment of life,
reported that physicians who spend more time caring for the moment of death, and the following moment) from
their patients experience feelings of loss when those that of dying (the time preceding death, when it is obvious
patients die (11). This may in turn contribute to avoidance that the ongoing process will end in death). Also, the view
if no debriefing is available. of death and dying in older people not in an end-of-life
Older people being willing to talk about death and dy- phase (i.e., it has been established that the person is in the
ing coincides with observations from interview studies terminal phase of life) may differ from that of older peo-
carried out at our department, including especially the ple in the end-of-life phase or people who have just
oldest old. Interviewees spontaneously brought up issues been told that they have a terminal disease. Kbler-Ross
related to death and dying, although the interviews (16) suggested that people in the end-of-life phase went
were not set to cover those areas. In their narrations, it through five stages from denial/shock, anger, bargaining,
was striking that they seemingly did not fear death; and depression, preparatory to acceptance. To my knowl-
some of them looked forward in a seemingly unaffected edge, this theory of the dying process was not developed
way, others more in a style of adaptation and accepting primarily from older peoples views. However, Thom (12)
that this is what life is about (12). Their way of narrating found that older people (range 75-88), when told that they
gave the impression that they were mentally prepared had cancer, were stricken by a sudden awareness of the
for the fact that their remaining lifetime was limited, and finiteness of life, which they processed in the light of their
they did not convey the impression of depression but life span and other peoples death and dying. They ac-
rather of acceptance. They did not speak about death cepted the finiteness of life, but feared dying. These sto-
with agony, which does not mean that they did not ries have features resembling Kbler-Ross (16) phases at
feel agony about death. Those who were agonized may the end of life. Fear of dying, agony, and anxiety, espe-
simply have decided not to speak about death. These ob- cially related to thoughts about suffering, being aban-
servations coincide with statements made by Kasten- doned, and that the health care system may fail to meet

88 Aging Clin Exp Res, Vol. 16, No. 2


Older peoples view of death and dying

their need of care were narrated in the interviews con- quality. The search and selection of studies was broad,
ducted in different studies at our department. Similar so as not to exclude studies that may have contained
observations have been reported previously (10, 13, findings pertinent to the aims. Thus, the large reduction
17). Thus, research about older peoples view of death was mainly due to the fact that it concerned studies giv-
and dying should perhaps take into consideration that ing family perspectives, not including older people at all
death and dying are two separate concepts, related but dif- or studies related to palliative care. The inclusion crite-
ferent, and that the view of them may differ depending on ria were that the study should include older peoples per-
whether a person is at the end-of-life phase or not. spectives, preferably only people over 80 years of age,
The observations about the older persons attitude to and that it should explicitly address their view of death
death and dying and lower degree of death anxiety may and/or dying, in the end-of-life phase or not. It turned
perhaps be understood within the theory of life span out that hardly any studies dealt only with people aged
development put forward by Eriksson et al. (18). They sug- 80 years or over, and thus a wider age range had to be
gested that the last phase of life had to do with coming to accepted: either only people aged 60 or over, or a
terms with integrity versus despair and that, if this is large proportion of people above 60 years of age
successfully resolved, it results in wisdom. Wisdom is the (above 40%). Excluded were studies using a second-or-
detached concern with life itself, in the face of death itself. der perspective (families or staff reporting on behalf of
It maintains and learns to convey the integrity of experi- the older person), studies related to end-of-life care
ence, in spite of the decline of bodily and mental func- programs, reporting no empirical data or no obvious
tions (18, pp. 37-8). Their research was longitudinal, and themes derived in the analysis, a large proportion of
included interviews with participants at a late stage of their younger informants or in which the proportion of peo-
lives. The participants thought about dying and death and, ple above 60 years of age could not be established, stud-
in contrast to what was stated earlier, fear of death was ies mainly related to suicide or euthanasia or older peo-
put forward. This was explained further in terms of being ple being suicidal, as well as quality of life at the end-
afraid of pain and suffering, thus referring to dying rather phase of life. Thus, in the third step, the 173 studies
than death. The theory of Eriksson et al. implies that at were evaluated in relation to the inclusion and exclusion
some point a transition may take place, emotionally criteria, resulting in 43 studies i.e., 130 studies did not
preparing older people for death, but not for dying. The meet the inclusion criteria. Lastly, scientific quality was
implication of empirical evidence for such knowledge is evaluated using the criteria of Goodman (19), focusing
above all that it would help health care staff to over- on internal and external validity, reliability, and critical
come their own fear of talking to older people about discussion. Qualitative studies were evaluated according
death and perhaps ease their own and the older persons to reliability: description of the framework, context,
fear of dying. The proportion and number of old people selecting respondents, data collection and analysis, con-
in the Western world have never been as large as they are sistency in framework, methods and analysis, as well as
now, which has implications for research. As a starting presenting excerpts that showed consistency with the
point, there is a need for a fresh overview of available re- themes (9).
search about, in particular, very old peoples view of
their death and dying. This will be even more important RESULTS
because of the changes in the proportion of older people Most studies came from the USA or Canada, and
in the population and longevity. This review therefore must thus be interpreted in the cultural context of these
aimed at a more comprehensive grasp of available em- countries. Very few studies focused solely on the oldest
pirical studies about older peoples view of death and dy- old, people aged 80 years or above, whether they were in
ing, whether in a terminal phase of life or not. the end-of-life phase or not (Tables 1-4). In some cases,
samples represented a broad age range, from very young
METHODS people to very old. The designs were mainly cross-sec-
A total of 33 publications were included, identified in tional, either quantitative or qualitative, and the pre-
a stepwise literature search of which the first step was dominant data collection method was personal inter-
the search done in Medline, CINAHL and PsychInfo us- views. Some studies dealt with both death and dying, and
ing the terms death, attitude to death, death and dy- they are reported here in accordance with where em-
ing in combination with aged over 80. Altogether, phasis was placed.
there were 715 references in these searches, of which
some occurred in all data bases (288 from Medline, Views of death in people in the end-of-life phase
153 from CINAHL, 274 from PsychInfo). In the second or not (20-35)
step, abstracts were reviewed in relation to the re- Two dominant lines of research on participants
search questions. One hundred and seventy-three papers views of death, including those not in an end-of-life
were closely reviewed for their relevance and scientific stage, were found: studies dealing with thoughts about

Aging Clin Exp Res, Vol. 16, No. 2 89


I.R. Hallberg

death and after-death, both qualitative and quantita- death anxiety was reported to be low (29, 30) and sig-
tive (20-24), and others, mainly quantitative, related nificantly lower than in younger adults (24). A meta-anal-
to death anxiety, hardiness, attitudes to hastening death, ysis of 49 studies (27) showed death anxiety to be sig-
and the will to live (25-32) (Table 1, 20-32). The level of nificantly related to ego integrity, more physical and psy-

Table 1 - Studies related to views of death in samples of older people, not at the end of life.1

Author(s) Aims Informants Design, methods Findings

Leichtentritt To describe elderly 26 elderly: Phenomenological, personal Death in and for itself was addressed as
and Rettig Israelis views of 15 women, 11 men interviews introducing the an inevitable completion of the life circle,
2000 (20) a good death. (range 60-86), selected questions: a phenomenon that has to be accepted.
Israel through advertisement, - their opinions about the con- Emotions were: anger, fear and anxiety;
chain sampling, personal cept of a good death sadness and grimness; confidence and
connections. - their end-of-life preferences security, mostly in Gods actions.
- how to distinguish between a Connections between the entire life and
desirable and an undesirable death as well as the socio-cultural traditions
death. of death.
Establishing time boundaries: making valu-
able and meaningful the last weeks of life and
the months after death.
The process: 1) Prior to physiological death:
dying at an appropriate age, maintaining
independence and self-reliance, completing
tasks, maintaining mental capacity, accept-
ing imminent death. 2) Physiological death:
natural dying vs prolonging life or promoting
death; avoiding pain and suffering; accepting
my death by others, sharing or isolating
others from the process, separating from the
world by stating ones wisdom, finding spir-
itual consolation. 3) After physiological death:
integrity of self and ritual, achieving respect,
providing continuity of heritage, promoting
a legacy, the role of religion in a Jewish
State, the consequences of deviant behavior
(suicide or being buried).
Van der Gest Views on death. 35 older people and Anthropological study - Older people looked forward to death,
2002 (21) some relatives, snowball - personal conversations. a welcome visitor that will bring peace
Ghana sampling (age not and rest after a strenuous life.
established). - Bad death is the death that terminates
life, not yet completed.
- Agnosticism prevailed you cannot tell
where a dead person goes.
- Life after death could be about reward or
punishment.
- Ancestorhood facilitates remembering.
- Reincarnation, especially the return of
children accepted.
Fry Cognition of fear and 198 elderly home-bound: Semi-structured interviews. Category 1: Physical and emotional inse-
1990 (22) concerns about 100 women, 78 men - Concerns about death and curity including factors: pain and suffering;
Canada death and dying. (mean age 70.8; range dying, fear and anxiety. fear of sensory loss; risk to personal safety.
1. Themes of death 65.5-87.3). - List and describe aspects of Category 2: Self-esteem concerns being
and dying concerns death and dying causing permanently forgotten, no-one caring, life
2. Coping, dealing concern, and specific contents has been useless, no-one attending the
with concerns. of fears and anxiety about funeral, no-one paying respect or tribute,
death and dying. Factor analysis. autopsy, indignity at the hands of under-
takers. Rejection by God, reincarnation,
punitive elements, no admission to heav-
en, timelessness in life beyond death, loud
noise everywhere.
Category 3: Uncertainty of life beyond death
(magical supernatural), vacuum beyond
death, stillness, emptiness, shadow every-
where beyond death, darkness beyond death.
Coping: Internal self-control, social support
seeking, prayer, becomes occupied with ob-
jects of attachment, avoidance, denial and
escape.

Continued

90 Aging Clin Exp Res, Vol. 16, No. 2


Older peoples view of death and dying

Table 1 - Continued

Author(s) Aims Informants Design, methods Findings

Rao et al. To assess the nature 125 randomly selected Personal interviews including 40% said they thought about dying, occa-
1997 (23) of thoughts regarding community residents: Cambridge mental disorder of sionally (66.4%) or often (10.4%), and
UK death and dying. 92 women, 33 men elderly examination, geriatric 89.6% said they were not worried about
(mean age 86.1; range depression scale, modified the prospect of dying. Thoughts about
81-96). version of scale for suicidal death were associated with depression
ideation. and low contact with informants and nega-
tively with cognitive impairment. Comments
included circumstances of dying (alone), tim-
ing of death, effects of others (joining dead
spouse, other people dying), effects on oth-
ers (burden, what happens to family), health
status, material concern (what happens to
possessions, enough money to stay alive),
current wish to die (nothing to live for).
Cicirelli The influence of 68 older adults: Instruments: The personal Death meanings different only for sub-
2001 (24) age and gender on 56 women, 12 men meaning of death scale (death scale motivator, indicating that these
USA meanings of death (mean age 82.6; as legacy, as afterlife, extinction, conceptions of death were stable over
and fears of death, range 70-97); motivator); MFODS sub-scales; age. Fear of death showed significantly
comparing young 78 younger adults: fear of the dying process, fear higher score in younger adults in all sub-
and older adults. 56 women, 22 men of being destroyed, fear for scales except being destroyed. Gender
(mean age 21.9; significant others, fear of the differences were obtained with higher
range 20-29). unknown; SES. scores for women than men in death as
extinction, fear of dying and fear of the
unknown. Death meanings of what will
happen after death (afterlife, extinction)
had the closest relationship to fear of
death; afterlife lower fear of death, extin-
ction greater fear.
Carmel How does the will to 987 persons: Personal interviews. Women have weaker will to live and less
2001 (25) die associate with the 47.4% women, Will to live from 0-5, desire to prolong life. Variables explain-
Israel wish to prolong life? 52.6% men hypothetical situations of life- ing the will to live:
(mean age 77.5; restraining treatment, psycho- For men: psychosocial indicators, age, psy-
range 70-101). somatic symptoms and health, chosomatic symptoms, living with a partner,
mood, self-esteem, life satisfac- self-esteem, fear of death.
tion, fear of death and dying, For women: life satisfaction, self-esteem,
religiosity, social support. fear of death.
Cataldo To investigate the 90 hospital residents: Personal interviews with - There was an inverse relationship
1994 (26) relationship between 46 women, 44 men self-report instruments: the between hardiness and depression.
USA hardiness, death (mean age 73.3; Zung-SDS for depression, - There was an inverse relationship
attitudes and range 65-92). health-related hardiness, death between healthy death attitudes and de-
depression. attitude profile. pression (i.e., approach-oriented death,
acceptance and neutral death acceptance).
- There was a positive relationship
between depression and escape-orien-
ted death, acceptance, and fear of
death/dying.
Fortner and Systematic review; 49 studies: 66.5% Literature search, manual and Higher levels of death anxiety are related
Neimeyer relationship between women, 33.5% men electronic up to December to lower levels of ego integrity (r=0.30),
1999 (27) death anxiety, age, (mean age 72.68; range 1996. Published and unpub- more physical problems (r=0.19), more
USA gender, ego integrity, 61-87; mean number lished papers, meta-analysis. physiological problems (r=0.28).
institutionalization, of participants 91.98; Age, gender and religiosity are not reliable
physical and psycho- range 16-293). predictors of death anxiety.
logical problems and Religious beliefs measure predicted greater
religiosity. correlation between religiosity and death
anxiety than measures that mix religious be-
lief and religious behavior. Death anxiety
proved to be higher among elderly living in
nursing homes than those living in more in-
dependent settings (independent residence or
assisted-living facilities).
Cicirelli To determine the 388 inhabitants: Interviews including measures for Significant correlations between fear
1999 (28) relationship of psycho- 285 women, 103 men fear of death (MFODS, two sub- of dying and ethnicity, gender, external
USA social and demogra- (265 whites, 123 African- scales; fear of dying, fear of the locus of control, and negatively correlated
phic variables to spe- Americans; mean age unknown); religiosity, locus of with religiosity. Fear of the unknown
cific fear of death. 72.65; SD 7.73). SES control, perceived social support, was correlated negatively with SES and
showed a social status demographic variables, analysis with external locus of control, religiosity,
slightly above that of high to test a hypothesized model and perceived social support.
school graduate or similar. (LISREL8).
Continued

Aging Clin Exp Res, Vol. 16, No. 2 91


I.R. Hallberg

Table 1 - Continued

Author(s) Aims Informants Design, methods Findings

Cicirelli Fear of dying in path analysis; externality


1999 (28) and religiosity (negative) had a direct effect
USA on fear of dying and also ethnicity, age
(negative) and gender, whilst social support
had no significant effect. Ethnicity and gen-
der had an indirect effect through religiosity.
Fear of the unknown: external locus of con-
trol, religiosity (negative), and social sup-
port (negative) had a direct effect on fear.
Ethnicity had an indirect effect through re-
ligiosity; age through externality and social
support; gender through religiosity and so-
cial support; SES through social support
and externality.
Wu et al. Cognitive and emotio- 237 respondents: Personal interviews including Low death anxiety score (3.54, possible 15)
2002 (29) nal reactions to death 175 women, 62 men death anxiety scale, general most fear painful death, getting cancer,
Hong Kong in Chinese (Hong (mean age 73.9; SD 6.8; health questionnaire, physical thoughts about shortness of life, nervous
Kong) elderly. range 60-91). disorders, recent stressors, reli- when others spoke about death, and
gious affiliation, demographics, horrified by the sight of a dead body.
income. No gender differences, negative relation to
age.
Multiple regression analysis showed age,
recent stressors and psychological distress to
predict death anxiety, the last with the
strongest explanatory value.
Sullivan et al. Older Dutch peoples Functionally impaired Personal interviews: Think a lot Preoccupation with death, fear of death
1998 (30) view on death, dying community-dwelling of death? Afraid of death? Fears stable and low over time. Fear of death
Netherlands and hastened death elders: of the dying process (6 items); and dying related to poor health, especially
and in relation to 72% women, 28% men beliefs about hastening death (4 mental. Preoccupation with death predicted
health and (n=643-1994; n=575- items); health status; religious by HADS, Calvinist membership, support
demographics. 1995; mean age 73). beliefs; HADS-hospital, anxiety from religion, education, neuroticism. Fear
depression scale; Eysenck pers. of death predicted by neuroticism, low men-
scale; life satisfaction. tal health and lower age. Fear of the dying
process predicted by HADS-score, neu-
roticism, number of chronic medical condi-
tions, negatively correlated with Calvinist
membership. Hastened death explained by
low religious belief, not Calvinist member-
ship or HADS-score.
Cicirelli Older peoples views Older white and African- Personal interviews, open and Fear of dying was predicted by gender
2002 (31) on death and influence Americans. Varied SES: with standardized measures; de- (women), health (poor), viewing death as
USA of fear of death, mean- 42 white high SES; 26 mographic, SES, fear of death, extinction (neg) and death as a motivator
ings of death, dying low SES; 41 Afr-Am. personal meanings of death, for further accomplishment (neg).
process, family relat- (mean age 84, 80.5, dying process, religious behavior Fear of being destroyed was predicted by
ion, health, religion 77.5; age range 70-97). and feelings, health, morale, low SES, number of dead siblings (neg)
and SES.2 family relations. and deceased children (neg).
Only results from regression Fear for significant others was predicted by
analysis are reported. age (low), number of dead siblings (neg)
and children (neg), and death as motivator
(neg) and as extinction (neg).
Fear of the unknown was predicted by re-
ligiosity (less fear), death as afterlife (less
fear), and death as legacy (more fear).
Williams Attitudes to death 60 Aberdonians Personal interviews and question- Four coherent themes, although some
1990 (32) and illness.3 (70 years +). naires about views of probable fell between more than one theme:
Scotland death. Ritual dying: readiness for death, reunion
with those close before death, fully aware,
affirmation of sanctity of life.
Disregarded dying: natural, quick death,
dying unaware, excluding euthanasia.
Transitional pattern: lifelong preparedness,
quick death, minimize awareness, excluding
euthanasia.
Controlled dying: control over time of death,
full knowledge of ones fate, quick death, re-
union before death.
1Some of the studies cover both death and dying, but are presented here because of their emphasis on the importance of after-death. 2This is a very comprehensive
study and report, with in-depth discussion of views on death; thus, only the results of regression analysis are reported. 3This study is reported extensively in a book
and only the findings related to dying are reported here.

92 Aging Clin Exp Res, Vol. 16, No. 2


Older peoples view of death and dying

chological problems, whilst the other variables did not anxiety (27, 31). Similar findings were reported in oth-
predict death anxiety. There was, however, a significant er studies as well; psychological problems, neuroticism
tendency for strong religious beliefs to mean low death (25, 26, 29-31), depression (23, 26, 30), external locus

Table 2 - Studies related to views of death in samples of older people at the end of life.

Author(s) Aims Informants Design, methods Findings

Hinton Awareness and Hospice cancer patients, Semi-structured interviews weekly Awareness The proportion of older people
1999 (33) acceptance of n=76: 34 women, during the last 8 weeks of life. who were certain that they were dying stayed
UK dying during the final 42 men (median age 65; Ratings made on a linear analog almost the same (38-48%), whilst 50%
8 weeks of life. range 33-84). scale for awareness, acceptance, thought it was probable or possible. Only a
Relatives1. mood, anxiety, consciousness. few expected recovery. Age and gender did
not correlate with awareness. Social class,
severity, weakness correlated to awareness.
Awareness and anxiety had a weak correla-
tion, but not depression.
Acceptance increased over time from 38 to
52%. Being troubled by the prospects of
dying fell from 10 to 2-3% in the last 6
weeks. Acceptance was not related to age,
women were more accepting than men.
Social class and weakness had no relation to
acceptance, whilst impaired mental state had.
Patterns of acceptance included: Factual,
death is inevitable; Religious faith, peace, re-
union; Life no longer rewarding time to go;
Completing tasks preparing for death;
Life complete had a good life; Final gains
making most of remaining life; Humour;
Facing situation together; Experience of
others dying.
Langley-Evans Communication 14 patients: 7 women, Ethnography: 7 weeks partici- Death talk talk concerning illness, symp-
and Payne among patients with 7 men (mean age 59; pant observations as an active toms and treatment, stories about illness
1999 (34) terminal disease, range 36-82). member. and death, talk about other patients deaths,
UK open awareness. talk about bereavement, talk concerning
personal mortality.
Form of death talk Mainly light-hearted
jovial, sometimes serious (bereavement), hu-
mor.
Yedidia and Dominant themes in 30 patients: 22 women, Serial semi-structured interviews All acknowledged that their life was coming
MacGregor patients views of 8 men, (range 40-99; (4.2/patient)-ethnographic: to an end. Some desired death, others were
2001 (35) death. 44% above 70 years of - Illness, resistant, and others neutral. Seven motifs
USA age) in hospice home care - Awareness of dying, of participants view on the prospects of
mainly. - Prospects of dying, their death were:
- Process of dying. Struggle motif (Living and dying is a strug-
gle), which seemed linked to their entire life;
Dissonance motif (Dying is not living), felt
positive about their past, which contrasted
with the present; distress;
Endurance motif (Triumph of inner
strength), dealt with difficulties by taking
control, a product of their character;
Incorporation motif (Belief system accom-
modates death), death understandable with-
in their belief system, secular or spiritual;
Coping motif (Working to find a new bal-
ance), instrumental outlook, worked to find
new sources of strength to re-establish equi-
librium;
Quest motif (Seeking meaning in dying), re-
flected and worked to find meaning in their
experiences, life presented itself as even
more meaningful, new opportunities, new
perspectives to be examined;
Volatile motif (Unresolved and unresigned),
contained unresolved issues; patients were
more victims of forces they could not control,
lacked clarity of direction.
1Results not reported here.

Aging Clin Exp Res, Vol. 16, No. 2 93


I.R. Hallberg

of control (28), and low will to live (25) were related to or after-death as a punishment or reward, but also with
death anxiety (or fear), whereas the reverse was true for the fear of extinction, thinking about autopsy, vacuum, be-
hardiness (26) and self-esteem (25). Thus, aspects related ing treated with dignity after death, preserved integrity,
to a well-integrated personality seemed to play a pro- treating ones belongings with respect, and the importance
tective role, whilst psychosocial problems meant more of feeling that one belonged, the continuity between
death anxiety. There were no obvious gender differences generations (20, 21, 23, 24, 31).
in any study except one (31). Depression (23, 26, 30) Only a few studies dealt with death in people at the end
obviously highly influenced death anxiety, indicating of life (33-35) and samples included a wide range with re-
the importance of detecting depression in older people. gard to age (Table 2, 33-35). In contrast with studies re-
Also, religious beliefs or other belief systems were found lated to older people, not in the end-of-life phase, these
to relate to low anxiety (27, 28, 30, 31) but not religious studies dealt mainly with the acceptance of, and the ap-
affiliation (29). Social support was found in some stud- proach to death (33-35). Awareness of closeness to
ies (22, 25, 28) to relate to low death anxiety or the loss death differed in one study in which 40-50% were certain
of close family members (31); in other studies it was not (33) and awareness was shown indirectly as small talk (34),
included. Attitudes to death varied: wanting it to be whereas in another study all acknowledged that their
quick or not, and wanting to be aware or not (32). lives were coming to an end (35). Death talk in commu-
Few studies dealt with how much older people actually nication between patients in a palliative unit indicated
think about death, and those which did, showed either open awareness, i.e., death was talked about as part of
that they did so occasionally or often (23), or that pre- their social life (34). Awareness showed an association
occupation with death was stable and low (30). The will mainly with health status, but not with depression (33).
to live was found to be less pronounced in women, Patterns of acceptance resembled findings from the stud-
who were less in favor of prolonging life (25). ies presented in Table 1, and included approaching death
Although the studies dealing with thoughts about as a fact, life no longer rewarding, preparing for death by
death and after-death were different and therefore difficult completing tasks, looking back at a good life, and facing
to present in a coherent manner, some common or sim- the situation together with those close by (33). Analysis of
ilar themes were identified (Table 1, 20-24). This was the ways of approaching death was found to relate to previ-
view of death as such, with an accepting attitude, as ous life strategies and could mean approaching death be-
something unavoidable, a completion of the life circle, but ing distressed or seemingly not distressed. These motifs
it could also evoke very different emotions: anger, sadness were living and dying as a struggle, distress in that dying
or confidence (20), not worrying (23) or looking for- was not living, taking control, instrumental outlook,
ward to death, as in a study from Ghana (21). Another meanings found, or unresolved issues that made death and
theme that stood out was the approach to the time span, dying chaotic (35).
connecting the past, present and future, including after-
death (20-23). Death was spoken about in relation to so- Views of dying in people in the end-of-life phase
cial circumstances, relationships having negative as well as or not (36-52)
positive connotations (20, 23) for instance, being a bur- Like studies about death, in those about dying in old-
den, dying alone, worries about what would happen to er people, not in the end-of-life phase, some common
survivors, no-one coming to the funeral, or joining the themes were identified, although they took on different
dead spouse, and providing continuity of heritage. The life meanings for different people (Tables 3-4, 36-52). These
span perspective, looking back, emerged in some studies had to do with control, directly or indirectly spoken of (36-
(20-23) and was spoken about in terms of connecting, 41), who should take decisions when the time comes (38-
completing, continuity, or establishing time boundaries, 44), awareness (37), being prepared (36, 37, 42, 43), dig-
which had to do with the entire life but also stretched in- nity or being treated as a whole person (42-44), achiev-
to the future, that is, after death, and thus took on a more ing a sense of completion (42, 43), family (41, 42, 44),
existential meaning, connecting generations to each oth- and the circumstances under which end-of-life care was
er (20-23). The circumstances under which death oc- provided (40, 41), especially symptom management (37,
curred had to do with whether it was at an appropriate 40, 43) and not prolonging life (39-41), outcome of
time, with maintained independence, quick or over a treatment should be the end-point, not treatment per se
period of time, and both could be favored (20, 21, 23, (41), and also wanting to care for themselves or being in-
32). Poor health and having lost those close by drove peo- volved in other peoples lives (41, 42).
ple towards death (20, 23, 31). Making the most of the There were variations with regard to older peoples at-
last period of life was emphasized (20). Thoughts about af- titude to advanced care planning, being aware or not, and
ter-death had to do with religious beliefs, being admitted a quick versus prolonged death, which seemingly had to
to heaven or not, silence or loud noise everywhere, do with completing life and whether to take control over
doubts of an afterlife existence, reunion with loved ones decisions oneself or delegate them to others, as well as

94 Aging Clin Exp Res, Vol. 16, No. 2


Older peoples view of death and dying

Table 3 - Studies related to views of dying, the end-of-life phase, in samples of older people not in an end-of-life phase.

Author(s) Aims Informants Design, methods Findings

Carrese et al. Elderly patients view 20 chronically ill: Ethnographic study. Personal in- Respondents spoke about future differently:
2002 (36) on advanced care 16 women, 4 men, terviews regarding future illness - Uncertain future, which they were un-
USA planning. (mean age 86). and end-of-life care. Thoughts willing to plan for or even contemplate.
about it? Planned for? How should - A time when death is near and certain
others know what to do for you? more likely to plan for and discuss.
How do you think about these - The event of death itself for which they are
issues? likely to have made arrangements.
Themes: Considering the future, contin-
gencies of serious future illness, living in the
present, final arrangements for death, ad-
vance directives, views of the future.
Vig et al. Attitudes of older 16 people with non- Personal interviews: What are Current life values cannot be extended to
2002 (37) adults about end of life. terminal heart disease or the most important things in your end-of-life preferences. Aspects contribut-
USA Can current values be cancer: 2 women, 14 men life right now? What would you ing to good or bad death: suffering (inclu-
extended to end-of-life (range 60-84). consider a good or bad death? ding pain, cancer, feeling messy); quick vs
preferences? Questions of health status, prolonged death, aware vs unaware, pre-
symptoms, QOL. pared vs unprepared, alone vs families/
friends, control vs dependency, hope vs
anger, savings.
Kelner Attitude to exert 38 hospitalized patients: Personal interviews. Thoughts of Two approaches: activists (n=27) and dele-
1995 (38) control over end-of-life 25 women, 13 men control over end-of-life decisions. gators (n=11).
Canada decisions. (range 65-85+). Views on control over decision- Activists had higher socioeconomic status,
making. Preferences when time were middle class, rejected the idea of eu-
comes. View on withdrawal of thanasia and assisted suicide, favored with-
treatment, euthanasia, holding and withdrawing treatment, wanted
physician-assisted suicide. to obtain control, decided for themselves
when QoL was too low, but also when to go
on living.
Delegators delegated decisions to their physi-
cians, God or fate, more often married.
Cicirelli Views on various end- 447 healthy elders: Structured interview; 17 decision Three factors: Maintain (continue to live,
1997 (39) of-life decisions. 330 women, 117 men scenarios with 7 options each. not withholding treatment); Others (some-
USA (mean age 72.1; range - Personality, one else makes end-of-life decisions); End life
60-100; 292 whites, - Death anxiety, (suicide, voluntary euthanasia).
155 blacks). - Life values, Eight sub-groups, of which four cover
- Locus of control, 59% of the sample:
- Self-esteem, A. Little desire to prolong life, little de-
- Depression, sire to let others make decisions, do not
- Life satisfaction, actively wish to end their lives.
- Loneliness, B. Actively wish to end their lives, not to
- Life event stress, prolong them; others should not make
- Social support. decisions.
C. No desire to prolong life and no desire to
actively end life, would allow others to make
decisions.
E. Want to prolong life, no wish to actively
end life, not letting others make decisions.
Group C older than A, B and E; groups A,
B had greater fear of death than E. Self-es-
teem higher for group E than C.
Wilson Preferences of know- 49 seniors: (range 60-85 Personal interviews: Two concepts identified:
2000 (40) ledgeable senior citi- years old), all having past - Please tell me about your expe- 1. Dependency while dying: Quick death
Canada zens about end-of-life experience of providing rience with death or dying. saves others from the burden. Availability
care. end-of-life care. - What do you think is the best of others determines where to die as well
Gender not stated. place and type of care for senior as whether living alone or not.
citizens during the last days of 2. Appropriate end-of-life care: Home is
their life? the optimal place to receive end-of-life care,
otherwise equally distributed between acute
hospital and long-term care facilities. Care by
people who know and care for the person;
Allow death to occur; Care that meets needs;
No treatment to extend life; In control of de-
cision-making.
Rosenfeld et al. To identify the desired 21 informants with prior Personal interviews focusing on Three major themes:
2000 (41) features of end-of-life. experience of end-of-life personal narratives of serious - Factors patients and families weigh in
USA decision-making: illness, with probing of how deci- treatment decisions;
81% women, 19% men. sions were made and by whom, - The manner in assigning decision-mak-
All had completed advance clinical scenarios including illness, ing authority;

Continued

Aging Clin Exp Res, Vol. 16, No. 2 95


I.R. Hallberg

Table 3 - Continued

Author(s) Aims Informants Design, methods Findings

Rosenfeld et al. directives (mean age 83; interventions and prognoses. - The role of caring behavior in end-of-life de-
2000 (41) range 72-92), mainly Content analysis. cision-making.
USA white. Outcome, not treatment, guides treatment
preferences; valued life. Advanced age is
relevant in treatment considerations; per-
sonal issues, natural life span.
Decision-making authority: given to both
physicians and families based on their re-
spective areas of expertise.
When functional recovery is not possible
precedence to families; caring during serious
illness; not burden to loved ones; desire to
provide care for a loved one.
Steinhauser et al. Descriptions of good Elderly, staff, chaplains, Grounded theory. Focus groups - Pain and symptom management.
2000 (42) death. volunteers, family mem- and personal interviews, 3 - Clear decision-making, empowering
USA bers1, 14 patients, in all patients groups, 1 group of patients and involving them in decision
75 participants, 4 bereaved bereaved family members. -making.
family members. Discuss their experience of death - Preparation for death; knowing what is
64% women, 36% men of family members what made to come.
(age of entire sample those deaths good or bad. - Completion: meaningfulness, spirituality,
26-77). life review, resolving conflicts, saying good-
bye.
- Contributing to others.
- Affirmation of the whole person.
Steinhauser et al. Factors considered at 340 seriously ill: Mailed survey using 44 attributes Considered important by more than 70%
2000 (43) end of life by patients, 78.2% men (mean age of quality of end-of-life based on of entire sample were 26 items related to:
USA families, physicians 68 years). findings from Steinhauser et al. pain and symptom management, prepa-
and other care provi- (42). ration for death, achieving a sense of com-
ders.2 pletion, decisions about treatment prefer-
ences, being treated as a whole person.
Patients emphasized being mentally aware,
having funeral arrangements planned, not
being a burden, helping others, and coming
to peace with God more than physicians.
Leichtentritt and To reveal values that 19 elderly, snowball Hypothetical scenarios: withold- Four life domains: Physical-biological; Social-
Rettig would have attention technique:11 women, ing treatment, active euthanasia, psychological; Familial; Societal with cen-
2001 (44) when considering 8 men3 (range 65+). physician-assisted death. tral values: competence, integrity, loyalty,
Israel end-of-life decisions. legacy. Transcendent values: dignity, quality
of life, quality of death.
1Reporting only patients perspectives. 2Only patients perspectives are reported. 3Also 28 family members.

views of various end-of-life decisions (Table 3, 36-44). Re- though various foci were applied, some areas stood out
spondents did not necessarily share the same view of how (Table 4, 45-52): physical problems, social network (pos-
they wished the end-of-life phase to unfold or to be han- itive), and depression or emotional distress having a neg-
dled. The most uniform directions were those stating no ative impact on this last phase of life. Fractured dignity
life prolongation, being respected, the importance of in- turned out to be rather uncommon and was predicted by
tegrity, being approached as a whole person, living one being a burden to others, dependent, in pain, and
day at a time, having the opportunity to be involved changed in appearance (45). Quality of life over the last
with others, caring for others, and the balance between year of life showed no overall deterioration over time, but
having dear ones close by but not being a burden to improvement in the physical domain and decrease in
them. Patients put more emphasis than physicians do on the existential domain. Overall, the lowest score was on
preparation for death, coming to peace with God, and not the physical domain (46). Desire for death was low, but
being a burden, which indicates that some aspects may be closely related to depression (47). Apart from depression,
overlooked in the health care system (43). As in those pain and low level of family support explained the desire
studies related to death, completion was also brought for death (47). Desire to hasten death (48, 49) was also
up in relation to dying, and symptom control was brought closely related to clinical depression, severity of depressive
up in studies related to death. symptoms, hopelessness and low social support, and
Studies about dying, including people in the end-of-life physical functioning contributed to this desire to hasten
phase, had samples with a broad age range, making death (52). The wish to hasten death was prevalent in 14-
conclusions specific to older people hard to establish. Al- 17% (48, 49) and closely related to physical symptoms,

96 Aging Clin Exp Res, Vol. 16, No. 2


Older peoples view of death and dying

Table 4 - Studies related to view of dying and end-of-life phase in older people at the end of life.

Author(s) Aims Informants Design, methods Findings

Chochinov et al. To identify to what 213 terminally ill patients Sense of dignity 0-7; symptom 7.5% reported loss of dignity as of great
2002 (45) extent dying people with cancer: 55% women, distress, 13 items; McGill painconcern, 54% reported no loss of dignity,
Canada maintain their sense 45% men (mean age 69; questionnaire; ADL capacity; and 30% minimum loss of dignity.
of integrity, and how SD 12.6). QoL measure; emotional state, Those with a fractured sense of dignity
disease-specific and desire for death, anxiety, hope-
were younger, had a stronger desire for
demographic variables lessness, will to live, burden to
death, bowel concerns, appearance,
relate to dignity. others, social support. anxiety, depression, hopelessness, lower
Comparing those with and will to live, a sense of burden, low QoL,
without intact sense of dignity.
and low life satisfaction after controlling for
age. Multiple logistic regression showed ap-
pearance, burden to others, requiring as-
sistance with bathing, pain intensity, location
of care (i.e., inpatient) to predict a fractured
sense of integrity.
Lo et al. Evaluation of QoL 58 patients: 22 women, Personal interviews. McGill QoL Mean total QoL score 7, lowest on phys-
2002 (46) during last years of life. 36 men (mean age 60; scale two weeks before death and ical domain 5.9 (from 4.8-7.2 on different
Hong Kong SD 15; range 17-86). on admission, 0-10 scale (10 sub-domains); existential 6.0 (5.4-6.1 on
positive). sub-domains), support 6.6 (6.5-6.8 on sub-
domains), psychological 7.8 (6.9-8.8 on
sub-domains), sexuality 8.4. Over time,
mean total QoL score remained the same,
with a trend toward improvement in the
physical domain, deterioration in the exis-
tential domain; others remained the same.
Chochinov et al. Desire for death in the 200 terminally ill: Semi-structured interviews incl. Desire for death above 4 (moderate) was
1995 (47) terminally ill, and rela- 103 women, 97 men SADS (Schedule for Affective present in 8.5%, 55% reported no desire,
Canada tion to psychiatric (mean age 70.9; SD 10.6; Disorders & Schizophrenia); Do 30% slight desire, and 6% mild desire.
disorders. range 31-94). you ever wish that illness would No demographic differences or in religious
progress more rapidly so that affiliation. Strong association with
your suffering could be over depression in those with a strong wish to die
sooner?; Beck depression scale; (58.8 vs 7.7%) compared with those with
Social support; Pain intensity; no desire or fluctuating wish to die.
Karnofsky scale. The wish to die was related to pain, not to
functional ability or social contacts but to
the level of satisfaction, support also showed
a weak correlation.
Three main variables explained the desire for
death: depression, pain, and low level of
family support.
Kelly et al. To identify factors 72 hospice and home Semi-structured interviews inclu- No wish to hasten death in 59%, moderate
2002 (48) associated with the palliative care patients: ding a modified wish-to-hasten- in 27%, and high in 14%. Central themes
Australia wish to hasten death. 37 women, 35 men death scale, and 8 questions were negative or positive experiences of
(mean age 61-71; about illness experience, caring health care, demoralization as an impact
SD 13.9). experiences, euthanasia, life on patients, support vs burden as impact
review. on others, and positive reflections on life
as a whole. Those with a high wish to hasten
death had greater concerns with physical
symptoms, psychological suffering, saw them-
selves more as a burden to others, higher lev-
els of demoralization, and less confidence in
symptom control, less social support, less life
satisfaction and fewer religious beliefs com-
pared with the other two groups.
Breitbart et al. Prevalence of desire 92 terminally ill cancer Interviews including SAHD 17% reported a high desire for hastened
2000 (49) to hasten death and patients: mean age 65.9; (Structured clinical interview for death and 16% major depressive symp-
USA correlation to this SD 15.6; range )50 (20%): DSM-IV), Hamilton depression toms. Hastened death desire was associated
desire. to above 85 years of age rating scale, Becks depression with clinical depression and depressive
(9%), above 65 years scale, social support, spiritual symptom severity as well as hopelessness.
(56%). well-being, pain, memorial sym- Social support and physical functioning
ptoms scale, Karnofsky scale, contributed slightly to the wish to hasten
McGill QoL. death, whilst depression and hopelessness
had the strongest explanatory value.
Engle et al. To describe and com- 13 nursing home residents: Ethnographic, personal interviews 1. Dying was reflected in: thinking of
1998 (50) pare experiences, 6 women, 7 men (mean (1-4 times): How have things dying, having little or no fear of dying;
USA needs, priorities and age 77; range 54-91; been for you?, What would finding comfort in religious faith.
concerns during living- 8 black, 5 white). make things better?, What Afterlife envisioned as a better place.
dying interval. comforts you?, What does 2. Pain; black people consistently
dying mean to you?, Do reported moderate to severe pain, whilst
you ever think of passing on? white rarely reported pain.

Continued

Aging Clin Exp Res, Vol. 16, No. 2 97


I.R. Hallberg

Table 4 - Continued

Author(s) Aims Informants Design, methods Findings

Engle et al. 3. Nutrition incl. no appetite, swallowing


1998 (50) difficulties, no pleasure in eating, supple-
USA ments or soft food, fear of tube feeding.
4. Religion: trusting loving God, definite
sense of right and wrong, Golden Rule, im-
portance of attending church, little or no
fear of dying.
5. Care giving: the need to be of service to
others, adopting residents to care for, iden-
tifying others worse off.
6. Care receiving: staff doing extra activities,
using bantering to promote changes in be-
havior, responding slowly to requests, unable
to understand physicians.
Coping; anger, withdrawal, direct informa-
tion, humor, talking with staff. Residents
focused on quality of living rather than on
dying.
McKinlay Patients experience 6 hospice patients: Personal interviews:Whats your The circle of care: a pattern of lived exis-
2001 (51) of hospice care. 3 women, 3 men experience of care at the hospice, tence. Identity was central, i.e., being a
New Zealand (range 58-79). what service have you been person to themselves and to others.
At least one prior hospice involved in? Eight themes were generated: keeping
inpatient period. control; chosen isolation; being safe; relin-
quishment and relaxation; mortality aware-
ness. Patients interpretation of staffs actions
manifested in being watched, caring qualities,
humor. Environment and philosophy of care
also surrounded these themes.
Singer et al. To identify and 3 patient groups; Qualitative interviews, content Domains extracted from analysis in long-
1999 (52) describe quality end- Long-term residence analysis, interview guides differ term care. Receiving adequate pain and
Canada of-life care. (mean age 73.6; range for each group mainly focusing symptom management (39.5%), avoiding
65-85). Male: 13% groups on control of decision-making, inappropriate prolongation of dying (65.8%),
of dialysis and HIV not personal preferences, views of achieving a sense of control (47.4%), relie-
included. withdrawal or termination of ving burden (34.2%), strengthening rela-
treatment. tionships with loved ones (31.6%).

psychological suffering (including demoralization, low DISCUSSION


trust in symptom control, low life satisfaction), experi- This review of the literature confirms views from ear-
encing oneself as a burden to others, including less social lier research (7, 53) that there are few studies focusing
support and fewer religious beliefs, than those not want- solely on older people and their view of death and dying.
ing to hasten death (48). Such studies were often muddled by samples with a wide
As in the studies listed in Tables 1-2 presenting aspects age span, especially when including people at the end of
related to death, aspects related to dying also emphasized life, and it was not always possible to detect the views of
integrity, being regarded as a person, relationships which older people as opposed to younger ones. Few studies
meant a balance between being close and being a burden, had samples with very old people only, or separated the
control, and symptom management, as well as thoughts very old from the young old. As people live longer, it
about the afterlife. Fear in the end-of-life phase was re- seems even more important to recognize the fact that the
ported to be rare, comfort was found in religious faith, and time span from retirement to the end of life may be
the afterlife was envisioned as a better place (50). Reac- long, thirty to forty years, and that during these years
tions varied from anger, withdrawal and humor, to talking there are periods marked by enjoying life, emerging dis-
with staff about their situation (50). Preserved integrity eases and, later on, decreased functional ability, and dy-
(51), being in control (51, 52, 49), relationships with those ing and death (54). The fact that old age is made up of dif-
close by (50, 52), but also relieving them of being a bur- ferent phases that need to be addressed in research has
den (52), chosen isolation (51), being involved oneself in been recognized before (55) and is likely to be valid also
caring for others and feeling cared for (50) were all men- for research on older peoples death and dying. Also, very
tioned. Symptom management was also important, es- few studies seem to have been performed with samples
pecially pain and nutritional problems (50, 52), being safe from the European countries which is noteworthy,
(51) and the care atmosphere (51). Inappropriate pro- since there are very probably great cultural as well as his-
longation of life should be avoided (52). torical differences between North America and Europe

98 Aging Clin Exp Res, Vol. 16, No. 2


Older peoples view of death and dying

which may influence older peoples view of their death sume that peoples view of death affects the existential and
and dying. Also, within Europe one may expect great dif- psychosocial part of dying. Very few studies dealt with old-
ferences, because of the different culture, history and er peoples view of death from an emic (indigenous in-
political systems of the various countries. Thus, because terpretation) perspective. Most of them dealt with topics
of the lack of studies dealing with older peoples view of such as death anxiety, fear, or hastening of death, which
death and dying, heterogeneity as regards age in these is different from investigating older peoples view of
studies, and heterogeneity with regard to research ques- death. There were, however, some intriguing findings in
tions and samples, the findings of this review may serve the few studies, mainly regarding narrations about death.
mainly as inspiration for further research, although some These represented positive as well as negative connota-
findings are thought-provoking and may serve to inspire tions of death and after-death, not necessarily tied to
clinical practice as well. religious beliefs. For instance, sometimes the afterlife
It is noteworthy that so few studies considering the old- was spoken about in terms of punishment or reward, con-
er persons own account of death and dying seem to have tinuity of heritage, ancestorhood facilitating being re-
been performed, although gerontology as well as geriatric membered or being forgotten, being abandoned at the fu-
research has developed rapidly over the last century. neral (no-one coming), reunion with loved ones or vacu-
Bauman (56) suggested that death is a problem for the um, stillness, darkness or extinction. These studies demon-
post-modern society because its underlying promise is the strated in a sense what Bauman (56) called striving for
opposite, the promise of the indivisible sovereignty of rea- immortality, which may take on different guises, perhaps
son. He stated that it is difficult for human beings to related to faith and culture. It had, however, both negative
understand death, since it represents the ultimate vacuum; and positive aspects which probably have implications for
we cannot think beyond this, since thinking is part of dying. Cicirelli (24) found a significant relationship between
imagining and death becomes a secret, which we protect fear of dying and the meaning of death; those holding
with silence. Because we push death away from our- views of extinction were more fearful than those not. Per-
selves, we create a taboo around talking about it. Could it haps an even stronger research focus on views on death
be that it is still taboo to talk about death, or that research with carefully selected samples with regard to age and dis-
related to aging and older people is too preoccupied tance to death may reveal new aspects enhancing the cur-
with an everlasting life, thereby denying death and dying rent knowledge base as well as practice.
as part of life? The results of various studies are quite con-
sistent, in that old people do not mind talking about Completion of life a life span perspective
their forthcoming death and dying, although this is not Although the results of this review present very dis-
without pain. However, the review indicated that older parate findings, mainly because of different research
people also wanted to be alive until the last minute of questions, methods, samples and design, some themes re-
their lives, suggesting that there is a delicate balance be- lated to death were present across the studies: these
tween recognizing approaching death and recognizing as- were in particular emphasis on completion of the life cy-
pects that bring about the feeling of still being alive. cle and the life span perspective when facing imminent
Kaufmann (57) stated that end-of-life care is still not ca- death, and this stretched beyond death. This was espe-
pable of balancing the problem of foreseeing when death cially present in studies on death but also in those related
is approaching and providing high-quality end-of-life to dying. Completion of the life cycle as well as a life span
care, recognizing approaching death rather than pro- perspective was mainly positive, comforting in the face of
longing life. Researchers from the fields of gerontology as death, but also connecting generations and as part of a
well as geriatrics and geriatric nursing need to take up the higher meaning in their lives. In some studies it was also
challenge and enter this field of research. It would be es- put forward in negative terms, as having lived a worthless
pecially helpful if aspects of death and dying were included life. These findings are in line with the theory about ego
in ongoing longitudinal studies on aging and thereby integrity proposed by Erikson et al. (18), suggesting that
overcome the problem of understanding whether changes the last phase of life has to do with the struggle between
in our views of death take place as time goes by, which despair and integrity and results in wisdom, and also in re-
cross-sectional studies cannot answer. solving earlier tasks or demands. Interestingly, this was al-
so supported in a meta-analysis (27), showing that ego in-
Studies related to death versus dying tegrity was strongly related to low death anxiety, and in a
There was no distinct boundary between studies about study about the motifs of participants views on the
death and those about dying, which may be understand- prospect of their death (35). The life span perspective
able from the perspective that dying precedes death. seems to be connected to the concept of completion, and
Yet this is problematic, in that older peoples precon- has to do with death being faced in the light of the entire
ceptions of death remain mainly unknown and become life and how life has evolved. This resembles the concept
hidden in problems related to dying. It is reasonable to as- of life review put forward by Butler, who suggested that

Aging Clin Exp Res, Vol. 16, No. 2 99


I.R. Hallberg

older persons review their lives in the face of approach- to those close by but also wanting them close by, was ev-
ing death. However, although the universality of this ident in several studies. It seems difficult to verify whether
concept has been questioned (58) and investigated in there is a true wish to be alone in the dying process or just
older people, no strong evidence has been found for it. to avoid burdening loved ones. Interestingly, there also
The life span perspective when approaching death was, appeared to be a strong desire, even at the end of life, to
however, also different from the concept of life review, in be useful to other people and thus have a meaningful life
that it was a spontaneous evaluation of life that brought in spite of approaching death. This wish for involve-
about a feeling that life was completed and had been ment in other peoples well-being as well as other peoples
worthwhile or the opposite. Completion of life was put involvement in the dying persons life highlights the the-
forward in various ways but with a common core of ories of Disengagement and Objective Self-Awareness. As
meaning; connections, completion of the life circle, a wel- in the study by Baugher et al. (61), the studies included
come visitor that will bring peace and rest to a strenuous here gave no firm evidence of the idea of disengaging one-
life, completing tasks, peace, reunion, and seeking self from others or coping with negative self-awareness by
meaning in dying. The importance of entering into a avoiding others, although this may be so in some cases.
phase of completing life has been put forward as an However, the handling of, perhaps, guilt, or the shame of
end-of-life task by Byock (59). He suggested that devel- being a burden on loved ones may play a role in this
opmental landmarks and tasks for the end of life were to process and needs further research. Feelings of being a
achieve a sense of completion of, for instance, worldly af- burden may also be related to the seemingly fearful ex-
fairs, relationships with the community, and relation- perience of dependency that was prevalent in studies
ships with family and friends. Completion thus involves about dying.
evaluation of life to achieve the feeling that it has been
completed, i.e., a life span perspective and life review. Death anxiety and fear of death and dying
However, not all old people review their life positively. Death anxiety, fear of death and dying, a wish to live,
berg (60) suggested that old age is closely intertwined or a wish to hasten death and its antecedents were com-
with previous life and that this stretches into the future, to mon objectives of research mainly done using stan-
death. From a thorough study of Finns he presented a list dardized methods, but unfortunately of different kinds. Per-
of six ways of life, of which some were negative: the bit- haps the strongest evidence came from the meta-analysis
ter life, and life as a pitfall. This was also shown in the (27), although that study investigated only some of the
study about participants views of the prospects of their possible antecedents of death anxiety. Studies about
death (35), in which at least the volatile and dissonance death anxiety in younger and older people showed indi-
motifs seemed painful. It seems reasonable to believe cations that death anxiety decreases at higher ages al-
that death and dying are experienced in relation to life as though, due to the study design, it was not possible to sep-
a whole, and thus knowledge about the life span may be arate these findings from cohort effects. There were no
useful for completing life with less fear of death and after- firm indications that death anxiety was common in older
death, thus supporting the idea that there are some end- people; on the contrary, most studies showed a rather low
of-life tasks to complete. The meaning of these end-of-life death anxiety or fear of death. It is noteworthy that Ci-
tasks perhaps needs some deeper exploration to under- cirelli (31) reported a peak in death anxiety in the age
stand the mechanisms and how to achieve completion. range 80-84, indicating that there is a transition, perhaps
along the lines of the theory of Erikson et al. (18).
Separating from those close by Death/dying anxiety or fear of death/dying appears in a
Another theme that pervaded the results of the studies complex interaction, especially of aspects most probably
was the impact on, and of the closest relatives, especial- related to factors such as personality aspects (hardiness,
ly spouses, children and grandchildren. Studies about ego integrity, low neuroticism, external locus of con-
death were preferably related to themes like reunion after trol), social network, physical problems in the end-of-
death and continuity, as well as the lack of people who life phase, age, and beliefs, including religious beliefs. Most
care, as discussed previously. Also, losing ones coevals re- of the studies did not satisfactorily include all these aspects,
duced the reasons for staying on, i.e., it was not worth go- so that the relative impact of each of them cannot be es-
ing on living. However, studies about dying highlighted the tablished. A causal model was tested in a sample of hos-
disparity in results that characterized several studies, pice patients at various ages (30 to 89), although only a
even when research questions were similar. There were few variables of importance were included (age, gender,
two main positions in relation to close relatives: wanting length of illness, physical functions) (62). Psychological
them close by, including saying goodbye, which also had adaptation proved to be directly related to social support
to do with timing of death, or not wanting them nearby. and low pain. More research is needed to take these
Another common finding complicated this, that of being aspects into consideration as well as ego integrity and per-
a burden to others. The fear of being a burden, especially sonality, which had strong support for their importance,

100 Aging Clin Exp Res, Vol. 16, No. 2


Older peoples view of death and dying

not only in quantitative but also qualitative studies, and al- be related, will be addressed a little more: dependency and
so in beliefs and views of death per se. Findings also in- control, both probably related to the emphasis on being a
dicated that a particularly sensitive group for observation burden to others. Western culture is known to value in-
is those who have a lifelong unstable personality or, in the dependence and autonomy highly and, although it has
words of berg (60), have lived a bitter life or one been stated that it is also part of the culture to accept de-
marked by pitfalls. pendency in children and older people, this may not be so
Another important aspect of fear and anxiety of death in the views of older people. It seems important and a chal-
and dying was the fine line between these aspects and psy- lenge to our society to alter this view of dependency,
chiatric problems especially depression, but also other bearing in mind that it is a probable component of old age
psychiatric problems. It was obvious in several of the and end of life and, as population demography changes to-
studies that anxiety and fear, as well as a wish to hasten ward a larger proportion of very old people, dealing with
death or not wishing to go on living, were significantly re- dependency will be relevant for many people. There is
lated especially to depression. This is not surprising and probably a relationship between the negative connotation
perhaps not as straightforward as it may seem. In the pre- of being dependent and the fear of being a burden to oth-
sent study, it was decided not to include the wealth of stud- ers, emphasizing autonomy as the ultimate value of life. Al-
ies on suicidal thinking or views of euthanasia, which may so, the wish to be of value to others may be part of the in-
be even more closely related to the prevalence of de- teraction between dependency and being a burden, as a
pression or other problems of a psychiatric nature. Al- way of repaying the help required in old age, and thus even
though there is no answer to this, perhaps we must rec- of being a burden to others and imputing meaning to
ognize the fine line between regarding sadness and sorrow the life of the old person. Lawton et al. (65) stated that the
about imminent death and viewing it through a diagnostic valuation of life is complex and needs to be explored in re-
filter, such as depression, psychosis, or the like. Death lation to environment, health and intra-psychic processes.
and dying are by nature socially, emotionally and exis- Perhaps the cultural meanings of burden, dependency
tentially, and in many cases also physically, a painful and autonomy need to be explored as to whether they may
separation of oneself from life and entering a vacuum or hinder the well-being of older people at the end of life and
afterlife, for which we have no conception that we can dependent on others for their last phase.
hold for true (56), and sadness and sorrow seem to be a
natural part of this process. This must be balanced against CONCLUSIONS
the likeliness of a psychiatric problem open to treat- This review chiefly demonstrated the lack of research on
ment. The longitudinal Berlin Aging Study (63) found that, older peoples view of death and dying. Although quite a
in the very old, the wish to be dead and suicidal intentions few studies were included, they were different in many re-
were related to psychiatric disorders. However, in practice spects and therefore did not contribute to a coherent body
there is a very fine line in differentiating between the un- of knowledge. Thus, the knowledge base for providing
avoidable sadness of approaching death and a treatable high-quality end-of-life care to older people is currently
psychiatric disorder. not strong. Several areas deserve their own review of cur-
rent knowledge available for understanding older people.
Worries about dying; control versus dependency However, the review did support the view that older peo-
Least surprising was perhaps the strong evidence of old- ple do want to speak about death and dying, and this
er peoples worries about dying, the end-of-life phase. should encourage researchers to enter this field, chiefly in
Themes that appeared in the various studies were the im- terms of open conversations about death and dying in
portance of awareness, control, decision-making, depen- the relationship between family doctor and older people.
dency, dignity, feeling treated as a whole person and There is an urgent need for research focusing solely on old-
symptom control; preferably pain and nutrition, not pro- er people from about the age of 60-65 to 90-100, in order
longing life, as well as leading a meaningful life even at the to understand whether there is a later adaptation and
end of life and being meaningful to others. Some studies peaks during which emerging thoughts about death and dy-
also indicated that older people were perhaps prepared for ing are most painful. Such research should preferably be
death but not for dying and for end-of-life care. This had longitudinal, or at least include follow-up studies and com-
been reported before in a comparative study (USA and bine in-depth studies of older peoples reasoning, with
Germany) showing that older people were less likely to pre- surveys giving a broader overview of their view of death and
pare themselves for care at the end of life than for death dying. It seems worthwhile to include aspects like the impact
(64) which is interesting, bearing in mind that they of the life span, the meaning of the completion of life, au-
seem to be more worried about the end-of-life phase tonomy, dependency and control, as well as thoughts
than about death as such. The themes mentioned above about death and after-death, not only from a religious
are wide-ranging and deserve a review of their own in re- perspective, but also fear of death and what is meant by fear
lation to older people. However, two themes, which may of dying. The fact that most people in the Western world

Aging Clin Exp Res, Vol. 16, No. 2 101


I.R. Hallberg

die after the age of 70 calls for better understanding of their 18. Erikson EH, Erikson JM, Kivnick HQ. Vital involvement in old
unique situation, and understanding these aspects pro- age: the experience of old age in our time. New York: W.W. Nor-
ton & Company, 1986.
vides a knowledge base that can inform practitioners of how
19. Goodman C. Literature searching and evidence interpretation for
to approach older people about death and dying when they assessing health care practices. Stockholm: SBU, 1993.
are in the terminal phase, and how to provide them with 20. Leichtentritt DR, Rettig DK. The good death: reaching an in-
high-quality palliative care. ductive understanding. Omega 2000; 41: 221-48.
21. Van der Geest S. I want to go: how older people in Ghana
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS look forward to death. Aging Soc 2000; 22: 7-28.
The Medical Faculty, Lund University, supported this study. I am
22. Fry PS. A factor analytic investigation of home-bound elderly in-
grateful to Ewa Olson, librarian, for efficient support with the data
dividuals concerns about death and dying, and their coping re-
search, Anna Blomgren, secretary, and Alan Crozier for revising the lan-
sponses. J Clin Psychol 1990; 46: 737-48.
guage. Most of all, I am grateful to The Nordic Gerontological Feder-
ation for inspiring me to carry out this review. 23. Rao R, Dening T, Brayne C, Huppert FA. Attitudes toward
death: a community study of octogenarians and nonagenarians.
REFERENCES Int Psychogeriatr 1997; 9: 213-21.
1. Cameron ME. Older persons ethical problems involving their 24. Cicirelli VG. Personal meanings of death in older adults and
health. Nurs Ethics 2002; 9: 537-56. young adults in relation to their fears of death. Death Stud
2001; 25: 663-83.
2. Thom B, Dykes AK, Hallberg IR. Quality of life in old people
with and without cancer. Qual Life Res 2004 (in press). 25. Carmel S. The will to live: gender differences among elderly
persons. Soc Sci Med 2001; 52: 949-58.
3. Brock DB, Foley DJ, Salive ME. Hospital and nursing home use
in the last three months of life. J Aging Health 1996; 8: 307-19. 26. Cataldo JK. Hardiness and death attitudes: predictors of de-
pression in the institutionalized elderly. Arch Psychiatr Nurs
4. Burge F, Lawson B, Johnston G. Trends in the place of death of 1994; 8: 326-32.
cancer patients, 1992-1997. CMAJ 2003; 168: 265-70.
27. Fortner BV, Neimeyer RA. Death anxiety in older adults: a
5. Grande GE, Addington-Hall JM, Todd CJ. Place of death and ac- quantitative review. Death Stud 1999; 23: 387-411.
cess to home care services: are certain patient groups at a dis-
advantage? Soc Sci Med 1998; 47: 565-79. 28. Cicirelli VG. Personality and demographic factors in older adults
fear of death. Gerontologist 1999; 39: 569-79.
6. Glare P, Virik K, Jones M, Hudson M, Eychmuller S, Simes J. A
systematic review of physicians survival predictors in terminally 29. Wu AM, Tang CS, Kwok TC. Death anxiety among Chinese el-
ill cancer patients. BMJ 2003; 327: 195-200. derly people in Hong Kong. J Aging Health 2002; 14: 42-56.
7. Somogyi-Zalud E, Zhong Z, Lynn J, Hamel MB. Elderly persons 30. Sullivan M, Ormel J, Kempen GI, Tymstra T. Beliefs concerning
last six months of life: findings from the Hospitalized Elderly death, dying, and hastening death among older, functionally im-
Longitudinal Project. J Am Geriatr Soc 2000; 48: S131-9. paired Dutch adults: a one-year longitudinal study. J Am Geriatr
Soc 1998; 46: 1251-7.
8. Clark D, Seymor J. Reflections on palliative care. Buckingham:
Open University Press Buckingham, 1999. 31. Cicirelli VG. Older adults views on death. New York: Springer
Publishing Company, 2002.
9. Ottosson J-O, Ed. Patient-lkarrelationen: lkekonst p veten-
skaplig grund. (The patient-doctor relationship). Stockholm: 32. Williams RA. A protestant legacy: attitudes to death and illness
Natur och Kultur, 1999: 282-308. among older Aberdonians. Oxford: Clarendon Press, 1990.
10. Kastenbaum R. Death and dying. In Birren JE, Ed. Encyclopedia 33. Hinton J. The progress of awareness and acceptance of dying as-
of gerontology, age, aging and the aged. San Diego: Academic sessed in cancer patients and their caring relatives. Palliat Med
Press, 1996: 361-81. 1999; 13: 19-35.
11. Redinbaugh EM, Sullivan AM, Block SD, et al. Doctors emotional 34. Langley-Evans A, Payne S. Light-hearted death talk in a palliative
reactions to recent death of a patient: cross sectional study of hos- day care context. J Adv Nurs 1997; 26: 1091-7.
pital doctors. BMJ 2003; 327:185-90. 35. Yedidia MJ, MacGregor B. Confronting the prospect of dying: re-
12. Thom B. Living with cancer in old age: quality of life and ports of terminally ill patients. J Pain Symptom Manage 2001;
meaning. Bulletin No. 14. Lund: Lund University, Faculty of 22: 807-19.
Medicine, 2003 (Thesis). 36. Carrese JA, Mullaney JL, Faden RR, Finucane TE. Planning
13. Aiken LR. Aging: an introduction to gerontology. Oxford: Sage, for death but not serious future illness: qualitative study of house-
1995: 366-98. bound elderly patients. BMJ 2002; 325: 125.
14. Ingebretsen R, Solem PE. Death, dying and bereavement. In 37. Vig EK, Davenport NA, Pearlman RA. Good deaths, bad deaths,
Nordhus I, VandenBos G, Berg S, Fromholt P, Eds. Clinical and preferences for the end of life: a qualitative study of geriatric
geropsychology. Washington: American Psychological Associa- outpatients. J Am Geriatr Soc 2002; 50: 1541-8.
tion, 1998: 177-81. 38. Kelner M. Activists and delegators: elderly patients preferences
15. Kenyon G, Clark P, de Vries B, Eds. Narrative gerontology: about control at the end of life. Soc Sci Med 1995; 41: 537-45.
theory, research and practice. New York: Springer Publishing 39. Cicirelli VG. Elders end-of-life decisions: implications for hospice
Company, 2001: 3-62. care. Hosp J 1997; 12: 57-72.
16. Kbler-Ross E. On death and dying. London: Tavistock, 1970. 40. Wilson DM. End-of-life care preferences of Canadian senior cit-
17. Reker GR, Wong PTP. Aging as an individual process: toward a izens with caregiving experience. J Adv Nurs 2000; 31: 1416-21.
theory of personal meaning. In Birren JE, Bengtsson VL, Eds. 41. Rosenfeld KE, Wenger NS, Kagawa-Singer M. End-of-life decision
Emergent theories of aging. New York: Springer Publishing making: a qualitative study of elderly individuals. J Gen Intern Med
Company, 1999: 214-46. 2000; 15: 620-5.

102 Aging Clin Exp Res, Vol. 16, No. 2


Older peoples view of death and dying

42. Steinhauser KE, Clipp EC, McNeilly M, Christakis NA, McIntyre 54. Hallberg IR. Brytpunkter under ldrandet av relevans fr vrd och
LM, Tulsky JA. In search of a good death: observations of patients, omsorg. (Turning points in the aging process relevant for nursing
families, and providers. Ann Intern Med 2000, 132: 825-32. care). Incitament 2003; 12: 331-5.
43. Steinhauser KE, Christakis NA, Clipp EC, McNeilly M, McIntyre 55. Given B, Given CW. Cancer nursing for the elderly. A target for
L, Tulsky JA. Factors considered important at the end of life by research. Cancer Nurs 1989; 12: 71-7.
patients, family, physicians, and other care providers. JAMA
56. Bauman Z. Mortality, immortality and other life strategies. Cam-
2000; 284: 2476-82.
bridge: Polity, 1992.
44. Leichtentritt RD, Rettig KD. Values underlying end-of-life decisions:
a qualitative approach. Health Soc Work 2001; 26: 150-9. 57. Kaufman SR. Senescence, decline and the quest for a good
death: contemporary dilemmas and historical antecedents. J
45. Chochinov HM, Hack T, Hassard T, Kristjanson LJ, McClement Aging Studies 2000; 14: 1-23.
S, Harlos M. Dignity in the terminally ill: a cross-sectional, cohort
study. Lancet 2002; 360: 2026-30. 58. Merriam SB. Butlers life review: how universal is it? Int J Aging
Hum Dev 1993; 37: 163-75.
46. Lo RS, Woo J, Zhoc KC, et al. Quality of life of palliative care pa-
tients in the last two weeks of life. J Pain Symptom Manage 59. Byock IR. The nature of suffering and the nature of opportunity
2002; 24: 388-97. at the end of life. Clin Geriatr Med 1996; 12: 237-52.
47. Chochinov HM, Wilson KG, Enns M, et al. Desire for death in the 60. berg P. Livet som en berttelse: om biografi och ldrande
terminally ill. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152: 1185-91. (Life as a narrative: on biography and aging). Uppsala: Acta
Universitatis Upsaliensis, 1997 (Thesis).
48. Kelly B, Burnett P, Pelusi D, Badger S, Varghese F, Robertson M.
Terminally ill cancer patients wish to hasten death. Palliat Med 61. Baugher RJ, Burger C, Smith R, Wallston K. A comparison of ter-
2002; 16: 339-45. minally ill persons at various time periods to death. Omega
49. Breitbart W, Rosenfeld B, Pessin H, et al. Depression, hope- 1989-90; 20: 103-15.
lessness, and desire for hastened death in terminally ill patients 62. Dobratz MC. Causal influences of psychological adaptation in dy-
with cancer. JAMA 2000; 284: 2907-11. ing. West J Nurs Res 1993; 15: 24-9, 708-24.
50. Engle VF, Fox-Hill E, Graney MJ. The experience of living-dying 63. Linden M, Barnow S. 1997 IPA/Bayer Research Awards in
in a nursing home: self-reports of black and white older adults. J Psychogeriatrics. The wish to die in very old persons near the end
Am Geriatr Soc 1998; 46: 1091-6. of life: a psychiatric problem? Results from the Berlin Aging
51. McKinlay EM. Within the circle of care: patient experiences of re- Study. Int Psychogeriatr 1997; 9: 291-307.
ceiving palliative care. J Palliat Care 2001; 17: 22-9. 64. Pinquart M, Srensen S. Preparation for death and preparation
52. Singer PA, Martin DK, Kelner M. Quality end-of-life care: patients for care in older community-dwelling adults. Omega 2002; 45:
perspectives. JAMA 1999; 281: 163-8. 69-88.
53. Aday RH, Shahan DR. Elderly reactions to a death education 65. Lawton MP, Moss M, Hoffman C, Grant R, Ten Have T, Kleban
program in a nursing home setting. Gerontol Geriatr Educ MH. Health, valuation of life, and the wish to live. Gerontologist
1995; 15: 3-18. 1999; 39: 406-16.

Aging Clin Exp Res, Vol. 16, No. 2 103

You might also like