You are on page 1of 9

Sleep Duration, Restfulness, and

Screens in the Sleep Environment


Jennifer Falbe, ScD, MPHa, Kirsten K. Davison, PhDb,c, Rebecca L. Franckle, MPHb,c, Claudia Ganter, MPHb,d,
Steven L. Gortmaker, PhDc,e, Lauren Smith, MD, MPHf, Thomas Land, PhDg, Elsie M. Taveras, MD, MPHb,h

Associations of inadequate sleep with numerous health outcomes


BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVE: abstract
among youth necessitate identifying its modifiable determinants. Television (TV) has been
associated with sleep curtailment, but little is known about small screens (eg, smartphones),
which can be used in bed and emit notifications. Therefore, we examined associations of
different screens in sleep environments with sleep duration and perceived insufficient rest
or sleep.
METHODS:Participants included 2048 fourth- and seventh-graders participating in the
Massachusetts Childhood Obesity Research Demonstration Study in 2012 to 2013. Using linear
and log binomial regression, we examined cross-sectional associations of small screens and
TVs in sleep environments and screen time with weekday sleep duration and perceived
insufficient rest or sleep in the past week.
RESULTS: Children who slept near a small screen (compared with never) reported 20.6 fewer
minutes of sleep (95% confidence interval [CI], 229.7 to 211.4) and had a higher
prevalence of perceived insufficient rest or sleep (prevalence ratio, 1.39; 95% CI, 1.21 to
1.60). Children who slept in a room with a TV (compared with no TV) reported 18.0
fewer minutes of sleep (95% CI, 227.9 to 28.1). TV or DVD viewing and video or computer
game playing were associated with both sleep outcomes (P , .01). Some associations
were stronger among Hispanic, non-Hispanic black, and older children (P , .05 for
heterogeneity).
Sleeping near a small screen, sleeping with a TV in the room, and more
CONCLUSIONS:
screen time were associated with shorter sleep durations. Presence of a small screen,
but not a TV, in the sleep environment and screen time were associated with perceived
insufficient rest or sleep. These findings caution against unrestricted screen access in
children’s bedrooms.

a
Division of Community Health and Human Development, School of Public Health, University of California Berkeley, WHAT’S KNOWN ON THIS SUBJECT: Inadequate
Berkeley, California; Departments of bNutrition and cSocial and Behavioral Sciences, and ePrevention Research sleep has been identified as a risk factor for
Center, Harvard School of Public Health, Boston, Massachusetts; dTechnische Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany;
f
National Institute for Children’s Health Quality, Boston, Massachusetts; gBureau of Community Health and obesity and other outcomes. Screen time and the
Prevention, Massachusetts Department of Public Health, Boston, Massachusetts; and hDivision of General presence of a television in the bedroom have
Academic Pediatrics, Department of Pediatrics, Massachusetts General Hospital for Children, Boston,
Massachusetts been associated with inadequate sleep, but little
is known about small screens
Dr Falbe designed, conceptualized, and carried out the analysis, interpreted the data, and drafted
and revised the manuscript; Dr Davison, Ms Franckle, and Drs Gortmaker, Smith, and Land (eg, smartphones).
contributed to the analysis and interpretation and reviewed and revised the manuscript; Ms Gehre
WHAT THIS STUDY ADDS: Among 2048 fourth-
contributed to acquisition of data and reviewed and revised the manuscript; Dr Taveras contributed
to the design, concept, analysis, and interpretation and reviewed and revised the manuscript; and and seventh-graders, children who slept near
all authors approved the final manuscript as submitted. a small screen reported shorter sleep durations
www.pediatrics.org/cgi/doi/10.1542/peds.2014-2306 and perceived insufficient rest or sleep. Presence
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2014-2306 of a television in the bedroom and more screen
Accepted for publication Nov 10, 2014 time were also associated with poorer sleep.

PEDIATRICS Volume 135, number 2, February 2015 ARTICLE


Accumulating evidence indicates that texts, and e-mail). Because these communities were substantially
inadequate sleep is a novel risk factor devices are held near the face, they lower (∼$22 900 and $21 300) than
for obesity in childhood and later in may delay melatonin release more in the state overall (∼$35 500) in
life.1–7 However, consequences of strongly than TV light, which decays 2012.28
inadequate sleep extend well beyond with distance.22 Across 29 schools, 2456 students
obesity. Improving sleep duration and Despite the dramatic increase in the were invited to participate in the
quality may improve somatic and use of small screens, few studies have MA-CORD survey administered by
psychosocial health, school examined children’s use of small school nurses during annual BMI
performance, and risk-taking screens in relation to sleep screenings mandated in Massachusetts
behaviors among youth8 and reduce duration,23–25 and these studies have public schools. All data collection
hypertension, coronary heart disease, taken place outside the United States procedures were approved by
and stroke in adulthood.9,10 Sleep may among predominantly white or Asian the internal review board at the
also play a critical role in immunity.11 youth. However, the presence of a small Massachusetts Department of Public
Therefore, the steady decline in screen in a child’s bedroom may Health.
child sleep duration throughout the affect sleep beyond use, because unlike
past century is troubling.12 TVs, small screens can emit audible Measures
Effective sleep promotion necessitates notifications (eg, text messages) when
Primary outcomes were usual
identifying its modifiable not in use. These alerts may not only
weekday sleep duration and
determinants in children’s delay sleep but also interrupt it,
perceived insufficient rest or sleep in
environments and routines. Sleep thereby reducing overall sleep quality.
the past week. Secondary outcomes
curtailment has widely been We are aware of only 1 study assessing
were usual weekday bedtimes and
attributed to modern lifestyle, small screens in children’s sleep
waketimes in the past week. We
especially technology. Fueled by the environments and sleep duration.23
estimated sleep duration by
proliferation of new devices and the Therefore, we sought to examine subtracting self-reported waketime
affordability of older technologies such associations of small screens and TVs from bedtime, assessed by asking, “On
as televisions (TVs), youth are now in children’s sleep environments and a usual weekday this past week, when
consuming media for the amount of reported screen time with children’s did you go to bed at night?” and
time that most adults spend at work.13 sleep duration, perceived insufficient “On a usual weekday this past week,
Presence of a TV in a child’s bedroom rest or sleep, and usual bedtimes when did you wake up the next
and TV viewing have been linked to and waketimes. We examined these morning?” Child-reported sleep
shorter sleep duration, later relationships among racially and durations from similar surveys have
bedtimes, and other dimensions of ethnically diverse fourth- and been moderately to strongly
sleep.14 TV viewing is a risk factor for seventh-graders attending public correlated with duration from
weight gain,15,16 decreased academic schools in 2 Massachusetts cities. actigraphy and sleep diaries.29 We
achievement, and behavioral assessed perceived insufficient rest or
problems.15 Although there is ample sleep in the past week by using an
METHODS
evidence that food marketing item modified from the Behavioral
mediates the TV–obesity Participants and Setting Risk Factor Surveillance System
relationship,17–20 sleep may also Questionnaire30: “On how many days
Data were collected in October
mediate this relationship and others. in the past week have you felt like you
through December 2012 as part of
Possible mechanisms include direct needed more sleep?” Response
the baseline assessment of fourth-
displacement of screen time for sleep, options ranged from 0 to 7 days.
and seventh-grade public school
increased cognitive and emotional Because of its strongly bimodal
students participating in the
arousal, and delays in circadian distribution, perceived insufficient
Massachusetts Childhood Obesity
rhythm from screen light.14 rest or sleep was modeled
Research Demonstration Study
Compared with TV, which involves dichotomously, as feeling like more
(MA-CORD).26,27 MA-CORD is
passive observation, interactive sleep was needed most days
a multisector intervention to
media such as video games and address childhood obesity in 2 (ie, .3 days) or #3 days. In a study
smartphones may be more disruptive Massachusetts communities. examining perceived insufficient rest
of sleep.21 Smartphones and other MA-CORD communities are or sleep and chronic disease risk
Internet-enabled small devices are predominantly non-Hispanic white among adults, a similar cutoff was
particularly concerning, because they (68%) with large Hispanic clinically meaningful.31
are portals to almost all content populations (17% and 22%).28 Per Primary exposures were screens in
(eg, games, music, videos, Web sites, capita incomes in MA-CORD children’s sleep environments,

2 FALBE et al
including small screens Analytic Sample beyond curtailing sleep (eg, by
(eg, smartphones) and TVs, assessed Eligible participants (2061 of 2456) interrupting sleep or shortening rapid
with the following item: “Some kids had complete data on sleep, screens eye movement sleep). Consequently,
use devices to play games or send in their environment, and covariates. to examine associations between
text messages or chats to their Of the 2061 eligible participants, 13 media and aspects of sleep beyond
friends like cell phones, were excluded because of implausible duration, model 3, a model with
smartphones, and the iPod Touch. sleep durations (ie, ,3 hours or perceived insufficient rest or sleep as
How often do you sleep with one of .16 hours).34 Therefore, our primary the outcome, additionally adjusted for
these devices near where you sleep, analytic sample consisted of 2048 sleep duration. We conducted
such as in your bed or next to your children. Our sample for examining analyses by using SAS (version 9.3;
bed?” Response options ranged from TV or DVD viewing and video or SAS Institute, Inc, Cary, NC).
0 to 7 days and were dichotomized computer game playing consisted of
as ever and never in the main 1908 youth with complete and
analysis because of the strongly RESULTS
plausible screen times (71 youth with
bimodal distribution. However, we TV or DVD or gaming time .3 SDs Mean age 6 SD of the sample
also examined exposure as from the mean were excluded). (Table 1) was 10.6 6 1.5 years.
a continuous variable. We Hispanic (40%), non-Hispanic white
determined presence of a TV in the Statistical Analyses (38%), and non-Hispanic black (10%)
sleep environment by asking, “Is were the predominant racial and
there a television in the room where Using multivariate linear regression, ethnic groups. The majority reported
you sleep?” We assessed additional we examined associations of screens sleeping near small screens (54%)
exposures (TV or DVD viewing and in children’s sleep environments and and in a room with a TV (75%).
video or computer games in the past screen time with sleep duration, A higher proportion of seventh-graders
week) by asking how much time bedtime, and waketime. Multivariate (65%) reported sleeping near a small
students spent with each on a usual log binomial regression35 was used to screen than fourth-graders (46%).
weekday and weekend in the past examine associations between Seventh-graders also reported
week. Moderate validity has been screens in children’s sleep shorter sleep durations (8.8 hours)
reported for similar surveys of child- environments and perceived than fourth-graders (9.8 hours) but
reported screen time.32,33 insufficient rest or sleep. We used were less likely to have perceived
generalized estimating equations for insufficient rest or sleep.
Covariates included self-reported estimation, specifying an
gender, grade, race or ethnicity, and exchangeable covariance structure to Associations between screens in sleep
physical activity. Students described account for clustering by school.36,37 environments and sleep duration are
their race or ethnicity by selecting presented in Table 2. Described
$1 options (white, black or African For each outcome, we ran partially here are results from fully adjusted
American, Hispanic or Latino, Asian, (model 1) and fully adjusted (model models (model 2). Children who slept
Native Hawaiian or other Pacific 2) models. Model 1 adjusted for near a small screen reported 20.6
Islander, American Indian or Alaska gender, grade, race or ethnicity, and fewer minutes of sleep per weekday in
Native, or other). Race or ethnicity city. Model 2 additionally adjusted for the past week (95% confidence
was categorized into Hispanic, non- physical activity, and when exposures interval [CI], 229.7 to 211.4) than
Hispanic white, non-Hispanic black, were screens in the sleep those who never slept near a small
non-Hispanic other (because of small environment, model 2 simultaneously screen, independent of having a TV in
numbers), and non-Hispanic examined presence of small screens the room. When modeled as
multiracial. We assessed physical and a TV in the same model; when a continuous variable, each additional
activity by asking on which days in exposures were screen time, model day of sleeping near a small screen was
the last week students took part in 2 simultaneously included hours per associated with 3.7 fewer minutes of
physical activity that made their heart day of TV or DVDs and video or sleep per weekday (95% CI, 25.4 to
beat fast or made them breathe computer games. We examined 22.0). Children who slept in a room
hard for $30 minutes. Days were heterogeneity by grade, gender, with a TV reported 18.0 fewer minutes
summed and modeled continuously. physical activity, and race or ethnicity of weekday sleep (95% CI, 227.9 to
Physical activity was included as and tested for interaction between 28.1) than those without a TV in their
a covariate to reduce confounding by presence of a small screen and a TV room, independent of small screens.
common determinants of screen time by including cross-products of these For non-Hispanic black youth, sleeping
and physical activity terms in model 2. near a small screen was associated
(eg, socioeconomic status [SES], Additionally, small screens can affect with an additional 30.8 fewer minutes
parenting). perceived insufficient rest or sleep of weekday sleep (95% CI, 252.8 to

PEDIATRICS Volume 135, number 2, February 2015 3


TABLE 1 Sample Characteristics of 2048 Children Attending Public School in 2 Massachusetts Each hour per day of gaming was
Cities associated with a 9.8-minute later
Grade 4 Grade 7 bedtime (95% CI, 5.1 to 14.5). Screen
(n = 1194) (n = 854) time was not associated with
Child characteristics, mean 6 SD or % waketime.
Female 51.4 51.4
Age, y 9.4 6 0.6 12.3 6 0.6 Each hour spent watching TV or DVDs
Race or ethnicity and playing video or computer games
Hispanic 38.8 42.6 was associated with a higher
White, non-Hispanic 38.0 39.0 prevalence of perceived insufficient
Black, non-Hispanic 10.8 8.5
rest or sleep (Table 3). The
Other, non-Hispanica 5.4 3.2
Multiracial, non-Hispanicb 7.0 6.8 relationship between TV or DVD
Overweight (BMI $85th to ,95th percentilec) 18.6 19.6 viewing and perceived insufficient
Obese (BMI $95th percentilec) 27.2 30.1 rest or sleep was stronger for
Days in past week participated in $30 min of physical 3.3 6 2.2 2.8 6 2.2 seventh- than fourth-graders (Fig 2),
activity
and the association between video
Screens in child sleep environment, %
Ever sleeps near a small screend 45.9 64.5 or computer games and insufficient
TV in room in which child sleeps 74.1 75.6 rest or sleep was attenuated
Screen timee (prevalence ratio [PR] for the
TV or DVDs (h/d) 2.4 6 1.9 2.9 6 2.1 cross-product of video or computer
Video or computer games (h/d) 1.8 6 1.8 2.0 6 2.0
games and days of physical activity,
Outcomes, mean 6 SD or %
Usual sleep duration per 24 h on a weekday in the past 9.8 6 1.4 8.8 6 1.5 0.93; 95% CI, 0.88 to 0.98]).
week, h Associations did not vary by gender
Perceived insufficient rest or sleep .3 d in past week 32.8 22.6 or race or ethnicity (P . .05 for
Usual bedtime in the past week, hh:mm PM 9:15 6 2:34 10:12 6 3:09 heterogeneity).
a Includes American Indian or Alaska Natives, Asian, and Hawaiian or Pacific Islander.
b Includes youth who indicated .1 race but did not identify as Hispanic or Latino. In model 3, which additionally
c Heights and weights were measured by school nurses using the protocol in the Massachusetts Department of Public
adjusted for sleep duration, a likely
Health’s BMI Screening Guidelines for Schools. Percentile was calculated by using the 2000 CDC growth charts.
d Devices used to play games or send text messages or chats such as cell phones, smartphones, and the iPod Touch.
mediator, associations of small
e Sample consisted of 1139 fourth-graders and 769 seventh-graders. screens, TV or DVD viewing, and
video and computer game playing
with perceived insufficient rest or
28.9) compared with non-Hispanic 1.60) times the prevalence of sleep were significant with some mild
white youth (Fig 1). Results did not perceived insufficient rest or sleep. attenuation (PR, 1.33; 95% CI, 1.16
vary by grade, gender, or physical This did not vary significantly by to 1.53 for small screens; PR 1.04;
activity (P , .05 for heterogeneity). grade, gender, physical activity, or 95% CI, 1.01 to 1.07 for TV or DVD
Associations between screens in race. Each additional day of sleeping viewing; and PR 1.05; 95% CI, 1.01 to
sleep environments and sleep near a small screen (modeled 1.08 for games).
duration resulted from delayed continuously) was associated with
bedtime. Sleeping near a small 1.07 (95% CI, 1.05 to 1.09) times the DISCUSSION
screen and sleeping with a TV in the prevalence of perceived insufficient
Among 2048 fourth- and seventh-
room were associated with rest or sleep. For all outcomes, we did
grade public school students in
37.0-minute (95% CI, 20.1 to 53.9) and not detect interaction between small
Massachusetts, children who slept
31.1-minute (95% CI, 19.1 to screens and presence of a TV.
near a small screen (compared with
43.1) later bedtimes, respectively. Both Among the 1908 children with screen never) and children with a TV in their
associations were stronger for time data, time watching TV or DVDs sleep environment (compared with
non-Hispanic black and Hispanic and playing video/computer games those without) had shorter weekday
children compared with non-Hispanic was significantly associated with sleep durations. Relationships
white children (Fig 1) and for seventh- shorter weekday sleep durations between screens in the sleep
graders compared with fourth- (Table 2). Associations did not vary environment and sleep curtailment
graders (Fig 2). Neither screen was by grade, gender, activity, or race were accounted for by later bedtimes,
associated with waketime. (P . .05 for heterogeneity) and were consistent with students having fixed
Table 3 shows results for perceived accounted for by later bedtimes. Each weekday waketimes for school.
insufficient rest or sleep. Sleeping hour per day of TV or DVD viewing Presence of a small screen but not
near a small screen, but not a TV, was was associated with a 3.7-minute a TV in the sleep environment was
associated with 1.39 (95% CI, 1.21 to (95% CI, 0.7 to 6.7) later bedtime. significantly related to perceived

4 FALBE et al
TABLE 2 Associations of Screens in Children’s Sleep Environment and Screen Time With Typical and between TV or DVD viewing and
Weekday Daily Sleep Duration in the Past Week perceived insufficient rest or sleep
Weekday Sleep Duration, min were stronger for seventh-graders
Model 1 (partially Model 2 (fully than fourth-graders. Lastly, physical
adjusted)a adjusted)b activity attenuated the association
between video or computer games
b (95% CI) b (95% CI)
and perceived insufficient rest or
Presence of screen in sleep environment sleep. Although most studies of
(n = 2048)
stressors related to race or ethnicity
Small screenc 222.6*** (231.3 to 213.8) 220.6*** (229.7 to 211.4)
TVd 221.3*** (230.5 to 212.1) 218.0*** (227.9 to 28.1) and sleep have been conducted among
Screen time, h/d (n = 1908) adults, it is possible that similar
TV or DVDs 24.8*** (26.4 to 23.2) 23.6*** (25.3 to 21.9) stressors increase youth’s
Video or computer games 26.0*** (28.1 to 23.9) 25.1*** (27.4 to 22.7) vulnerability to effects of screens on
a Results from multivariate linear regression models using generalized estimating equations for estimation, adjusted for sleep. Shorter sleep durations have
grade, gender, race or ethnicity (Hispanic, non-Hispanic white, non-Hispanic black, non-Hispanic other, non-Hispanic
multiracial), and city.
been documented among racial or
b Additionally adjusted for days in past week participated in $30 min of physical activity, simultaneously included ethnic minorities,38–40 and
indicators for presence of a small screen and TV in the child’s sleep environment for models examining screens in the discrimination may partially mediate
sleep environment, and simultaneously included hours per day of TV or DVD viewing and video or computer game playing
in models examining screen time.
these differences.40 Other
c The reference group reported never sleeping near a small screen in the past week. psychosocial stressors associated with
d The reference group reported there was not a TV in the room in which they sleep.
sleep problems (eg, SES, relationship
***P , .001.
stress, neighborhood disorder, abuse,
and stressful life events)40 may have
insufficient rest or rest or sleep in the some groups. Small screens were also been more common among
past week. TV or DVD viewing and associated with shorter sleep minorities in our sample. Stronger
video or computer game playing were duration among non-Hispanic black associations observed for older youth
associated with shorter weekday may have been accounted for by
compared with non-Hispanic white
differences in content or timing. For
sleep duration and perceived children. Having a small screen or TV
example, seventh-graders may watch
insufficient rest or sleep. Children in the sleep environment was more violent or stress-inducing
with more screen time also had later associated with later bedtimes among programs and at later times than
bedtimes. non-Hispanic black and Hispanic fourth-graders, increasing impacts of
The large size of our sample enabled compared with non-Hispanic white screens on sleep. Lastly, physical
us to detect stronger associations children. Associations between activity has been associated with
between screens and sleep among presence of a TV and later bedtime better sleep quality,41,42

FIGURE 1
Differences by race or ethnicity in the associations between screens in the sleep environment and weekday sleep duration and bedtime estimated from
fully adjusted models (model 2). A, Minutes of sleep duration associated with sleeping near a small screen. B, Bedtime (minutes) associated with sleeping
near a small screen. C, Bedtime (minutes) associated with sleeping in a room with a TV. P values for differences in associations compared with non-
Hispanic white youth: aP , .05, bP , .01, cP , .001.

PEDIATRICS Volume 135, number 2, February 2015 5


sleeping near a small screen (54%)
than was found in the Canadian
sample (17%).
Additionally, we found that children
who slept near a small screen had
a higher prevalence of perceived
insufficient rest or sleep, even after
adjustment for sleep duration, a likely
mediator. Perceived insufficient rest
or sleep may reflect not only duration
but also other sleep parameters,
including poor quality, awakenings, or
parasomnias. Exposure to stimulating
content on small screens around
FIGURE 2 bedtime and receiving calls or audible
Differences by grade in the associations between (A) sleeping in a room with a TV and later bedtime
(minutes) and (B) TV or DVD viewing (h/d) and perceived insufficient rest or sleep from fully
alerts while sleeping may alter these
adjusted models (model 2). *P , .05. parameters. The 2011 Sleep in
America Poll revealed that 18% of
adolescents were awakened at least
a characteristic that may partially cellular phones and other handheld a few times a night by phones.21 Also,
compensate for sleep-disruptive communication devices in a child’s a longitudinal study of children in
activities such as video or computer bedroom in relation to sleep duration Belgium found that using mobile
game playing. These results suggest among a predominantly Caucasian phones after lights out was associated
that effects of screens on sleep may sample of fifth-graders in Canada.23 with feeling very tired 1 year later.43
not be experienced uniformly. The authors reported that presence of
Our finding that a TV in the sleep
cell phones or similar devices was
This study makes a novel environment was associated with
associated with 6 fewer minutes per
contribution by examining how shorter sleep duration is concordant
day of sleep. We observed a stronger
sleeping near a small screen is with other studies.44–48 Also
magnitude of association, perhaps
associated with sleep, independent of consistent with the extant literature14
because of our use of child report
the known association with TV in is our finding that watching TV or
rather than their use of parent report,
DVDs and playing video or computer
the bedroom, among a diverse sample our examination of weekday duration,
games were associated with shorter
of children in the United States. One or differences in populations. We
sleep duration and perceived
other study has examined presence of also observed a higher prevalence of
insufficient rest or sleep. However,
unlike small screens, TV presence
TABLE 3 Associations of Screens in Children’s Sleep Environment and Screen Time With Perceived was not significantly related to
Insufficient Rest or Sleep
perceived insufficient rest or sleep,
Perceived Insufficient Rest or Sleep
perhaps because TV sets do not
Model 1 (partially Model 2 (fully interrupt sleep when turned off. Also,
adjusted)a adjusted)b compared with TV or DVD viewing,
PR (95% CI) PR (95% CI) which was significantly associated
with perceived insufficient rest or
Presence of screen in sleep environment (n = 2048)
Small screenc 1.38*** (1.19 to 1.61) 1.39*** (1.21 to 1.60) sleep, for TV presence it is possible
TVd 1.13 (0.99 to 1.29) 1.07 (0.91 to 1.26) that not all TV sets in this sample
Screen time, h/d (n = 1908) were functional or used; TV presence
TV or DVDs 1.06*** (1.03 to 1.09) 1.05*** (1.02 to 1.08) may not guarantee high screen time,
Video or computer games 1.07*** (1.03 to 1.11) 1.05** (1.01 to 1.09)
especially if parents impose screen
a Results from multivariate log binomial regression models using generalized estimating equations for estimation,
limits, or TV viewing may occur in
adjusted for grade, gender, race or ethnicity (Hispanic, non-Hispanic white, non-Hispanic black, non-Hispanic other, non-
Hispanic multiracial), and city. other locations.
b Additionally adjusted for days in past week participated in $30 min of physical activity, simultaneously included

indicators for presence of a small screen and TV in the child’s sleep environment for models examining screens in the For all media, additional mechanisms
sleep environment, and simultaneously included hours per day of TV or DVD viewing and video or computer game playing that may underlie the screen–sleep
in models examining screen time. relationship include direct
c The reference group reported never sleeping near a small screen in the past week.
d The reference group reported there was not a TV in the room in which they sleep. displacement of sleep or of behaviors
**P , .01, ***P , .001. that may promote sleep with screen

6 FALBE et al
time; consumption of heavily potentially important confounders, a small screen and those with more
advertised beverages containing such as SES, parenting style screen time were more likely to
caffeine; evening exposure to bright, (eg, permissive parenting), or the have perceived insufficient rest or
short-wavelength light interfering with overscheduling of youth. This study sleep in the past week. Although
circadian rhythm40,41; or increased also has several strengths. It longitudinal and experimental
cognitive, physiologic, and emotional included a large, racially and studies are needed to confirm these
arousal14 after playing a video game,49 ethnically diverse sample, associations, our findings caution
watching an exciting movie or show, or investigated differences by grade, against children’s unfettered access
sending chats or texts. gender, and race or ethnicity, and to screen-based media in their
The primary limitation of this study helped to address the dearth of rooms. Future studies should
is its cross-sectional design, limiting studies assessing small screens and incorporate detailed assessments of
our ability to make causal sleep. screen content to identify the types
inferences. Although longitudinal most strongly related to poor
evidence indicates that screens CONCLUSIONS sleep. Longitudinal studies should
affect subsequent sleep, the Among a diverse sample of also continue to examine the
relationship may be bidirectional. Massachusetts fourth- and seventh- mediating contribution of sleep to
Another limitation is that measures grade public school students, the screen time’s impact on obesity and
were self-reported, introducing presence of small screens and TVs other outcomes.
random error and possible social in sleep environments and the use
desirability bias. Additionally, we of TV or DVDs and video or ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
did not measure weekend sleep computer games were associated The authors thank the thousands of
duration or media content, nor did with shorter weekday sleep students who participated in
we assess and therefore adjust for duration. Children who slept near MA-CORD.

Address correspondence to Jennifer Falbe, ScD, MPH, 50 University Hall #7360, Berkeley, CA 94720-7360. E-mail: jfalbe@berkeley.edu
PEDIATRICS (ISSN Numbers: Print, 0031-4005; Online, 1098-4275).
Copyright © 2015 by the American Academy of Pediatrics
FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.
FUNDING: Supported by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC), National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion (award
U18DP003370). Dr Falbe’s work was supported by the National Institutes of Health Training Grant in Academic Nutrition (DK007703) and the American Heart
Association Postdoctoral Fellowship (14POST20140055). The contents of this study are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the
official views of the listed funding sources. Funded by the National Institutes of Health (NIH).
POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

REFERENCES
1. Knutson KL. Does inadequate sleep play 5. Bell JF, Zimmerman FJ. Shortened 9. Guo X, Zheng L, Wang J, et al.
a role in vulnerability to obesity? Am J nighttime sleep duration in early life and Epidemiological evidence for the link
Hum Biol. 2012;24(3):361–371 subsequent childhood obesity. Arch between sleep duration and high blood
2. Reilly JJ, Armstrong J, Dorosty AR, et al; Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2010;164(9): pressure: a systematic review and meta-
Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and 840–845 analysis. Sleep Med. 2013;14(4):324–332
Children Study Team. Early life risk 6. Seegers V, Petit D, Falissard B, et al. 10. Cappuccio FP, Cooper D, D’Elia L,
factors for obesity in childhood: cohort Short sleep duration and body mass Strazzullo P, Miller MA. Sleep duration
study. BMJ. 2005;330(7504):1357 index: a prospective longitudinal study in predicts cardiovascular outcomes:
3. Snell EK, Adam EK, Duncan GJ. Sleep and preadolescence. Am J Epidemiol. 2011; a systematic review and meta-analysis of
the body mass index and overweight 173(6):621–629 prospective studies. Eur Heart J. 2011;
status of children and adolescents. Child 7. Patel SR, Hu FB. Short sleep duration and 32(12):1484–1492
Dev. 2007;78(1):309–323 weight gain: a systematic review. Obesity 11. Bryant PA, Curtis N. Sleep and infection:
4. Taveras EM, Rifas-Shiman SL, Oken E, (Silver Spring). 2008;16(3):643–653 no snooze, you lose? Pediatr Infect Dis J.
Gunderson EP, Gillman MW. Short sleep 8. Shochat T, Cohen-Zion M, Tzischinsky O. 2013;32(10):1135–1137
duration in infancy and risk of childhood Functional consequences of inadequate 12. Matricciani L, Olds T, Petkov J. In search
overweight. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. sleep in adolescents: a systematic of lost sleep: secular trends in the sleep
2008;162(4):305–311 review. Sleep Med Rev. 2014;18(1):75–87 time of school-aged children and

PEDIATRICS Volume 135, number 2, February 2015 7


adolescents. Sleep Med Rev. 2012;16(3): 23. Chahal H, Fung C, Kuhle S, Veugelers PJ. 33. Schmitz KH, Harnack L, Fulton JE, et al.
203–211 Availability and night-time use of Reliability and validity of a brief
13. Rideout VJ, Foehr UG, Roberts DF. electronic entertainment and questionnaire to assess television
Generation M2: Media in the Lives of 8- to communication devices are associated viewing and computer use by middle
18-Year-Olds. Menlo Park, CA: Kaiser with short sleep duration and obesity school children. J Sch Health. 2004;74(9):
among Canadian children. Pediatr Obes. 370–377
Family Foundation; 2010:8010
2013;8(1):42–51
14. Cain N, Gradisar M. Electronic media use 34. Kim S, DeRoo LA, Sandler DP. Eating
24. Arora T, Hussain S, Hubert Lam KB, Lily patterns and nutritional
and sleep in school-aged children and
Yao G, Neil Thomas G, Taheri S. Exploring characteristics associated with sleep
adolescents: a review. Sleep Med. 2010;
the complex pathways among specific duration. Public Health Nutr. 2011;
11(8):735–742
types of technology, self-reported sleep 14(5):889–895
15. Tremblay MS, LeBlanc AG, Kho ME, et al. duration and body mass index in UK
Systematic review of sedentary 35. Spiegelman D, Hertzmark E. Easy SAS
adolescents. Int J Obes (Lond). 2013;
behaviour and health indicators in calculations for risk or prevalence ratios
37(9):1254–1260
school-aged children and youth. Int J and differences. Am J Epidemiol. 2005;
25. Yen CF, Ko CH, Yen JY, Cheng CP. The 162(3):199–200
Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2011;8:98
multidimensional correlates associated
16. Falbe J, Rosner B, Willett WC, Sonneville 36. Hanley JA, Negassa A, Edwardes MD,
with short nocturnal sleep duration and
KR, Hu FB, Field AE. Adiposity and Forrester JE. Statistical analysis of
subjective insomnia among Taiwanese
different types of screen time. correlated data using generalized
adolescents. Sleep. 2008;31(11):
Pediatrics. 2013;132(6). Available at: estimating equations: an orientation.
1515–1525
www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/132/ Am J Epidemiol. 2003;157(4):364–375
26. Taveras EM, Blaine R, Davison KK, et al.
6/e1497 37. Liang K-Y, Zeger SL. Longitudinal data
Design of the Massachusetts Childhood
17. Harris JL, Bargh JA, Brownell KD. Obesity Research Demonstration analysis using generalized linear
Priming effects of television food (MA-CORD) Study [published online models. Biometrika. 1986;73(1):13–22
advertising on eating behavior. Health ahead of print December 3, 2014]. Child 38. Matthews KA, Hall M, Dahl RE. Sleep in
Psychol. 2009;28(4):404–413 Obes. PMID:25469676 healthy black and white adolescents.
18. Halford JC, Boyland EJ, Hughes G, 27. Davison KK, Falbe J, Taveras EM, et al. Pediatrics. 2014;133(5). Available at:
Oliveira LP, Dovey TM. Beyond-brand Evaluation overview for the www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/133/
effect of television (TV) food Massachusetts Childhood Obesity 5/e1189
advertisements/commercials on caloric Research Demonstration (MA-CORD) 39. Maslowsky J, Ozer EJ. Developmental
intake and food choice of 5–7-year-old Study. Child Obes. 2015, In press) trends in sleep duration in
children. Appetite. 2007;49(1):263–267 28. US Census Bureau. State and County adolescence and young adulthood:
19. Borzekowski DL, Robinson TN. The QuickFacts. 2012; http://quickfacts. evidence from a national United States
30-second effect: an experiment revealing census.gov/qfd/states/25000.html. sample. J Adolesc Health. 2014;54(6):
the impact of television commercials Accessed February 1, 2014 691–697
on food preferences of preschoolers. 29. Matricciani L. Subjective reports of 40. Slopen N, Williams DR. Discrimination,
J Am Diet Assoc. 2001;101(1):42–46 children’s sleep duration: does the other psychosocial stressors, and self-
20. Falbe J, Willett WC, Rosner B, Gortmaker question matter? A literature review. reported sleep duration and difficulties.
SL, Sonneville KR, Field AE. Longitudinal Sleep Med. 2013;14(4):303–311 Sleep. 2014;37(1):147–156
relations of television, electronic games, 30. Centers for Disease Control and 41. Lang C, Brand S, Feldmeth AK, Holsboer-
and digital versatile discs with changes Prevention (CDC). Behavioral Risk Factor Trachsler E, Pühse U, Gerber M.
in diet in adolescents. Am J Clin Nutr. Surveillance System Questionnaire. Increased self-reported and objectively
2014;100(4):1173–1181 Atlanta, GA: US Department of Health and assessed physical activity predict sleep
21. Gradisar M, Wolfson AR, Harvey AG, Hale Human Services, Centers for Disease quality among adolescents. Physiol
L, Rosenberg R, Czeisler CA. The sleep Control and Prevention; 2008. Available Behav. 2013;120:46–53
and technology use of Americans: at: www.cdc.gov/brfss/annual_data/ 42. Yang PY, Ho KH, Chen HC, Chien MY.
findings from the National Sleep pdf-ques/2008brfss.pdf Exercise training improves sleep
Foundation’s 2011 Sleep in America 31. Shankar A, Syamala S, Kalidindi S. quality in middle-aged and older adults
poll. J Clin Sleep Med. 2013;9(12): Insufficient rest or sleep and its relation with sleep problems: a systematic
1291–1299 to cardiovascular disease, diabetes and review. J Physiother. 2012;58(3):
22. Calamaro CJ, Mason TB, Ratcliffe SJ. obesity in a national, multiethnic sample. 157–163
Adolescents living the 24/7 lifestyle: PLoS ONE. 2010;5(11):e14189 43. Van den Bulck J. Adolescent use of
effects of caffeine and technology on 32. Gortmaker SL, Peterson K, Wiecha J, mobile phones for calling and for
sleep duration and daytime functioning. et al. Reducing obesity via a school- sending text messages after lights out:
Pediatrics. 2009;123(6). Available at: based interdisciplinary intervention results from a prospective cohort study
www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/123/ among youth: Planet Health. Arch Pediatr with a one-year follow-up. Sleep. 2007;
6/e1005 Adolesc Med. 1999;153(4):409–418 30(9):1220–1223

8 FALBE et al
44. Garmy P, Nyberg P, Jakobsson U. Sleep hygiene: findings from the 2004 National exposure and daytime sleep-related
and television and computer habits of Sleep Foundation Sleep in America Poll. behaviours among Israeli
Swedish school-age children. J Sch Nurs. Sleep Med. 2009;10(7):771–779 adolescents. Acta Paediatr. 2010;
2012;28(6):469–476 47. Owens J, Maxim R, McGuinn M, Nobile C, 99(9):1396–1400
45. Li S, Jin X, Wu S, Jiang F, Yan C, Shen X. Msall M, Alario A. Television-viewing 49. Higuchi S, Motohashi Y, Liu Y, Maeda A.
The impact of media use on sleep habits and sleep disturbance in school Effects of playing a computer game
patterns and sleep disorders among children. Pediatrics. 1999;104(3). using a bright display on presleep
school-aged children in China. Sleep. Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/ physiological variables, sleep
2007;30(3):361–367 content/full/104/3/e27 latency, slow wave sleep and
46. Mindell JA, Meltzer LJ, Carskadon MA, 48. Shochat T, Flint-Bretler O, Tzischinsky REM sleep. J Sleep Res. 2005;14(3):
Chervin RD. Developmental aspects of sleep O. Sleep patterns, electronic media 267–273

PEDIATRICS Volume 135, number 2, February 2015 9

You might also like