You are on page 1of 6

Received: 28 February 2017 Revised: 5 June 2017 Accepted: 29 June 2017

DOI: 10.1002/cpp.2116

RESEARCH ARTICLE

Ambivalence and guilt feelings: Two relevant variables for


understanding caregivers' depressive symptomatology
Andrés Losada1 | María Márquez‐González2 | Carlos Vara‐García1 |

Laura Gallego‐Alberto2 | Rosa Romero‐Moreno1 | Karl Pillemer3

1
Clinical Psychology Area, Universidad Rey
Juan Carlos, Madrid, Spain Providing care for a relative with dementia is considered to be a chronic stressor that has been
2
Department of Biological and Clinical linked to negative mental health consequences for caregivers. A theoretical model was developed
Psychology, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, and tested to assess the degree to which ambivalence and guilt feelings contribute to caregivers'
Madrid, Spain
depressive symptomatology. Participants included 212 dementia family caregivers. In addition to
3
Department of Human Development, Cornell
ambivalence and guilt feelings, sociodemographic characteristics, behavioural and psychological
University, Ithaca, NY, USA
symptoms of dementia, and depressive symptomatology were assessed. Hypotheses derived
Correspondence
Andrés Losada Baltar, Facultad de Ciencias de from the stress and coping model were tested using path analysis. The analysed model showed
la Salud, Universidad Rey Juan Carlos, an excellent fit to the data. In total, 35% of the variance in depressive symptomatology was
Departamental II, Avda de Atenas s/n 28922 explained by the assessed variables. Although significant correlations were obtained between fre-
Alcorcón, Spain.
Email: andres.losada@urjc.es
quency and appraisal of behavioural and psychological symptoms of dementia and depression,
the obtained model suggests that this effect was mediated through ambivalence. In addition,
ambivalence had an indirect effect on depression. The association of ambivalence with depres-
Funding information
Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competi- sion was explained through feelings of guilt; that is, respondents who reported ambivalent feel-
tiveness, Grant/Award Number: PSI2015‐ ings were more likely to experience guilt, leading in turn to greater depressive symptomatology.
65152‐C2‐1‐R and PSI2012‐31293; U. S.
The findings suggest that dementia caregivers' ambivalence and guilt feelings are relevant for
National Institute on Aging, Grant/Award
Number: P30AG022845 understanding their depressive symptomatology. These factors should be addressed in psycho-
logical interventions with caregivers.

KEY W ORDS

Alzheimer's disease, depression, disruptive behaviours, mediation, stress and coping

1 | I N T RO D U CT I O N contribute significantly to caregivers' level of depression, with disrup-


tive behaviours such as aggression among the most distressing (Fauth
Providing care for a relative with dementia is considered to be a & Gibbons, 2014). However, although most caregivers face stressful sit-
chronic stressor (Robinson‐Whelen, Tada, MacCallum, McGuire, & uations, not all of them suffer negative outcomes to the same degree.
Kiecolt‐Glaser, 2001) that has been linked to negative mental health An underexplored variable that may contribute to caregivers' depres-
consequences for caregivers (Pinquart & Sörensen, 2003). One of the sive symptoms is feelings of guilt. Guilt has been described as “the dys-
most studied consequences of caregiving is depressive symptomatol- phoric feeling associated with the recognition that one has violated a
ogy. Research suggests that over one third of caregivers suffer from personally relevant moral or social standard” (Kugler & Jones, 1992; p.
clinical depression (Sallim, Sayampanathan, Cuttilan, & Ho, 2015). 318). Studies have found that guilt feelings among caregivers to older per-
Several theoretical models, such as the stress and coping model sons are associated with depressive symptoms (Gonyea, Paris, & de Saxe
(Haley, Levine, Brown, & Bartolucci, 1987), have received strong empir- Zerden, 2008) and frequency of or reaction to care‐recipients disruptive
ical confirmation as explanatory models for caregivers' emotional dis- behaviours (Feast, Orrell, Russell, Charlesworth, & Moniz‐Cook, 2017;
tress. These models point to the pathways by which cultural, Losada, Márquez‐González, Peñacoba, & Romero‐Moreno, 2010). Similar
contextual, or personal factors and stressors affect caregivers' mental findings have been found in other caregiving populations. For example,
and physical health. According to this model, stressors such as care guilt has been found to be associated with long‐term reports of distress
recipients' behavioural and psychological symptoms of dementia among relatives of patients with schizophrenia (Boye, Bentsen, & Malt,

Clin Psychol Psychother. 2017;1–6. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/cpp Copyright © 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 1
2 LOSADA A. ET AL.

2002), and with psychological distress and mental, social, and physical
functioning among caregivers of cancer survivors (Spillers, Wellisch, Kim, Key Practitioner Message
Matthews, & Baker, 2008). Although research on guilt feelings among
dementia caregivers has increased in recent years (Feast et al., 2017; • Care‐recipients' disruptive behaviours increase

Gallego‐Alberto, Losada, Márquez‐González, Romero‐Moreno, & Vara, caregivers' ambivalent feelings.

2016; Roach, Laidlaw, Gillanders, & Quinn, 2013; You & Tak, 2014), few • Having ambivalent feelings in turn leads to stronger guilt
studies have analysed potential predictors of guilt or the mechanisms feelings in caregivers.
through which guilt may affect caregiver distress. • This process appears to significantly contribute to
A possible source of guilt may be specific emotional experiences. In caregivers' depressive symptomatology.
this article, we examine the relationship between the experience of
ambivalence and guilt feelings. In the caregiving context, ambivalence
is defined as the simultaneous experience of positive and negative feel- healthcare centres in Madrid, Spain. In order to be eligible for participa-
ings toward the care recipient (Fingerman, Pitzer, Lefkowitz, Birditt, & tion in the study, individuals were required to meet several criteria in
Mroczek, 2008; Willson, Shuey, & Elder, 2003). Research has focused order to ensure that the caregiving role has been established: (a) iden-
almost exclusively on caregivers' negative feelings or (to a lesser extent) tify themselves as the principal person taking care of a relative diag-
positive feelings, rather than on co‐occurrence of both types (Cohen, nosed with dementia; (b) devote at least 1 hr a day to the care of the
Colantonio, & Vernich, 2002). However, it is not uncommon to find relative; and (c) have provided care for at least 3 months to that
caregivers who report experiencing positive and negative feelings relative.
simultaneously, an internal conflict that is typically perceived as aver- These criteria are similar to those used in other studies analysing
sive by the individual (Pillemer et al., 2007). Ambivalence is especially caregivers, such as those considered by Perlick, Hohenstein, Clarkin,
likely when disruptive behaviours by the person with dementia are fre- Kaczynski, and Rosenbheck (2005), who consider as primary caregivers
quent and stressful for the caregiver (Pillemer & Suitor, 2005). those who satisfy at least three of the following criteria: Is the spouse,
Although the existence of ambivalent feelings among caregivers parent, or spouse equivalent; has the most frequent contact with the
has been recognized (Shim, Barroso, & Davis, 2012), few studies have patient; helps to support the care‐recipient financially; has most fre-
explored this construct among caregivers. Further, most existing quently been a collateral in the care‐recipients' treatment; and is
research is qualitative (Harding & Higginson, 2001; Shim et al., 2012), contacted by treatment staff in case of emergency. All of the partici-
although several quantitative studies exist that have measured ambiv- pants gave their consent to participate in the study, which was
alence indirectly by assessing positive and negative feelings toward the approved by the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness
care recipient separately, and then calculating an ambivalence score by and the Ethics Committee of the Universidad Rey Juan Carlos.
combining both dimensions (Iecovich, 2014; Willson et al., 2003).
Recently, Losada, Pillemer, Márquez‐González, Romero‐Moreno,
and Gallego‐Alberto (2016) developed and tested the Caregiving
2.2 | Variables and instruments
Ambivalence Scale. Using a sample of 401 dementia caregivers, the 2.2.1 | Sociodemographic variables
investigators found that ambivalent feelings (e.g., feeling both satisfac- Caregivers' gender, age, relationship to the care‐recipient, time since
tion and resentment due to caregiving) contributed significantly to beginning caregiving, and daily hours devoted to caregiving were
caregivers' depressive and anxious symptoms, controlling for other rel- assessed.
evant dimensions of stress and coping. However, their study did not
examine the specific mechanisms through which ambivalent feelings
2.2.2 | Disruptive behaviours
may contribute to caregivers' distress.
The frequency of occurrence of disruptive behaviours and the degree
The aim of this study was to analyse the role of ambivalent and guilt
to which these behaviours were rated as stressful by the caregivers
feelings in depressive symptomatology, using theoretical framework
were assessed through the 8‐item disruptive behaviours frequency
based on stress and coping model, The hypotheses were the following:
and reaction subscales of the Revised Memory and Behaviour Prob-
(a) Caring for a loved one with high disruptive behaviours will be associ-
lems Checklist (Teri et al., 1992). The internal consistency (Cronbach's
ated with a increased reporting of ambivalent feelings; (b) reporting
α) for the frequency and appraisal subscales in this sample was .67 for
simultaneous positive and negative feelings toward the care recipient will
each subscale.
be associated with increased feelings of guilt; and (c) guilt feelings will in
turn be associated with increased reports of depressive symptoms.
2.2.3 | Ambivalence
Caregivers' ambivalent feelings were assessed through the Caregiving
Ambivalence Scale (Losada et al., 2016). This 5‐item scale assesses
2 | METHODS
the degree to which caregivers' attitudes and feelings toward their rel-
atives are mixed or conflicted (e.g., “I have mixed feelings toward my
2.1 | Participants relative (tenderness‐rage; love‐hate, etc).”). The answers are rated on
In‐person interviews were conducted with 212 dementia family care- a Likert‐type scale that ranges from 0 (never) to 3 (always). The internal
givers. All respondents were recruited through social agencies and consistency (Cronbach's α) for this scale in this study was .87.
LOSADA A. ET AL. 3

2.2.4 | Guilt (41%) or a parent (49.1%). Participants were either taking care of a rel-
The Caregiver Guilt Questionnaire (Losada et al., 2010) was used to ative with Alzheimer disease (71.1%) or related disorder (29.9%) and
assess caregivers' feelings of guilt. It is a 22‐item scale (e.g., “I have felt had been providing care for a mean of 3.73 years (SD = 2.91), devoting
guilty about the way I've sometimes behaved with my relative”) with on average 13.19 daily hours to caregiving (SD = 8.30).
response options ranging from 0 (never) to 4 (always or almost always).
In this study, the internal consistency for this scale (Cronbach's α)
3.2 | Correlations
was .92.
The associations between the assessed variables are shown in Table 1.
As hypothesized, caregivers who reported higher frequency and reac-
2.2.5 | Depressive symptomatology
tion to disruptive behaviours reported higher ambivalent feelings
Caregivers' depressive symptoms were measured by the Center for
(Hypothesis 1), those who reported higher ambivalence scores also
Epidemiologic Studies—Depression Scale (Radloff, 1977). The Center
reported high guilt scores (Hypothesis 2), and high scores on ambiva-
for Epidemiologic Studies—Depression is a 20‐item scale that assesses
lence and guilt were associated with higher scores in depressive symp-
the frequency of depressive symptomatology during the past week
tomatology (Hypothesis 3). Other significant correlations were found
(e.g., “I felt that everything I did was an effort”). Response options range
between daily hours spent caring and frequency and reaction to dis-
from 0 (rarely or none of the time) to 3 (most or all of the time). The internal
ruptive behaviours and guilt and depressive symptomatology. In addi-
consistency (Cronbach's α) for this scale in this study was .89.
tion, being younger and caring for parents were associated with
higher ambivalent feelings and guilt, and female caregivers showed
2.3 | Data analyses higher scores on ambivalent feelings and depressive symptomatology.

Descriptive data (mean, standard deviations, ranges, and frequencies)


were calculated for the assessed variables. To analyse the relationship 3.3 | Path analysis
between variables, correlation analyses were conducted.
As shown in Figure 1, when all the assessed variables were considered,
Path analysis was employed to determine the goodness‐of‐fit of
being female, being older, more daily hours devoted to caregiving, and
the model testing the degree to which ambivalent and guilt feelings
having guilt feelings were directly associated with caregivers' depres-
are related to depressive symptomatology. Based on the stress and
sive symptomatology.
coping model adapted to caregiving (e.g., Haley et al., 1987), the fol-
The direct associations between frequency of and reaction to dis-
lowing dimensions of the model were considered: contextual variables
ruptive behaviours and ambivalent feelings with depressive symptom-
(caregivers' age, gender, and kinship), demands or stressors (frequency
atology, which were significant in the correlational analysis, were no
of disruptive behaviours), appraisal of demands as stressful (reaction to
longer significant once all the assessed variables were considered in
disruptive behaviours), and potential mediators between appraisals
the model. The results of the bootstrap analysis for testing mediation
and distress (ambivalence and guilt). The dependent variable was
showed that the indirect effect of frequency of disruptive behaviours
depressive symptomatology.
(standardized indirect effect = .08; p < .01; SE = .018; 95% CI = 0.05–
As a first step, all the associations between variables that were
0.11) and reaction to disruptive behaviours (standardized indirect
found to be significant in the correlation analyses were established.
effect = .10; p < .01; SE = .021; 95% CI = 0.06–0.14) on depressive
Next, following the model‐generating strategy (Joreskog, 1993), only
symptomatology were significant. These findings suggest that
those significant associations between variables that were observed
reporting a higher number of disruptive behaviours was associated
once the model was run were included in the final model. In addition
with the chances of perceiving them as stressful, and both factors were
to the chi‐square (χ2) statistic, the chi‐square value divided by the
related to the experience of ambivalent feelings and guilt by the care-
degrees of freedom (χ2/df) was considered, with values under or near
givers. These links were significantly associated with higher scores in
3 indicating good model fit (Bollen, 1989). The root mean square error
depressive symptomatology.
of approximation (RMSEA), the comparative fit index (CFI), and the
Regarding the specific effect of ambivalent feelings on depressive
Tucker–Lewis index (TLI) were also assessed, considering Hu and
symptomatology, the results of the bootstrap analysis for testing medi-
Bentler's (1998) indications of values under .06 (RMSEA) and over
ation suggest that the indirect effect of ambivalent feelings on depres-
.95 (CFI and TLI) as indicating excellent fit of the data to the model.
sive symptomatology through guilt was significant (standardized
Mediation analysis following Preacher and Hayes's (2004) recom-
indirect effect = .23; p < .01; SE = .037; 95% CI = 0.17–0.29). Thus, it
mended bootstrapping approach was conducted, using 1,000 boot-
appears that the association of ambivalent feelings with depressive
strap samples.
symptoms was not direct. More intense ambivalent feelings were asso-
ciated with higher guilt feelings, and this link was associated with care-
givers' levels of depressive symptomatology.
3 | RESULTS
The obtained model explained 18% of the variance of ambivalent
feelings, 41% of guilt feelings, and 35% of caregivers' depressive symp-
3.1 | Descriptive data tomatology. The obtained fit indexes suggest an excellent fit of the
The study sample had a mean age of 61.40 years (SD = 14.36) and model to the data (χ2 = 39.72; p = .11; χ2/df = 1.32; RMSEA = .039;
included mostly women (77.6%). Most participants cared for a spouse CFI = .986, and TLI = .979).
4 LOSADA A. ET AL.

TABLE 1 Correlation matrix and means, standard deviations, and ranges of the assessed variables
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

1—Gender (1 = female)
2—Caregiver age −.19**
3—Caring for parents (1 = yes) .19** −.65**
4—Time since being a caregiver .01 .20** −.15*
5—Daily hours caring −.04 .45** −.38** .00
6—Frequency disruptive behaviours .17* −.06 .08 .31** −.04
7—Reaction to disruptive behaviours .17* −.09 .14* .20** −.07 .83**
8—Guilt .07 −.27** .33** −.02 −.18* .25** .29**
9—Ambivalent feelings .18** −.15* .15* −.01 −.07 .32** .43** .62**
10—Depressive symptomatology .29** .13 −.05 .00 .29** .24** .23** .32** .30**
Mean 61.40 3.73 13.19 6.97 5.51 25.05 4.83 21.63
SD 14.36 2.91 8.30 5.52 5.16 14.17 3.51 12.20
Range 21–88 0.3–20 1–24 0–27 0–25 0–65 0–15 1–48

*p < .05;
**p < .01.

FIGURE 1 Path analysis testing the role of


ambivalence and guilt in the caregiving
process. Note. All associations are significant
(p < .05). The errors have been omitted for
ease of presentation

4 | DISCUSSION (e.g., disruptive behaviours) on caregiving consequences vary depend-


ing on resource variables (e.g., Haley et al., 1987).
The goal of this study was to analyse the role of ambivalence and guilt Moderate to strong associations were found between disruptive
feelings as contributors to caregivers' depressive symptomatology. behaviours, ambivalence, guilt, and depressive symptomatology, sug-
Although the associations between depressive symptoms and ambiva- gesting that the contribution of ambivalence and guilt to understand-
lence (Losada et al., 2016) and guilt feelings (Losada et al., 2010; Roach ing caregivers' distress is significant. A significant percentage of
et al., 2013) have been reported in previous research, this study offers variance of ambivalence, guilt, and depressive symptomatology was
the first evidence of the pathways through which they are linked with explained through the variables included in the tested model.
caregivers' depressive symptomatology. Specifically, we found that the Consistent with previous research, our results show that being
significant correlation between frequency of and reaction to disruptive female and providing a higher number of hours of care per day are asso-
behaviours with caregivers' depressive symptomatology became insig- ciated with higher depressive symptoms (Covinsky et al., 2003; Pinquart
nificant when other variables (ambivalence and guilt feelings) were & Sörensen, 2006). In addition, compared with individuals caring for
controlled. These results suggest that the association between these spouses, younger caregivers (Spillers et al., 2008) and those caring for
variables is indirect, mediated through ambivalence and guilt. That is, parents show higher levels of guilt (Romero‐Moreno et al., 2014) and
caregivers' ambivalent feelings are higher when caregivers face disrup- ambivalence. Interestingly, those providing care for more hours daily
tive behaviours (Hypothesis 1), and, in turn, having ambivalent feelings reported lower guilt scores but higher depressive symptomatology. Indi-
is related to stronger guilt feelings in caregivers (Hypothesis 2). This viduals providing more hours of care may perceive that they are behav-
process is significantly associated with caregivers' depressive symp- ing consistently with their values that families should provide as much
tomatology (Hypothesis 3). The findings provide support for the stress care as possible. Nevertheless, they may also feel more depressed due
and coping model, which postulates that the relationship of stressors to the activity restriction resulting from the time spent providing care.
LOSADA A. ET AL. 5

These results have several implications for practice. Of particular symptomatology supports a potential etiological pathway for the relation-
note is the finding that care recipients' disruptive behaviours are associ- ship between these variables and depression. The results suggest that
ated with caregivers' ambivalent feelings. Although most caregivers will- ambivalence and guilt feelings are relevant variables for understanding
ingly provide care to a loved one, the occurrence of disruptive behaviours caregivers' distress and may be important targets for interventions aimed
may also generate negative feelings such as rejection, shame, or anger. at helping caregivers to cope with caring for a relative with dementia.
Such feelings are uncomfortable and distressing for caregivers. However,
they typically emerge that in the context of a close relationship, positive ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
affect and love toward the person with dementia are also present. The We thank all the caregivers for their participation in the study and also
co‐occurrence of negative feelings toward the care recipient because the following centres for collaborating with us in the project: Centros
of these disruptive behaviours and the positive feelings associated with de día SARquavitae, Fundación María Wolff, Centro de Salud General
long‐standing bonds and attachment produce the experience of ambiva- Ricardos, Centro de Salud García Noblejas, Centro de Salud Benita
lence. In turn, caregivers experience guilt about the experience of such de Ávila, Centro de Salud Vicente Muzas, Centro Reina Sofía de Cruz
mixed (rather than positive) feelings. Roja, Unidad de Memoria de Cantoblanco, Asociación de Familiares
The inclusion of strategies for addressing and managing ambiva- de Alzheimer de Alcorcón, and Centro de Psicología Aplicada de la
lence and guilt feelings in interventions for caregivers may increase Universidad Autónoma de Madrid.
the effects of available interventions for reducing caregivers' levels of
distress (e.g., Logsdon, McCurry, & Teri, 2007; Losada et al., 2015). RE FE RE NC ES
Therapeutic strategies that focused on compassion (Gilbert, 2009), Bollen, K. A. (1989). A new incremental fit index for general structural
both for others and for self (self‐compassion), may also be useful inter- equation models. Sociological Methods and Research, 17, 303–316.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0049124189017003004
ventions to reduce guilt feelings and ambivalence.
Boye, B., Bentsen, H., & Malt, U.F. (2002). Does guilt proneness predict
Several limitations of the study should be mentioned. First, the
acute and long-term distress in relatives of patients with schizophre-
cross‐sectional design does not allow for causal inferences to be made. nia?. Acta Psychiatryca Scandinavica, 106, 351–357. https://doi.org/
We cannot reject the possibility that the direction of the observed asso- 10.1034/j.1600‐0447.2002.02276.x
ciations may be reversed. For example, it is possible that caregivers' Cherry, M. G., Taylor, P. J., Brown, S. L., Rigby, J. W., & Sellwood, W. (2017).
depressive symptomatology increases the reporting of ambivalence and Guilt, shame and expressed emotion in carers of people with long‐term
mental health difficulties: A systematic review. Psychiatry Research,
guilt feelings. Second, although the sample size of the study is acceptable
249, 139–151. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2016.12.056
(bearing in mind the difficulties in accessing this population), it does not
Coen, R. F., O'Boyle, C. A., Coakley, D., & Lawlor, B. A. (2002). Individual quality
allow us to test the influence of specific variables that have been shown of life factors distinguishing low‐burden and high‐burden caregivers of
to play a significant role in similar studies of caregivers. For example, kin- dementia patients. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 13, 164–170.
ship affects caregivers' distress (e.g., Romero‐Moreno et al., 2014; Viñas‐ Cohen, C. A., Colantonio, A., & Vernich, L. (2002). Positive aspects of care-
Diez et al., 2017), but this variable has not been controlled in this study. giving: Rounding out the caregiver experience. International Journal of
Geriatric Psychiatry, 17, 184–188. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.561
Similarly, in this study, data were available from the disruptive behav-
Covinsky, K. E., Newcomer, R., Fox, P., Wood, J., Sands, L., Dane, K., &
iours subscale of the Revised Memory and Behaviour Problems Checklist
Yaffe, K. (2003). Patient and caregiver characteristics associated with
(Teri et al., 1992), which measures the symptoms that are most depression in caregivers of patients with dementia. Journal of the
distressing for the caregivers (Fauth & Gibbons, 2014). Future studies General Internal Medicine, 18, 1006–1014.
should analyse if the obtained results are also valid for other behavioural Fauth, E. B., & Gibbons, A. (2014). Which behavioral and psychological
symptoms of dementia are the most problematic? Variability by preva-
and psychological symptoms of dementia such as depressive symptoms
lence, intensity, distress ratings, and associations with caregiver
or memory problems. In addition, dementia family caregivers also show depressive symptoms. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 29,
low levels of quality of life (Coen, O'Boyle, Coakley, & Lawlor, 2002) 263–271. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.4002
and high levels of anxiety (Joling et al., 2015). Further, emotion‐regulation Feast, A., Orrell, M., Russell, I., Charlesworth, G., & Moniz‐Cook, E. (2017). The
variables have been found to influence in caregivers' anxiety levels, such contribution of caregiver psychosocial factors to distress associated with
behavioural and psychological symptoms in dementia. International Jour-
as emotional intelligence (Weaving, Orgeta, Orrell, & Petrides, 2014) and
nal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 32, 76–85. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.4447
rumination (Romero‐Moreno, Losada, Márquez‐González, & Mausbach,
Fingerman, K. L., Pitzer, L., Lefkowitz, E. S., Birditt, K. S., & Mroczek, D.
2016). Future studies should also analyse the association between (2008). Ambivalent relationship qualities between adults and their
ambivalence, guilt feelings, and emotion‐regulation variables in dementia parents: Implications for the well‐being of both parties. Journals of
Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 63, 362–371.
family caregivers. In addition, it has been suggested that guilt has a salient
impact on expressed emotion (e.g., emotional overinvolvement; Cherry, Gallego‐Alberto, L., Losada, A., Márquez‐González, M., Romero‐Moreno, R.,
& Vara, C. (2016). Commitment to personal values and guilt feelings in
Taylor, Brown, Rigby, & Sellwood, 2017), and expressed emotion appears dementia caregivers. International Psychogeriatrics, 29, 57–65. https://
to predict negative care recipients' behaviours over time (Vitaliano, doi.org/10.1017/S104161021600139
Young, Russo, Romano, & Magana‐Amato, 1993). Therefore, analysing Gilbert, P. (2009). Introducing compassion‐focused therapy. Advances in
the impact of guilt, ambivalence, and expressed emotion in the caregiving Psychiatric Treatment., 15, 199–208. https://doi.org/10.1192/apt.
bp.107.005264
process may provide useful information regarding the design and imple-
Gonyea, J. G., Paris, R., & de Saxe Zerden, L. (2008). Adult daughters and
mentation of interventions for caregivers.
aging mothers: The role of guilt in the experience of caregiver burden.
Despite these limitations, this first study to analyse the association Aging & Mental Health, 12, 559–567. https://doi.org/10.1080/
of caregivers' ambivalence and guilt feelings with depressive 13607860802343027
6 LOSADA A. ET AL.

Haley, W. E., Levine, E. G., Brown, S. L., & Bartolucci, A. A. (1987). Stress, Radloff, L. (1977). The CES‐D scale a self‐report depression scale for
appraisal, coping, and social support as predictors of adaptational out- research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement,
come among dementia caregivers. Psychology and Aging, 2, 323–330. 1, 385–401. https://doi.org/10.1177/014662167700100306
https://doi.org/10.1037/0882‐7974.2.4.323 Roach, L., Laidlaw, K., Gillanders, D., & Quinn, K. (2013). Validation of the
Harding, R., & Higginson, I. (2001). Working with ambivalence: Informal caregiver guilt questionnaire (CGQ) in a sample of British dementia
caregivers of patients at the end of life. Support Care Cancer, 9, caregivers. International Psychogeriatrics, 25, 2001–2010. https://doi.
642–645. https://doi.org/10.1007/s005200100286 org/10.1017/S1041610213001506
Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1998). Fit indices in covariance structure modeling: Robinson‐Whelen, S., Tada, Y., MacCallum, R. C., McGuire, L., & Kiecolt‐
Sensitivity to underparameterized model misspecification. Psychologial Glaser, J. K. (2001). Long‐term caregiving: What happens when it ends?
Methods, 3, 424–453. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082‐989X.3.4.424 Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 573–584. https://doi.org/
Iecovich, E. (2014). The Association Between Older Israelis' Quality of Rela- 10.1037/0021‐843X.110.4.573
tionships With Their Family and Migrant Live‐in Caregivers and Their Romero‐Moreno, R., Losada, A., Marquez, M., Laidlaw, K., Fernández‐
Loneliness. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci., 71, 526–537. https://doi. Fernández, V., Nogales‐González, C., & López, J. (2014). Leisure, gender,
org/10.1034/10.1093/geronb/gbu169 and kinship in dementia caregiving: Psychological vulnerability of caregiv-
Joling, K.J., van Marwijk, H.W., Veldhuijzen, A.E., van der Horst, H.E., ing daughters with feelings of guilt. Journals of Gerontology Psychological
Scheltens, P., Smit, F., & van Hout, H.P. (2015). The two‐year incidence Sciences, 69, 502–513. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbt027
of depression and anxiety disorders in spousal caregivers of persons Romero‐Moreno, R., Losada, A., Márquez‐González, M., & Mausbach, B. T.
with dementia: who is at the greatest risk?. American Journal of Geriatric (2016). Stressors and anxiety in dementia caregiving: Multiple media-
Psychiatry, 23, 293–303. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jagp.2014.05.005 tion analysis of rumination, experiential avoidance, and leisure.
Joreskog, K. G. (1993). Testing structural equation models. In K. A. Bollen, & International Psychogeriatrics, 28, 1835–1844. https://doi.org/
J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 294–316). 10.1017/S1041610216001009
Newbury Park: Sage. Sallim, A. B., Sayampanathan, A. A., Cuttilan, A., & Ho, R. C. M. (2015). Prev-
Kugler, K., & Jones, W. H. (1992). On conceptualizing and assessing guilt. alence of mental health disorders among caregivers of patients with
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 318–327. https://doi. Alzheimer disease. Journal of the American Directors Association, 16,
org/10.1037/0022‐3514.62.2.318 1034–1041. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jamda.2015.09.007

Logsdon, R. G., McCurry, S. M., & Teri, L. (2007). Evidence‐based psycho- Shim, B., Barroso, J., & Davis, L. L. (2012). A comparative qualitative analysis
logical treatments for disruptive behaviors in individuals with of stories of spousal caregivers of people with dementia: Negative,
dementia. Psychology and Aging, 22, 28–36. https://doi.org/10.1037/ ambivalent, and positive experiences. International Journal of Nursing
0882‐7974.22.1.28 Studies, 49, 220–229. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2011.09.003

Losada, A., Márquez‐González, M., Peñacoba, C., & Romero‐Moreno, R. Spillers, R. L., Wellisch, D. K., Kim, Y., Matthews, A., & Baker, F. (2008).
(2010). Development and validation of the Caregiver Guilt Question- Family caregivers and guilt in the context of cancer care. Psychoso-
naire. International Psychogeriatrics, 22, 650–660. https://doi.org/ matics, 49, 511–519.
10.1017/S1041610210000074 Teri, L., Truax, P., Logsdon, R., Uomoto, J., Zarit, S., & Vitaliano, P.‐P. (1992).
Losada, A., Marquez‐Gonzalez, M., Romero‐Moreno, R., Mausbach, B. T., Assessment of behavioral problems in dementia: The revised memory
Lopez, J., Fernandez‐Fernandez, V., & Nogales‐Gonzalez, C. (2015). and behavior problems checklist. Psychology and Aging, 7, 622–631.
Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) versus acceptance and commitment https://doi.org/10.1037/0882‐7974.7.4.622
therapy (ACT) for dementia family caregivers: Results of a randomized Viñas‐Diez, V., Turró‐Garriga, O., Portellano‐Ortiz, C., Gascón‐Bayarri, J.,
clinical trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 83, 760–772. Reñé‐Ramírez, R., Garre‐Olmo, J., & Conde‐Sala, J. L. (2017). Kinship
https://doi.org/10.1037/ccp0000028 and cohabitation in relation to caregiver burden in the context of
Losada, A., Pillemer, K., Márquez‐González, M., Romero‐Moreno, R., & Alzheimer's disease: A 24‐month longitudinal study. International
Gallego‐Alberto, L. (2016). Measuring ambivalent feelings in dementia Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.4656
family caregivers: The caregiving ambivalence scale. The Gerontologist. Vitaliano, P. P., Young, H. M., Russo, J., Romano, J., & Magana‐Amato, A.
https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/gnw144 (1993). Does expressed emotion in spouses predict subsequent prob-
Perlick, D.A., Hohenstein, J.M., Clarkin, J.F., Kaczynski, R., & Rosenheck, R. lems among care recipients with Alzheimer's disease? Journal of
A. (2005). Use of mental health and primary care services by caregivers Gerontology, 48, 202–209. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronj/48.4.P202
of patients with bipolar disorder: a preliminary study. Bipolar Disorders, Weaving, J., Orgeta, V., Orrell, M., & Petrides, K. V. (2014). Predicting anx-
7, 126–135. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1399‐5618.2004.00172.x iety in carers of people with dementia: The role of trait emotional
Pillemer, K., & Suitor, J. J. (2005). Ambivalence in intergenerational relations intelligence. International Psychogeriatrics, 26, 1201–1209. https://doi.
over the life‐course. In M. Silverstein, R. Giarrusso, & V. L. Bengtson org/10.1017/S1041610214000404
(Eds.), Intergenerational relations across time and place (pp. 3–28). New Willson, A. E., Shuey, K. M., & Elder, G. H. (2003). Ambivalence in the rela-
York: Springer. tionship of adult children to aging parents and in‐laws. Journal of
Pillemer, K., Suitor, J. J., Mock, S. E., Sabir, M., Pardo, T. B., & Sechrist, J. Marriage and the Family, 65, 1055–1072. https://doi.org/10.1111/
(2007). Capturing the complexity of intergenerational relations: Explor- j.1741‐3737.2003.01055.x
ing ambivalence within later‐life families. The Journal of Social Issues, 63, You, S. Y., & Tak, Y. R. (2014). Feelings of guilt and depression in family
775–791. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540‐4560.2007.00536.x caregivers after nursing home placement of older adults. Journal of
Pinquart, M., & Sörensen, S. (2003). Differences between caregivers and Korean Gerontological Nursing, 16, 276–287. https://doi.org/
noncaregivers in psychological health and physical health: A meta‐anal- 10.17079/jkgn.2014.16.3.276
ysis. Psychology and Aging, 18, 250–267. https://doi.org/10.1037/
0882‐7974.18.2.250
Pinquart, M., & Sörensen, S. (2006). Gender differences in caregiver
How to cite this article: Losada A, Márquez‐González M, Vara‐
stressors, social resources, and health: An updated metaanalysis. Journal García C, Gallego‐Alberto L, Romero‐Moreno R, Pillemer K.
of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 61, 33–45. https://doi.org/ Ambivalence and guilt feelings: Two relevant variables for under-
10.1093/geronb/61.1.P33
standing caregivers' depressive symptomatology. Clin Psychol
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimat-
Psychother. 2017;1–7. https://doi.org/10.1002/cpp.2116
ing indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research
Methods, 36, 717–731. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03206553

You might also like