You are on page 1of 17

Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, Vol. 22, No. 2, pp.

143–159, 1998
䉷 1998 Elsevier Science Ltd
Pergamon Printed in Great Britain. All rights reserved
0149-7634/98/$32.00 + .00

PII: S0149-7634(97)00008-0

Age, Experience and the Changing Brain

BRYAN KOLB*, MARGARET FORGIE, ROBBIN GIBB, GRAZYNA GORNY AND SHARON ROWNTREE

Department of Psychology, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, Alberta T1K 3M4, Canada

KOLB, B., M. FORGIE, R. GIBB, G. GORNY, S. ROWNTREE. Age, experience and the changing brain. NEUROSCI BIOBEHAV
REV 22(2) 143–159, 1998.—In this review, various aspects of how environmental experience affects the structure of the cortex at
different times in the age of the animal are summarized. The interactions of brain injury and sex on the age-dependent plastic changes in
the cortex are also considered. Finally, we have attempted to reach some general conclusions that describe the effects of age, experience,
sex, and injury on the cortex. 䉷 1998 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.

Cortex Enriched experience Sex differences Gonadal hormones Cortical development Aging Spines Dendrites

1. INTRODUCTION experiences has provided a rich broth of information that is


IN PRINCIPLE, there are two ways that experience could relevant both to basic neurobiological theories of brain
alter the brain: either by modifying existing circuitry or by function as well as to general theories of behavioral
creating novel circuitry (63). It is reasonable to suppose that organization. The goal of the current review is to illustrate
the brain makes use of both strategies, although the details some of principles that have emerged. The review will begin
of the particular strategy will likely vary with the age of the with a summary of some of experience-dependent changes
animal. Indeed, during development of the brain all circuitry in the intact brain followed by a consideration of the effects
is, by definition, novel. One way to examine the experience- of manipulating factors such as gonadal hormones or
dependent changes in the brain is to look at the effects of neurotrophins and the effects of cortical injury.
different experiences on neuronal structure and function.
For psychologists, this rationale usually means adopting one
2. ASSUMPTIONS
of two approaches: either animals are placed in differential
environments such as so-called ‘‘enriched environments’’ As we begin, we must first admit to several biases. Firstly,
versus ‘‘impoverished environments’’; or animals are we assume that the structural properties of the brain are
trained in specific types of tasks, such as mazes. In either important in understanding its function. Although such an
paradigm, the experience is correlated with some measure assumption is self-evident to most neuroscientists, it is not
of structure such as brain weight or dendritic extent (e.g. as ubiquitously assumed by psychologists who do not study
(17)). These experiments generally show that particular the brain (e.g. (69,83)). An important corollary of this
experiences embellish circuitry relative to the absence of assumption is that changes in the structural properties of
experience, which fails to do so. Although this type of the brain reflect changes in the function of neural circuits.
experimental psychological approach would appear to have Secondly, we assume both that the mechanisms of corti-
considerable appeal in understanding experience-dependent cal plasticity are most likely to be found at the synapse and
changes in the brain, the impact of this type of research has that synaptic changes can be measured by analysis of either
been surprisingly limited. Indeed, Purves (63) noted that for pre- or post-synaptic structure. Traditionally, the emphasis
reasons that are as much sociological as scientific, the in the literature on synaptic plasticity has been upon the
experimental neuropsychological perspective has not been presynaptic, or axonal terminal side. For example, in the
embraced generally by most neurobiologists and that these studies of the effects of unilateral entorhinal cortex in rats,
psychological experiments are rarely referred to in the various investigators have shown a major reorganization of
mainstream literature. Oddly, again for reasons that are the remaining hippocampal afferents (e.g. (85)). Similar
both sociological as much as scientific, the importance of inferences have been made in other models, such as in
studies of enriched experience also have had limited impact studies of cholinergic outgrowth after cortical injury (e.g.
in mainstream psychology where there has been a long- (6)), collateral sprouting after peripheral nerve crush (7),
standing bias against structural interpretations of psycho- and terminal sprouting after various types of central injuries
logical phenomena. Nevertheless, the study of experience- (e.g. (12)). One difficulty with studying presynaptic changes
dependent changes in experiments that manipulate external is that they are very difficult to locate unless one knows a

*Corresponding author. Tel.: 403-329-2405; Fax: 403-329-2555; E-mail Kolb@HG.ULETH.CA.

143
144 KOLB ET AL.

priori where to look. In addition, once found, they are morphology was so changeable in absence of any particular
difficult to quantify. The ability to quantify specific mor- training. One could reasonably expect even greater change
phological features is critical if one is to correlate structural in an animal that was given special somatosensory-related
change with behavior. training or that had peripheral nerve injury.
An alternative way to look at synaptic change is to study Thirdly, although the emphasis in most studies of
the postsynaptic or dendritic side. This requires that the structure–function relationships falls upon the analysis of
complete cell body and dendritic tree be stained, such as neurons, there are solid grounds for looking at changes in
in a Golgi-type stain. Since the dendritic surface receives the structure and number of glial cells. Glial cells play an
more than 95% of the synapses on a neuron it is, therefore, important role in synaptic modification, and thus, can be a
possible to infer changes in synapse number from measure- clue to the location and nature of experience-dependent
ments of dendritic extent and spine density. One clear changes in neurons and their synapses.
advantage of this measure is that one need not know a priori Fourthly, it is implicit in the foregoing discussion that
where to look since it is possible to stain, and to examine, changes in the postsynaptic structure will be visible in the
the structure of cells throughout the entire brain. In addition, light microscope. Although the final verification of the nat-
analysis requires only a light microscope (and a lot of ure of structural modification must be at the ultrastructural,
time!). Golgi analysis of the postsynaptic side has proven and thus, electron microscopic (EM) level, EM studies are
useful in several types of studies of cortical plasticity. For impractical on a large scale as they are time (and money)
example, various groups have shown that housing animals consuming, even if one knows where to look. Practically,
in ‘‘enriched’’ environments leads to increased dendritic therefore, our studies are carried out in tissue that is stained
outgrowth (e.g. (17)). Similarly, training animals in specific with a Golgi-type stain (for neurons) or with other
tasks leads to dendritic changes in specific populations of specialized histochemical procedures that identify specific
neurons (e.g. (21)). One inescapable conclusion of post- proteins, such as glial fibrillary acidic protein (GFAP), in
synaptic studies is just how plastic dendritic (and presum- glial cells.
ably synaptic) structure is. One example is especially Fifthly, the emphasis of our work and this review will be
intriguing. Purves and Hadley (64) were able to label cells on the cerebral cortex. As psychologists, our primary
in vivo in the dorsal root ganglion of mice. The dendritic interest is in cognitive function and it is our assumption
field was then mapped. The same cell was relabelled at that the changes in the cerebral cortex form the principal
different times ranging from a few days to weeks and it was mechanism for cognitive change. This assumption comes
possible to see obvious qualitative changes in dendritic from several lines of evidence. For instance, it is generally
extent, which can be taken as at least suggestive evidence agreed that the relative increase in cortical volume across
of synaptic plasticity (Fig. 1). Perhaps the most surprising mammalian evolution is associated with increased cognitive
aspect of the Purves and Hadley study was that the dendritic capacity. It follows that changes in cognitive functions in a

FIG. 1. Camera lucida reconstructions of portions of the dendritic arbors of five mouse superior cervical ganglion cells visualized at an interval of 3 months.
Changes involving both the extension and retraction of particular branches are evident. Open arrowheads show examples of branches that appear to have
retracted; asterisks mark examples of branches that have formed de novo in the interval. The black arrows indicate the appearance after the specified number
of days. (After Purves and Voyvodic (68).)
AGE, EXPERIENCE AND THE CHANGING BRAIN 145

particular mammal likely will involve changes in cortical and his colleagues who have utilized environments that are
structure or organization. Furthermore, studies of decorti- richer in visual complexity, and thus, these researchers have
cated rats show that although they are capable of a remark- focused upon analyses of structural changes in the visual
able behavioral repertoire (e.g. (94)), there is limited system (e.g. (17–19,90–93)). These two approaches are,
functional flexibility under conditions that would normally thus, complementary and will be considered in this way.
lead to marked functional and/or structural change in intact
animals (e.g. (56,96)). Finally, there are marked interspecies
differences in the details of cortical organization, such as in 3. HISTORICAL CONTEXT

Old world and New world monkeys, and it has been Although the idea that experience can modify brain struc-
assumed that these differences reflect the clear differences ture can probably be traced back at least to Ramon y Cajal
in perceptual and cognitive abilities (36). (70), it was Hebb who made this a central feature of his
Finally, since most work on cortical plasticity and neuropsychological theory (25). Hebb did the first experi-
behavior is done using rats, we have assumed that changes ments on the consequences of enriched rearing on the
in cortical structures are likely to be largest, and thus, easiest behavior of the rat (24), but it was not until the group at
to study in those structures that play a central role in somato- Berkeley began to demonstrate changes in brain weight,
sensory and motor function. Rats are nocturnal animals that cortical thickness, acetylcholine levels, and dendritic struc-
rely heavily on tactile sensitivity, especially in the represen- ture that there was any structural correlate of the behavioral
tation of the face and snout, and they have well developed changes related to experience (8,9,10,15,74,75). Later,
forelimb manipulatory abilities. In fact, rats are skilled beginning in the 1970s and continuing still, Greenough
climbers and jumpers, and are able to use their forelimbs and his colleagues initiated a multidisciplinary investigation
in ways that are strikingly similar to those seen in primates of the effects of rearing animals in visually or motorically
(95). Thus, our enriched environments are organized to enriched environments (Table 1). These experiments
allow considerable motor activity and feature many tunnels, demonstrated that not only were there changes in the
the opportunity to climb, and to manipulate objects with dendritic structure but that these changes reflected an
both the forelimbs and mouth (Fig. 2). This contrasts increase in the number of synapses that could be identified
somewhat with the extensive experiments of Greenough using EM techniques. Evidence that changes in dendritic
structure reflect changes in connectivity is important for it
implies that it is possible to make inferences about con-
nectivity from dendritic structure. More recently, Purves
and his colleagues have demonstrated that there is a direct
relationship between dendritic geometry and synaptic input
(63–67,73). These authors have shown that the number of
inputs to a sympathetic or parasympathetic ganglion in an
adult animal is directly related to the number of dendritic
branches (Fig. 3). Importantly, however, these experiments
have also shown that this relationship is not found in
newborn animals. Rather, the number of inputs to a given
neuron in young animals is about the same regardless of the

TABLE 1
PRINCIPAL CELLULAR DIFFERENCES IN THE OCCIPITAL
CORTEX BETWEEN RATS RAISED IN ENRICHED CONDITION (EC)
AND IMPOVERISHED CONDITION (IC)

Cellular variable Environment Reference

Neuron size EC ⬎ IC (9)


Neuron density IC ⬎ EC (91)
Dendritic branching EC ⬎ IC (92)
Dendritic spine density EC ⬎ IC (15)
Number of unmyelinated axons EC ⬎ IC (35)
in splenium of corpus callosum
Size of unmyelinated axons in EC ⬎ IC (35)
splenium of corpus callosum
Number of synapses per neuron EC ⬎ IC (91)
Size of synaptic contact EC ⬎ IC (93)
Synaptic plate perforations EC ⬎ IC (19)

Percentage of total tissue volume


Capillary vessels EC ⬎ IC (2)
Astrocytic nuclei EC ⬎ IC (80)
Oligodendrocytic nuclei EC ⬎ IC (80)
Mitochondria EC ⬎ IC (80)
FIG. 2. Schematic illustration of the rat condominiums used in studies of
the effects of enriched experience. After Juraska (32).
146 KOLB ET AL.

distributed. The disadvantage historically has been that the


staining is capricious and sometimes obscured with
precipitate. In recent years, there have been several modi-
fications to the procedures that have made the staining more
consistent. One assumption of Golgi studies is that the stain-
ing of neurons is random and does not reflect some specific
feature of particular neurons. That is, since only about 1% of
cells stain, it is important that those cells that are stained are
not unique in some consistent manner. Several studies have
now demonstrated the aselectivity of the Golgi procedures
(60). We have modified the Golgi-Cox procedure to allow
us to stain consistently and with a high signal-to-noise ratio
(14). In addition, our procedure allows us to visualize spines
clearly and consistently (Fig. 4).
The second type of staining procedure is to inject indi-
vidual cells with dyes that diffuse throughout the cell,
including the dendrites and axons (e.g. (61)). The advantage
of this procedure is that specific cell populations can be
labelled and the issue of stain selectivity is removed,
although there is obvious experimenter selectivity. The
FIG. 3. Correlation between input number and postsynaptic geometry disadvantages are that it is very time consuming and that
among rabbit ciliary ganglion cells, using the number of primary dendrites it is only practical if one knows a priori where one wishes to
as an index of the complexity. Each point represents the mean of measure- look at cells. However, very often in our experiments we
ments on a number of neurons ( ⫾ SE). The straight line was fitted to the
data by a least squares linear regression program and has a slope of about 1. wish to look in many regions of the brain, making this
(After Purves and Hume (65).) procedure impractical.
Once the cells are stained, the dendritic length can be
measured in several ways. Cells first are drawn using a
size of the dendritic field. It is reasonable to presume that microscope, which is typically set at 250–400 ⫻ magni-
experience plays an important role in shaping the adult fication. The drawing can be done using some type of com-
structure but the details of how this might happen are not yet puterized imaging system or it can be done by using a
known. The Purves experiments can be generalized to other camera-lucida procedure (Fig. 5) in which cells are drawn
parts of the brain, such as the cortex, where such experi- with pen and ink. The advantage of the computerized
ments are impractical because of the very large number of systems is that the precise length of all dendritic segments
inputs, which may range up towards 100 000 per neuron.
The accumulating evidence showing that experience
could modify cortical structure even in adult animals led
many investigators to turn to environmental manipulations
as a method for influencing recovery from brain injury (e.g.
(82,97)). After all, if experience could modify cortical
structure and allow enhanced performance in a variety of
cognitive tasks, then one would expect that specific envir-
onmental experiences could also enhance the reorganization
of the brain after cerebral injury. Although some studies
have shown clear beneficial effects of such treatments,
others have failed to find enhanced functional recovery.
Furthermore, it has become apparent that the direct effects
of brain injury interact not only with the experience but also
with various factors such as age and sex (see below).

4. ANALYSIS OF DENDRITIC MATERIAL

We indicated earlier that there is a high correlation


between the extent of dendritic arborization and the number
of synapses. Although synapses can only be studied (and
counted) directly with EM procedures, an estimate of
synapse number can be made by calculating the total den-
dritic length. Cells can be stained using one of two different
procedures. The oldest procedure was devised by Golgi in
the late 19th century and it involves depositing a heavy
metal, such as gold or silver, on cell bodies. A more recent
recipe, known as Golgi-Cox, uses mercury. The Golgi stains FIG. 4. Photograph of a layer-V pyramidal neuron from the parietal cortex.
have the advantage that they stain about 1% of the cells in The cell is stained with a variant of the Golgi-Cox procedure. Dendritic
the tissue and these cells are thought to be randomly spines are clearly visible.
AGE, EXPERIENCE AND THE CHANGING BRAIN 147

do so. This means that an operator must guide all of the


computer drawing. If cells are drawn using pen and ink, then
the analysis is normally done by using a procedure that
estimates dendritic length. One way is to count the total
number of dendritic branches, whereas another way is to
place some sort of grid over the drawing and to count the
number of intersections of the dendrites with the grid lines
(Fig. 6). Although the two procedures give very different
statistical views of dendritic arborization, both measures
lead to the same conclusions. For example, in a study of the
effects of ovariectomy on dendritic growth in the cortex of
rats, Stewart and Kolb (87) measured dendritic growth using
both methods (Fig. 7) and found essentially the same result:
removal of the ovaries in adult female rats led to an increase
in dendritic arborization relative to intact female controls. It
can be seen in the branching analysis that the increased
arborization was seen at the higher order branches, meaning
that it reflected growth at the end of the existing branches. It
FIG. 5. Illustration of the camera lucida method for tracing neurons. Mirrors can be seen in the Sholl analysis that the branches crossed
in the drawing tube allow the drawer to visualize both the cell through the
microscope as well as the tracing page. Cells, therefore, can be traced. more of the circles, meaning that the branches were longer
in the ovariectomized animals.
A second measure of synaptic density is to look at the
can be calculated and various statistical measurements distribution of dendritic spines on the dendrites (Fig. 8).
can be made (e.g. (4)). The disadvantage is that these About 90% of excitatory synapses are made on dendritic
semi-automated procedures are very slow and, somewhat spines so a count of the number of spines allows an estimate
paradoxically, are labor-intensive. Although only 1% of the of the number of synapses (22). Spine density varies on
neurons are stained in Golgi-type stains, the cells are still different parts of the neuron (Fig. 8), so it is typical to
close together and dendritic branches from different cells measure spine density at several places on the neuron.
overlap. The human eye can easily distinguish which Measurement of spine density requires a higher magnifica-
branches belong to which cells, but computers cannot yet tion than dendritic arbor, with most investigators using a

FIG. 6. An illustration of the two main methods of quantifying dendrites. The concentric rings on the left form the grid for the Sholl analysis. The number of
ring crossings of each branch gives an estimate of length of dendrite. The numbers on the branches on the right indicate the branch order, which offers a
different estimate of dendrite length.
148 KOLB ET AL.

exemplified by pyramidal cells in the cortex. In our initial


studies, we looked at cells in cortical layers II, III, and V but
since we have found cells in layer II to be less affected by
experience or other factors, we have focused on layers III
and V in our more recent studies. Although other investi-
gators have focused upon cells in the occipital areas, we
have focused more on cells in the somatosensory, motor
and prefrontal cortices. Our selection of the somatosensory
cortex has been guided by our recognition that rats are very
tactile animals, by the ease in identifying the primary
somatosensory cortex in animals with cortical injuries,
and by the fact that most of our lesion studies have investi-
gated the effects of prefrontal or motor cortex injuries (e.g.
(37,38)). Somatosensory cortex changes in response to these
injuries and we have been able to investigate the interaction
of these changes with various factors such as experience and
sex.

5. ANALYSIS OF GLIA

There are three main types of glia in the cortex: astro-


cytes, microglia, and oligodendroctyes. The oligodendro-
ctyes form the myelin and have not been studied with
respect to experience. The astrocytes are large glia that
are found both in the white matter as well as the grey matter
of the cortex. Many astrocytes have processes that resemble
dendrites and these processes expand in response to various
events, including experience. Microglia are normally visible
only when the brain is injured and they function as macro-
phages. Both astrocytes and microglia can be visualized by
using immunohistochemical procedures. Histological
sections are exposed to antibodies to different proteins
expressed by the cells. For example, astrocytes express pro-
teins such as glial fibrillary protein (GFAP) or vimentin and
with appropriate reactions the cells can be visualized (Fig.
9). Similarly, microglia can be located with an antibody
FIG. 7. A comparison of data summaries from Sholl and branch order known as OX-42. Once visualized, the glia can be quantified
analyses of the same neurons. Adult female rats were ovariectomized in two different ways. Astrocytes are best identified by using
(OVX) and layer-III parietal pyramidal neurons were drawn several months procedures analogous to the grid crossing systems used for
later. The branch order analysis shows that all branch orders after the first
were increased in the OVX rats. The Sholl analysis shows that the increased
dendrites. In this case, the cells are not drawn but rather a
branching was near the cell body, falling at about 30–60 m along the video camera is attached to a microscope, and under rela-
branches. (After Stewart and Kolb (86).) tively high power magnification (200–400 ⫻ ), the cells are
visualized on a monitor that has a grid superimposed. The
number of astrocytic branch crossings over the grids are
counted. When astrocytes are measured in this manner,
magnification of at least 1000 ⫻ . Dendritic segments are Greenough and his colleagues have found that there is a
drawn using camera lucida procedures, the segments consistent increase in astrocytic branching in the brains of
are measured, and the number of spines per unit length animals housed in enriched environments (23,81). The
are calculated. One of the difficulties in measuring spine simplest way to measure microglia is to use an imaging
density with standard light microscopy is that spines that lie system to measure the density of staining in different
either on the backside of the dendrite or spines that are cortical regions. Microglia are small and it is impractical
pointed directly up at the viewer cannot be seen. Thus, to count individual cells. Rather, it is more common simply
measurements of spine density with the light microscope to measure the density of staining, and to infer that there are
will necessarily underestimate the total number of spines. larger numbers of microglia present. Microglia are not
The newly developed confocal microscopes overcome this usually present in large numbers unless the brain is injured
difficulty, and thus, yield higher estimates of spine or if cells are dying for some other reason, such as in early
density. development or in dementia. Thus, they have not been
One of the decisions that must be made in measuring systematically studied in studies of experience-dependent
dendrites is which cells to measure. In any slice of brain changes. There are studies, however, showing that the
there will be multiple cell types (e.g. pyramidal versus density of microglia can be affected by experience. For
granule cells). In addition, there is the issue of which example, we have found that environmental stimulation in
brain regions to draw. In our experiments, we have chosen infancy will reduce the density of microglia observed both
to draw excitatory neurons, which are spiny and are best during infancy and in adulthood (47).
AGE, EXPERIENCE AND THE CHANGING BRAIN 149

FIG. 8. Illustrations of representative layer-III parietal pyramidal neurons from a rat placed in an enriched environment at weaning versus a littermate that
was housed in standard laboratory housing. The dendritic branches down the midline are expanded views of terminal (T), oblique (O), and basilar (B)
portions, illustrating the dendritic spines. Note that the spine density varies with location on the dendritic tree. Enriched housing produced an increase in
branching but a decrease in spine density.

6. EXPERIENCE-DEPENDENT CHANGE IN THE INTACT BRAIN

As we began our studies of experience-dependent


changes in the brain, we used the logic of those before us
who had compared animals in laboratory cages to others in
‘‘enriched environments’’ (e.g. (25,18,74)). Thus, we
placed animals in same-sex groups of 4–6 in rat condomi-
niums, which are 1 m ⫻ 0.6 m ⫻ 1.8 m high enclosures
(Fig. 2). The condominiums feature sawdust floors to allow
digging and three hardware cloth walls to allow climbing.
The enclosures contain numerous objects, tree branches,
and pieces of PVC pipe, and the objects are moved about
weekly. The animals are allowed to live in this environment
for about 90 days, at which time their brains and behavior
are compared to littermate controls that were group-housed
in standard hanging cages. In adulthood, the animals housed
in the condominiums typically have a decrease in body
weight, presumably because they are more active, and an
increase in brain weight of about 6%. Although this
increased brain weight likely reflects changes throughout
the brain, there are large effects upon cortical thickness,
with a typical increase in thickness the order of 7%.

6.1. Age, experience and dendritic changes


It has long been assumed in the psychological literature
that experiences in early childhood have greater effects
upon later behavior than do similar experiences in adult-
hood. Our analysis of dendritic changes following exposure
to enriched environments suggests that there is a structural
basis to this differential effect of early experience on
behavior. We placed rats in enriched environments for 3
months beginning either at weaning (21 days of age), at
FIG. 9. Photomicrographs illustrating staining of by antibodies specific for
young adulthood (4 months of age), or in senescence (2
basic fibroblast growth factor (bFCF) and glial fibrillary acidic protein years or older). The principal finding was that the age at
(GFAP). which animals were placed in the enriched environments
150 KOLB ET AL.

has qualitatively different effects upon dendritic structure. bulb neurons and has effects on later behavior (e.g.
Rats placed into the condominiums in young adulthood (6,57,88,98)). We therefore stroked infant rats with a
showed effects similar to those reported by others: there camel hair paintbrush three times daily from day 7 to day
was a large increase in dendritic length relative to cage- 21 of life. Animals were subsequently raised in standard lab
housed control animals (Fig. 10). In addition, there was an cages and were sacrificed in adulthood. Golgi analysis
increase in spine density (e.g. (45)). Parallel results were revealed that the early experience had no effect upon
seen in senescent animals, as they showed increases in dendritic length in adulthood but there was a significant
dendritic length and spine density relative to age-matched drop in spine density (45). These results surprised us and led
control rats (45). In contrast, when we analyzed the changes us to consider other changes in cortical morphology in the
in animals who were placed in the condominiums as tactile-stroking paradigm. For example, in one experiment
juveniles, we saw an increase in dendritic branching but a infant rats were given tactile stimulation and then were
consistent decrease in spine density. That is, the young sacrificed at different ages. At sacrifice we measured: (1) the
animals showed a qualitatively different change in the density of acetylcholinesterase staining, which allowed an
distribution of synapses on pyramidal neurons compared indirect measure of acetylcholine innervation in the cortex;
to older animals. and 2) the density of immunohistochemical staining for
The differential effect of enrichment in the young versus OX-42, which is a marker of microglia; and 3) the density of
older animals led us to look at the effects of environmental staining for GFAP, which is a marker for astrocytes. The
manipulation even earlier in the animals’ lives. It has been results showed that acetylcholinesterase levels were signifi-
shown that tactile stimulation of premature human babies cantly increased after only 7 days of stimulation and that
with a brush leads to faster growth and earlier hospital dis- this increase was still present 6 weeks after the stimulation
charge (11,79,84). In addition, studies in infant rats have was ended. Similarly, the number of microglia was signifi-
shown that similar treatment alters the structure of olfactory cantly decreased after only 7 days of stimulation and
remained lower than in controls 6 weeks later. Surprisingly,
GFAP-positive astrocytes were virtually unchanged by the
experience.
These results lead us to several conclusions. First,
‘‘enriched’’ experience can have very different effects
upon the brain at different ages. Second, experience not
only leads to ‘‘more’’ but can also lead to ‘‘less’’. That is,
although there is a temptation to presume that experiences
lead to increased numbers of synapses and probably to
increases in glia, it appears that there may be either
increases or decreases, the details varying with age at
experience. Third, changes in dendritic length and dendritic
spine density are clearly dissociable. It is not immediately
clear what the differences mean in terms of neuronal
function but it is clear that experience can alter these two
measures independently and in different ways at different
ages.

6.2. Task-dependent changes in dendritic arbor


Greenough and his colleagues probably were the first to
look at the effects of training in specific tasks on the struc-
ture of cortical neurons. In their first studies, they trained
rats in visual mazes and then analyzed the structure of
neurons in the visual cortex (e.g. (17–20,91,92)). Their
principal finding was that both pyramidal and stellate
neurons had significantly increased dendritic arbor in the
brains of trained relative to untrained control rats. In fact, in
one experiment the investigators took advantage of the fact
that the visual projections from the eyes to the visual cortex
of the rat are nearly completely crossed. Thus, input from
one eye goes largely to the contralateral hemisphere. The
authors therefore exposed one hemisphere to a visual task
by occluding the ipsilateral eye, and subsequently only one
hemisphere had the visual experience (5). As might be
expected, there was an effect of training on the visually
trained hemisphere but not on the untrained hemisphere.
FIG. 10. Summary of the effects of housing in condominiums beginning at Greenough and colleagues (21) subsequently trained
weaning (young), young adulthood (adult) or in old age (old). Enriched
housing led to an increase in dendritic branching in all groups. In contrast, animals to reach for food with a single forelimb and
spine density was decreased in the young group and increased in the adult showed growth in pyramidal neurons in layer III and V,
and old groups. (After Kolb and Gibb (45).) but not II, in the motor cortex. More recently, we trained rats
AGE, EXPERIENCE AND THE CHANGING BRAIN 151

either to reach with a single forelimb to obtain food or to (39,40,77)). This morphological plasticity may be related
pull up strings with both forelimbs to retrieve food attached to lesion size or locus, however, as we have also found that
to the end (54). We found that there was a significant large unilateral devascularizing lesions including portions
increase in dendritic branching in layer V pyramidal of motor, parietal and anterior visual cortex lead to a
neurons in the hemisphere controlling the reaching forelimb permanent atrophy of remaining cortical neurons (41).
versus the nonreaching forelimb in the unimanual task. In These animals showed little restitution of function, which
contrast, neurons in both hemispheres showed increased would be consistent with the neuronal atrophy. Two addi-
growth in the bimanual task. These changes were specific to tional factors may be important in understanding neuronal
the forelimb region of the motor cortex and were not changes after cortical injury in adulthood. First, in an
observed in motor cortex neurons located more laterally. innovative series of experiments, Jones and Schallert
We also measured spine density in these animals and found (28,29) demonstrated that rats with unilateral lesions of
no changes in any of the treatment groups. Thus, once again the forelimb region showed compensatory changes in
it appears that dendritic branch length and spine density are behavior (they favored the limb ipsilateral to the lesion)
independent processes. and that these behavioral changes were correlated with
dendritic growth in the normal hemisphere. When we
replicated these experiments we failed to find the same
6.3. Experience and the aging brain changes in the normal hemisphere (13,61) but one funda-
There have been few systematic studies of experience- mental difference was in the nature of the brain damage. In
dependent changes in the aging brain. One difficulty in put- our experiments, we surgically removed tissue whereas in
ting senescent rats that have lived their lives in laboratory the Jones and Schallert experiments, tissue was damaged in
cages into enriched environments is that the animals do not such a manner as to lead to slow neuronal death. It now
interact with the environment in the same manner as appears that slow death of neurons leads to changes in
younger animals. For example, in our rat condominiums mRNA in neurons in the intact hemisphere that are different
we have observed that whereas young or middle-aged rats from what is observed after rapid death of neurons (59).
explore the entire complex, and spend much time climbing Thus, a second factor affecting injury-induced neuronal
to the upper levels, old rats do not appear to have the plasticity is the etiology of the disease process.
motoric abilities (or inclination?) to explore the upper levels We have already noted the possibility that changes in glia
and stay on the ground level. In one experiment we were may correlate with changes in neuron morphology. One
able to identify ‘‘climbers’’ and ‘‘nonclimbers’’ and consistent effect of cortical injury is a marked proliferation
showed that climbers had significantly larger brains (45). of astrocytes in both the grey and white matter proximal to
This observation is important as it demonstrates that mere the injury. For example, when we made lesions to motor
passive exposure to experiences is not sufficient to ensure cortex, we observed a marked increase in the number of
neuronal change: there must be active interaction with the reactive astrocytes in the adjacent motor cortex, as well as
environment. This study also underscores the importance of more distal ipsilateral cortical regions, but there was no
observing the behavior of animals in specific environments detectable change in contralateral, uninjured hemisphere
for it may account for some of the individual variation in (11,77). Astrocytes also express other proteins that are
experience-induced brain changes. only observed after brain injury. For example, in post-
One interesting result in our study was seen in the com- mortem immunohistochemical studies astrocytes show
parison of the old adult animals and young adult animals virtually no reaction to antibodies specific for basic fibro-
with the same lab-cage housing. The old animals had much blast growth factor (bFGF) until the brain is injured. Then
simpler cells than did the younger animals. Curiously, a there is a dramatic expression of bFGF, which peaks 7 days
comparison of the enriched old rats and the young adults after the injury and declines to basal levels about 2 weeks
showed that the enriched housing returned the cells of the later. We return to this reaction below. The key point here is
old animals to the level of younger impoverished animals. It that like neurons, astrocytes change after cerebral injury.
would be interesting to determine if a longer period of hous-
ing might have stimulated growth similar to that seen in the
younger enriched rats. Furthermore, it would be interesting 7.2. Experience and the injured adult brain
to determine if a period of enriched housing during young
adulthood or even middle age might prevent the atrophy of 7.2.1. Changes in neurons
the neurons in the aged lab-reared rat. In view of the plastic nature of the injured brain, it is
reasonable to suppose that environmental manipulation
might influence both morphological change and functional
7. EXPERIENCE AND PLASTICITY IN THE INJURED ADULT BRAIN
outcome. Although there have been many studies of the
effects of experience on functional outcome after cerebral
7.1. Plasticity in the injured adult brain
injury, the results have been inconsistent (for reviews, see
When the cortex is damaged there are changes in the (82,97)). One difficulty with these studies is that few have
remaining cortex that are correlated with functional out- actually measured neuronal morphology but many have
come. For example, when we removed the frontal cortex focused upon functional outcome with different environ-
in adult rats, we found an initial drop in dendritic arboriza- mental manipulations. One exception is an experiment in
tion in proximal cortical regions such as parietal cortex. which we made large frontal lesions in rats and then either
This atrophy slowly resolved and 4 months later there was placed them in our rat condominiums or returned them to
a significant increase in dendritic morphology, which was their standard laboratory cages (44). After 2 months of
correlated with partial restitution of function (e.g. recovery and experience, the animals were tested on various
152 KOLB ET AL.

behavioral tasks. Our functional results were disappointing injury to the hippocampus mimics, to some extent, the
as we found only a limited benefit from the special housing immature state. This result is intriguing and leads us to
in the brain-injured animals. This result made sense, how- wonder if lesions to other regions implicated in cortical
ever, when we analyzed the neuronal morphology of the plasticity, such as the striatum, might produce parallel
animals. We found that the enriched experience interacted effects.
with the endogenous lesion-induced changes in neuronal
morphology. Thus, the frontal lesions stimulated an increase 7.2.2. Changes in astrocytes and neurotrophins
in dendritic arborization in parietal cortex but did not affect We noted earlier that astrocytes change with experience
visual cortex. Enrichment had no additional effect on the and with cortical injury. In order to determine if lesion and
parietal neurons but stimulated growth in the occipital experience interact, we have measured astrocytic mor-
neurons. It, therefore, appears that neuronal changes phology in animals housed in our rat condominiums for a
induced by the lesion may place limits upon the environ- month prior to sustaining unilateral motor cortex lesions
ment’s capacity for further neuronal, and subsequently (76). As would be anticipated, the animals with enriched
functional, change. Specifically, once having been altered rearing showed an increased expression of GFAP but,
in response to a frontal injury, adjacent parietal neurons may unexpectedly, there was an interaction between injury,
be unable to change further in response to experience. enrichment and astrocyte reactivity. Cortically injured
However, it may be that although the total change in animals with condominium experience had an enhanced
dendritic space is similar in the enriched and lab-housed
brain-injured animals, there is a qualitative difference in the
nature of the changes. Indeed, in view of our observations
on the changes in spine density with similar experience at
different ages, one might predict differences in this measure
to vary with experience after injury. This prediction is
particularly germane because we did find a small functional
benefit from the enriched experience. Unfortunately, we did
not measure spine density in this experiment. (We shall see
in Section 7.2.2 that changes in spine density are indeed
correlated with functional outcome after enriched
experience in animals with lesions in infancy.)
There is now a long history of experiments in which
damage to the hippocampal formation is associated with
deficits in various forms of learning and memory (e.g.
(55)). It is, thus, a logical question to ask how injury to
the hippocampus might affect experience-dependent
changes in the cortex. In principle, it should be easy to
answer this question. One need simply make hippocampal
lesions and place animals in enriched environments and see
what happens. Unfortunately, there is a problem with such a
study. Hippocampal lesions produce a variety of behavioral
changes so it would be difficult to determine if any reduc-
tion in the effects of enrichment on the cortex was due to a
direct effect of the hippocampal injury preventing
experience-dependent change in the cortex or, alternatively,
it was due to a change in the behavior of the animals such
that they did not interact with the environment in the same
manner as unoperated animals. In order to solve this
problem, we made unilateral hippocampal lesions in rats
and placed them in enriched environments or standard lab
cages (89). It was our expectation that, by using this
paradigm, we would control behavioral changes since one
would anticipate that both hemispheres in the same animal
would have the same experience. Thus, any difference in the
effects of experience on the two hemispheres would be a
result of the hippocampal lesion on cortical plasticity. Our
findings were quite unexpected. First, the intact hemisphere
changed as it did in intact rats. That is, there was a growth of
dendrites and an increase in spine density. In contrast, FIG. 11. Summary of the effect of 14 days of enriched housing on the
however, the hemisphere without a hippocampus showed number of GFAP-reactive astrocytes (top) or bFGF-reactive astrocytes
an increase in dendritic length but a decrease in spine (bottom). Rats either received a sham lesion (Control) or a unilateral
density. That is, this hemisphere behaved like an immature motor cortex ablation in the forelimb region. The lesion animals were killed
7 or 21 days after the lesion. The lesions and the enrichment both increased
cerebrum. In retrospect, this effect is perfectly sensible: the the reactivity to both GFAP and bFGF antibodies. Note, however, that
developing brain has an immature hippocampus which is control animals showed no reactivity to antibodies for bFGF. (After
presumably functioning only at a rudimentary level. Hence, Rowntree (76).)
AGE, EXPERIENCE AND THE CHANGING BRAIN 153

FIG. 12. Schematic illustration of the effects of infusion of nerve growth factor (NGF) into the lateral ventricle of adult rats. Both the dendritic branching and
the spine density were increased in neurons throughout the cerebral cortex. (After Kolb et al. (50).)

expression of GFAP, but even more importantly, these arborization and a decrease in spine density in neurons
animals also had an enhanced bFGF (basic fibroblast throughout the cortical mantle (43,49). This result is
growth factor) reaction even though enriched animals with- correlated with a miserable functional outcome and is
out brain damage did not show this bFGF reaction (Fig. 11). reminiscent of the marked abnormalities in the brains of
These results suggest that prior experience may affect the retarded children (62) (see Fig. 13). In contrast, when the
nature of the astrocytic response to injury. We can speculate cortical mantle of rats is damaged in the second week of life,
that this is likely to enhance the neuronal plasticity and which corresponds to the period of rapid dendritic growth
functional outcome but this has not yet been demonstrated. and synaptic formation, there is a generalized enhancement
One of the functions of bFGF appears to be to encourage of dendritic arborization and/or spine density throughout the
the brain to produce other proteins, one of which is nerve remaining cortex (49,51). This enhanced dendritic response
growth factor (NGF). NGF is known to facilitate recovery is correlated with dramatic functional recovery. Thus, we
from certain types of brain injury. For example, Kolb et al. see that if the injury in the developing brain leads to
(41) treated rats, that had sustained large unilateral vascular increased dendritic space there is a good functional out-
injuries to the cerebral cortex, with intraventricular infu- come, whereas if the injury leads to a retarded development
sions of NGF. These animals showed enhanced recovery on of dendritic material, there is a poor functional outcome. It
motor and cognitive tasks and this recovery was associated was our expectation, therefore, that if we could potentiate
with a reversal of atrophy in cortical neurons. Furthermore, dendritic growth in the animals normally showing poor
and unexpectedly, animals with NGF treatment and no brain recovery of function, we would enhance functional
injury showed a dramatic growth in dendritic length and recovery.
spine density in cortical neurons (Fig. 12). This result is
intriguing because the animals were trained on various tasks
after the NGF infusion. Thus, it is possible that the NGF
effects reflected an increased sensitivity of the brain to
various experiences. It would be instructive to determine
the effects of NGF on condominium experience.

8. EXPERIENCE AND PLASTICITY IN THE INJURED DEVELOPING BRAIN

8.1. Plasticity in injured developing brain


FIG. 13. Representative examples of dendritic branches from cortical neu-
One of our consistent findings over the past decade has rons of children (A) and rats (B). A. The left branch is from a child of
been that the anatomical sequelae of cortical injury vary normal intelligence, whereas the right branch is from a child with mental
with precise developmental stage. In brief, when the brain retardation. B. The left branch is from an adult rat that sustained a frontal
of rats is damaged in the first few days of life, which corre- lesion at 10 days of age, whereas the right branch is from an adult rat that
sustained a frontal lesion at 1 day of age. The latter rat performed miserably
sponds to a time just after neural proliferation is complete on all behavioral tests administered. Thus, the animals with severely com-
but neural migration and differentiation are still ongoing, promised cognitive skills had dendritic branches with few spines. (After
there is a marked generalized atrophy of dendritic Purpura (62) and Kolb (39), respectively)
154 KOLB ET AL.

8.2. Experience and the injured infant brain prediction is that we would still get improved functional
We saw earlier that the young brain responds to outcome. In this case, however, we would predict enhanced
experience differently at different ages, and that it also dendritic growth and increased spine density, which would
responds to injury differently at different ages. It is, thus, be parallel to effects of enrichment in the control animals.
reasonable to presume that there will be complex inter- Testing this prediction is important for it would show that
actions between the type of experience, the time of behavioral therapy consistently improves behavioral out-
experience and the time of injury. We shall consider these come by using a consistent anatomical change, namely
interactions by considering the effects of the earliest injuries growth of synaptic space, even though in the normal
(days 1–5) before considering the effects of injuries at days brain, the response to experience appears to vary with age.
7–10. Although we have lumped together the effects of lesions
Because the animal with a cortical lesion in the first days over the first few days, there is a large behavioral difference:
of life is functionally devastated in adulthood, and because lesions on day 1 are far more debilitating than comparable
it shows atrophy of cortical neurons, we anticipated that lesions on day 5. Similarly, we have found that the effect of
such animals would benefit the most from early experience. experience varies depending on whether the lesion was on
Animals were given frontal or posterior parietal lesions at 4 day 1 or 5: animals with injuries on day 1 show a much
days of age, followed by tactile stimulation (stroking) until greater behavioral response to the experience than do
weaning. They were group-housed in laboratory cages and animals with lesions on day 5. Furthermore, although we
then tested on various tasks sensitive to frontal or parietal did not measure dendritic changes, we did find that there
injury (47). The rats with tactile stimulation showed an was an increase in cortical thickness in the younger operates
unexpectedly large attenuation of the behavioral deficits of that was more than twice as large as in the older operates,
cerebral injury as a result of this rather brief environmental which is consistent with the behavioral result (42). It, thus,
‘‘therapy’’. Analysis of the brains showed a reversal of the appears that the youngest animal with the largest behavioral
atrophy of cortical neurons normally associated with such impairment is most influenced by the experience. Presum-
early lesions, and more interestingly, a reversal of the ably the experience at least partially reverses the dendritic
decrease in spine density that is normally associated with atrophy that lead to the dismal behavioral outcome; the
the tactile stimulation (Table 2). In other words stroking greater the atrophy, the greater the potential recovery. This
leads to a decrease in spine density in normal animals but to result speaks directly to the importance of initiating
an increase in spine density in the lesion animals. Thus, it is behavioral therapies in children with cerebral injuries late
clear that environmental events can have different effects on in gestation or at birth for these are the children with the
the normal and the injured brain. We are left, of course, with worst functional outcomes after cerebral injury.
the question, ‘‘why?’’ One possibility is that the lesion Now let us consider the brain of the animal with good
differentially affects the production of something, such as functional recovery after injury in the second week of life.
astrocytes and/or growth factors, and these, in turn, influ- Our first experiments looked at animals with frontal lesions
ence the effect of experience on the remaining brain. that were placed in condominiums at weaning (47). These
Another possibility is that behavior has been changed, and animals showed a much reduced enhancement of behavioral
the change in behavior alters the experience-dependent recovery, presumably, in part, because they were not as
effects. Although this latter explanation has some appeal impaired to begin with. Indeed, on some behavioral tasks
in the older animal, it seems unlikely in the neonate that has there was really no improvement at all with the behavioral
so little behavior other than feeding and sleeping. treatment. When we analyzed the brains, we found an
In another series of experiments, we placed animals, who unexpected result. Recall that normal animals show an
had lesions on postnatal day 4, in our rat condominiums for increase in branching and a decrease in spine density
3 months, beginning at the time of weaning (42,47). Once when they are placed in the condominiums at weaning.
again, there was a reversal of the behavioral impairments Furthermore, animals with day-7 frontal lesions show no
and this was, again, correlated with a reversal of the change in branching but an increase in spine density, which is
dendritic atrophy. As with the tactile stimulation, there associated with a good behavioral outcome. Enrichment in
was also a reversal of the decreased spine density in the day-7 animals produced an increase in dendritic branching
normal animal that was associated with the recovery. We
have not yet examined the effects of condominium housing
in the adult animal with a perinatal cortical injury, but our TABLE 3
SUMMARY OF THE EFFECTS OF ENRICHMENT BEGINNING AT
WEANING

TABLE 2 Group Isolated Enriched


SUMMARY OF THE EFFECTS OF TACTILE STIMULATION ON
BASILAR SPINE DENSITY A. Apical branches
Control 28.8 ⫾ .8 32.1 ⫾ .7*
Group Non-stroked Stroked Frontal 27.0 ⫾ .8 30.4 ⫾ .8*
B. Apical spines
Control 7.8 ⫾ .1 7.3 ⫾ .1* Control 5.5 ⫾ .1 5.2 ⫾ .1*
Frontal 7.8 ⫾ .1 7.4 ⫾ .1* Frontal 5.8 ⫾ .1 5.2 ⫾ .1*

After Kolb et al. (48). After Kolb et al. (47).


Numbers represent mean spines per 10 m ( ⫾ SE) for layer-III pyramidal Numbers represent mean branches per cell or spines per 10 m ( ⫾ SE) for
cells in parietal cortex. layer-III pyramidal cells in parietal cortex.
*Differs significantly from nonstroked group. *Differs significantly from isolated group.
AGE, EXPERIENCE AND THE CHANGING BRAIN 155

but a decrease in spine density relative to cage-reared arborizations in the orbital prefrontal regions in females
animals (Table 3). In other words, an increase in spine (52). Thus, these sex differences are not evidence of more
density is associated with recovery after day-7 lesions but synapses in one sex than the other, but rather they suggest a
this effect is reversed with enrichment! It appears that the fundamental difference in the organization of the prefrontal
brain’s response to the injury is altered fundamentally by the regions in males and females. Furthermore, we have found
enrichment, even though the behavioral compensation is sex differences in the effects of lesions to these regions, a
only minimally affected. It is unclear what this may mean to result that would be consistent with the anatomical results
the animal but one prediction is that, unlike the adult brain- (e.g. (40)). One explanation for the sex-related differences
injured rat who could not easily change its brain with in the prefrontal areas is that the presence of gonadal
experience (see above), the infant brain-injured rat may hormones during development fundamentally alters the
still be capable of considerable change. For example, we structure of the cells. We have tested this hypothesis directly
would predict that there would be greater changes in by looking at the effects of neonatal gonadectomy or
response to specific experiences (e.g. training to reach or treatment with testosterone on cortical morphology and
pull strings) in the infant versus adult injured animal. have found that hormone manipulations do indeed alter
To sum up, the effects of experience on the injured brain the neuronal structure in the different cortical regions (e.g.
are complex and vary with precise age at injury as well as (86,87)).
the time of onset of experience. Perhaps the most important Recently, Jacobs et al. (27) have described a sex differ-
message is that the infant with the most miserable functional ence in their quantitative dendritic analysis of Wernicke’s
outcome is especially helped by behavioral therapy. This is area, which is a language-related area, in humans. Their
an important lesson for treatment of children with perinatal finding was that females have more extensive dendritic
brain injuries. arbors than males. This result is consistent with the reliable
finding that females are superior in certain verbal skills than
males (for a review see (55)). Furthermore, in a subsequent
9. SEX AND EXPERIENCE-DEPENDENT CHANGES IN THE BRAIN study, Jacobs and Scheibel (26) found that this sex differ-
ence was present as early as age 9, suggesting that such sex
There is accumulating evidence that the male and female
differences emerge within the first decade. These sex
brain differ in their structure, respond differently to
differences in cortical architecture in humans are parallel
environmental events, and respond differently to injury.
to those reported in other studies showing sex differences in
We consider each aspect in turn.
cerebral blood flow and glucose metabolism with females
having about at 15% higher level than males (e.g. (1,73)).
The importance of hormones to cortical structure is not
9.1. Structural differences in the male and female brain
restricted to development. When Stewart and Kolb (87)
The first place to look for structural differences in the ovariectomized or gonadectomized adult rats they found a
brains of males and females is in those structures controlling significant change in cortical structure, especially in the
behaviors such as sexual behavior or maternal behavior in females. Thus, the brains of the ovariectomized rats not only
mammals or singing in birds. Sexual behavior is known to grew heavier but the cortical neurons showed an 25%
be highly dependent upon the hypothalamus and there is increase in dendritic arbor and an 10% increase in spine
now considerable evidence that the preoptic area of rodents density (Table 4). Males showed only an increase in spine
and primates is sexually dimorphic (3,16). Similarly, there density. This result is surprising as it implies that cortical
is substantial literature documenting sex-related differences morphology is hormone-dependent throughout the life of a
in structures related to singing in the song bird. Thus, there mammal. The finding of hormone-dependent changes in
is now little doubt that sexually dimorphic behaviors are cortical structure in the adult animal is corroborated by more
related to sexually dimorphic structure in the brain regions recent studies of astrocytes.
of mammals and birds that are related in some way to To summarize, there are sex-dependent differences in
reproductive behavior. What is more interesting, however, neuronal architecture in the cortex. These differences are
is the more recent evidence that there are sex-related due, in part, to the effects of gonadal hormones during
differences in cortical structures that are not known to be development but, especially in females, there is evidence
related to the control of behaviors related to mating. For that circulating hormones alter cortical architecture in
example, morphometric analyses of the male and female
brain have shown differences in neuron numbers in different
cortical regions (e.g. (71,72)), as well as in dendritic
structure (32). Recently, Seymoure and Juraska (78) TABLE 4
SUMMARY OF THE EFFECTS OF OVARIECTOMY OR
reported that there may be sex differences in visual perceptual CASTRATION IN ADULTHOOD
abilities of male and female rats. In particular, although they
found no differences in visual acuity, they did find that males Group Branches Terminal spines Secondary spines
had more acute vernier acuity than females, a result that has,
at least, circumstantial correlation with the morphological Male 30.3 5.3 ⫾ .2 8.6 ⫾ .2
difference in the visual cortex of male and female rats. Castrated 28.0 6.0 ⫾ 3 9.4 ⫾ .1
In our own work, we have found differences in prefrontal Female 24.8 5.7 ⫾ .1 9.1 ⫾ .2
Ovariectomy 31.6 5.9 ⫾ .1 9.9 ⫾ .2
and parietal regions as well (e.g. (53,86,87)). The sex
differences in the prefrontal regions are intriguing because After Stewart and Kolb (87).
we find greater dendritic arborizations in the midline pre- Numbers represent mean branches per cell or spines per 10 m ( ⫾ SE) for
frontal regions in males and, in contrast, greater dendritic apical field of layer-III pyramidal cells in parietal cortex.
156 KOLB ET AL.

adulthood as well. Finally, although it has not been studied not considered the role of sex. We therefore reanalyzed our
in detail, it also seems likely that there are sex-related studies in which juvenile rats were placed in the condo-
differences in astrocytic numbers and morphology in the miniums or infant rats were given tactile stimulation (46).
cortex as well. The results showed that the sex-dependent differences in the
effects of enrichment varied with age. Firstly, we found that
females showed a greater response to tactile stimulation
9.2. Sex, experience, and cortical structure than males. Hence, females showed a greater decrease in
Juraska (e.g. (30–35)) was the first to report that neurons spine density than males. Secondly, we found that both
in the occipital cortex of the male rat show significant sexes showed an increase in dendritic arborization and a
dendritic growth with 30 days of enriched experience, decrease in spine density when they were placed in the
whereas neurons in the occipital cortex of the female rat condominiums at weaning.
do not. In contrast, she found that hippocampal neurons Thus, it appears that sex differences in the effect of
show a greater response to the enriched rearing in the female experience are not only influenced by the length of the
brain than in the male brain. Although we found these enrichment experience but that the details of the
experiments intriguing, they were also puzzling since it environment-responsive changes vary with age.
seemed reasonable to suppose that experience was altering
the cortex of both males and females. We hypothesized that 10. CONCLUSIONS
perhaps Juraska’s sex differences were at least partly due to
differences in the sensitivity of the cortex to experience. In One of the most intriguing questions in behavioral
other words, we wondered what would happen if animals neuroscience concerns the manner in which the brain, and
were given more prolonged exposure to the enriched especially the neocortex, can modify its structure and
environments. We therefore placed adult male and female ultimately its function throughout one’s lifetime. As the
rats in condominiums for 5 months. Analysis of the review has suggested, the cortex can be changed dramati-
pyramidal cells in layer III of both parietal and occipital cally by experience and this change is modulated by various
cortex revealed an unexpected result: Although the dendritic factors. Several basic conclusions can be extracted regard-
arborization of both males and females increased with 5 ing the nature of the relationship between experience, brain
months of living in the condos, the increases were greater in plasticity, and behavior.
females. Thus, in contrast to Juraska’s studies of visual cortex 1. Experience alters the synaptic organization of the cortex.
after 30 days of enrichment, we found a larger increase in It is possible to visualize with a light microscope the morpho-
dendritic arborization in the cells in female cortex than in logical changes in the cortex that reflect synaptic modification.
male cortex (Table 5). Somewhat surprisingly, however, when These structural changes include modification of both neurons
we examined the spine density, we found a larger increase in and glia. The neuronal changes can be quantified as increases
spine density in males than in females (46,58). These results or decreases in the amount of dendritic arborization as well as
lead us to two conclusions. First, both males and females show in the density of dendritic spines. The glial changes can be
experience dependent changes in the pyramidal neurons of the measured by calculating the number and the size of astrocytes
cortex. It may be, however, that males are more sensitive than and the density of microglia.
females and show morphological changes sooner than 2. Experience can alter different parts of neurons differ-
females. If so, this may account for the apparent difference ently. For instance, alterations in dendritic length can occur
between the results of Juraska and the results of our experi- independently of changes in dendritic spine density.
ments. Second, there is a sex difference in the details of the Environmental stimulation during the juvenile and adoles-
structural changes in cortical neurons: males show a greater cent period produces increased growth of dendritic length,
increase in spine density, whereas females show a greater while at the same time producing a decrease in spine
increase in dendritic length. density. The increase in dendritic length would act to
The next question we considered was whether age at increase the number of synapses, whereas the decrease in
enrichment would interact with sex. We had shown spine density would act to decrease the total number of
previously that age at the onset of enriched rearing differ- synapses. Furthermore, different parts of the neuron can
entially affected spine density (see Section 6.1) but we had change independently of other parts. Thus, there may be
changes in distal parts of the dendritic arbor that are
independent of changes in proximal parts.
3. Changes in synaptic organization are correlated with
TABLE 5 changes in behavior. Animals with extensive dendritic
SUMMARY OF THE EFFECTS OF 5 MONTHS EXPERIENCE IN THE
CONDOMINIUMS
growth relative to untreated animals show facilitated per-
formance on many types of behavioral measures, especially
Group Isolated Enriched measures of cognitive activity. Such changes are visible not
only in laboratory animals but also in humans. For example,
Apical Basilar Apical Basilar Jacobs and Scheibel (26) and Jacobs et al. (27) found a
relationship between the extent of dendritic arborization in a
Male 29.5 40.6 30.7 51.4* cortical language area (Wernicke’s area) and the amount of
Female 26.3 38.9 33.5* 53.3* education. The cortical neurons from the brains of deceased
people with a university education had more dendritic arbor
After Kolb et al. (46).
Numbers represent mean branches per cell for layer-III pyramidal cells in
than those from people with a high-school education who, in
parietal cortex. turn, had more dendritic material than those with less than
*Significantly different from same-sex isolated group. high-school education.
AGE, EXPERIENCE AND THE CHANGING BRAIN 157

4. The effects of experience vary both qualitatively and 7. Damage to the hippocampus, and perhaps other
quantitatively with age. For example, ‘‘enriched’’ experi- structures, alters the brain’s response to experience. One
ences during the immediate postnatal period have no effect of this modification is that, at least in the case of
measurable effect on dendritic length and lead to a decrease hippocampal injury, the adult brain responds to experience
in spine density. Enriched experiences during the juvenile as though it were an immature brain. This has important
and adolescent period produce increases in dendritic length implications for understanding not only the function of the
but decreases in spine density. Similar experiences later in hippocampus in processes related to learning and memory,
life produce increases in both dendritic length and spine but also in understanding the behavior of the individual with
density. All of these experiences have behavioral sequelae hippocampal injury.
in adulthood. The age-dependent plastic changes in the 8. Gonadal hormones influence synaptic organization of
cortex are likely very important and reflect the differential the cortex throughout the lifetime of an animal. Males and
sensitivity of the child’s brain to experience during develop- females have brains that are, at least in some regions,
ment. What is not yet known is how different experiences at fundamentally different. Experience may have different
different times in life might interact and to what extent the morphological effects on the brains of males and females,
absence of a particular experience might be compensated and even a single sex may have different morphological
for later in life. changes in response to the same experience at different
5. There are compensatory plastic changes in the brain times in the life history. For females, this influence of
following brain injury that are similar in kind to those gonadal hormones may have especially important implica-
observed when animals learn from experience. These tions for differential synaptic plasticity at different times in
injury-induced changes are also age-dependent and are the menstrual cycle. Furthermore, the sudden loss of ovarian
thought to reflect the age-related recovery observed after hormones at menopause will have important implications
brain injury at different times throughout the lifetime. For not only for cognitive abilities during the second half of
example, animals with cortical injuries in the first week of life but also for the ways in which the brain copes not
life show decreased dendritic arborization that is associated only with normal cell death during aging but also with
with a poor functional outcome. In contrast, animals with injury and disease.
cortical injuries in the second week of life show increased 9. Neurotrophins, which may be produced either by
dendritic arborization and a good functional outcome. neurons or glia, influence the plastic changes in the cortex.
The similarity between the plastic changes in the brain in More importantly in the current context, the production of
response to injury or experience suggests that there may be neurotrophins is influenced by experience. Thus, one reason
basic mechanisms of synaptic change in the mammalian why behavioral therapies may be effective in changing
cortex that are used in many forms of synaptic plasticity. the brain is that the behavioral changes brought about
This is an encouraging possibility, for it allows hope that we by specific experiences actually stimulate the brain to
will be able to improve recovery from cerebral injury by produce neurotrophins. This link between behavior,
taking advantage of the innate mechanisms that the brain neurotrophins, and plasticity requires further study,
uses for other forms of plasticity. Furthermore, to offer a especially in the context of designing therapies for
more speculative hypothesis, it may also be possible to restitution of function.
reverse the effects of impoverished experience by using 10. There are limits to the amount that a brain can change.
pharmacological treatments that are effective in improving We still do not know what determines the limits, nor in most
recovery from brain injury. cases, what the limits might be. Nonetheless, it does appear
6. Injury-induced changes in cortical structure are that when the brain changes ‘‘spontaneously’’ after injury
modified by experience. Furthermore, brains that show the there is a reduction in the plasticity of the affected regions.
least change in response to a cortical injury appear to be the This implies that behavioral therapies should be initiated
most responsive to experience. For instance, animals with early in the postinjury period to ensure that the
neonatal brain injuries show a poor functional outcome and ‘‘spontaneous’’ changes can be influenced in such a way
an atrophy of cortical neurons but these animals show as to maximize functional recovery.
dramatic functional recovery and marked synaptic growth
in response to environmental manipulations. This is
encouraging for it suggests that behavioral therapies should ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
be especially helpful in reversing some of the devastating
consequences of brain damage in the latter periods of This research was supported by an NSERC of Canada
prenatal development in human infants. grant to Bryan Kolb.

REFERENCES
1. Baxter, L.R. Jr; Mazziotta, J.C.; Phelps, M.E.; Selin, C.E.; Guze, Becker, J.B.; Breedlove, S.M.; Crews, D., eds. Behavioral endo-
G.H.; Fairbanks, L., Cerebral glucose metabolic rates in normal crinology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1992:39–70.
human females versus normal males. Psychol. Res., 21, 237–245 4. Capowski, J. Computer techniques in neuroanatomy. New York:
1987. Plenum Press; 1989.
2. Black, J.E.; Isaacs, K.R.; Anderson, B.J.; Alcantara, A.A.; Greenough, 5. Chang, F.-L.F.; Greenough, W.T., Lateralized effects of monocular
W.T., Learning causes synaptogenesis, whereas motor activity causes training on dendritic branching in adult split-brain rats. Brain Res.,
angiogenesis, in cerebellar cortex of adult rats. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 232, 283–292 1982.
USA, 87, 5568–5572 1990. 6. Coopersmith, R.; Leon, M., Enhanced neural response to familiar
3. Breedlove, S.M. Sexual differentiation of the brain and behavior. In: olfactory cues. Science, 225, 849–851 1984.
158 KOLB ET AL.

7. Cuello, A.C., Trophic factor therapy in the adult CNS: remodeling of phology of pyramidal neurons in the rat hippocampal CA3 area. II.
injured basalo-cortical neurons. Prog. Brain Res., 100, 213–221 Effects of gender and experience. Brain Res., 79, 115–121 1989.
1994. 35. Juraska, J.M.; Kopcik, J.R., Sex and environmental influences on the
8. Diamond, J. Nerve growth factor and the reinnervation of skin after size and ultrastructure of the rat corpus callosum. Brain Res., 450, 1–8
peripheral nerve lesions. In: Flohr, H., ed. Post-lesion neural plasticity. 1988.
Berlin: Springer-Verlag; 1988:35–48. 36. Kass, J., The organization of neocortex in mammals: implications for
9. Diamond, M.C.; Lindner, B.; Raymond, A., Extensive cortical depth theories of brain function. Annu. Rev. Psychol., 38, 129–151 1987.
measurements and neuron size increases in the cortex of environ- 37. Kolb, B., Functions of the frontal cortex of the rat: a comparative
mentally enriched rats. J. Comp. Neurol., 131, 357–364 1967. review. Brain Res. Rev., 8, 65–98 1984.
10. Diamond, M.C.; Dowling, G.A.; Johnson, R.E., Morphologic cerebral 38. Kolb, B., Animal models for human PFC-related disorders. Prog.
cortical asymmetry in male and female rats. Exp. Neurol., 71, 261–268 Brain Res., 85, 501–519 1990.
1981. 39. Kolb, B. Brain plasticity and behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
11. Field, T.; Schanberg, S.M.; Scafidi, F.; Bauer, C.R.; Vega-Lahr, N.; Erlbaum; 1995.
Garcia, R.; Nystrom, J.; Kuhn, C.M., Tactile/kinesthetic stimulation 40. Kolb, B.; Cioe, J. Sex-related differences in cortical function after
effects on preterm neonates. Pediatrics, 77, 654–658 1986. medial frontal lesions in rats. Behav. Neuroscience, 110, 1271–1281
12. Flohr, H. (ed.). Post-lesion neural plasticity. Berlin: Springer-Verlag, 1996.
1988. 41. Kolb, B., Cote, S., Ribeiro-da-Silva, A., Cuello, A.C. NGF stimulates
13. Forgie, M.L.; Gibb, R.; Kolb, B., Unilateral lesions of the forelimb recovery of function and dendritic growth after unilateral motor cortex
area of rat motor cortex: lack of evidence for use-dependent neural lesions in rats. Neuroscience, 76, 1139–1151 1997.
growth in the undamaged hemisphere. Brain Res., 710, 249–259 42. Kolb, B.; Elliott, W., Recovery from early cortical damage in rats. II.
1996. Effects of experience on anatomy and behavior following frontal
14. Gibb, R., Kolb, B. A method for Vibratome sectioning of Golgi-Cox lesions at 1 or 5 days of age. Behav. Brain Res., 26, 47–56 1987.
stained whole brain. J. Neurosci. Methods, 1997, submitted. 43. Kolb, B.; Gibb, R., Sparing of function after neonatal frontal lesions
15. Globus, A.; Rosenzweig, M.R.; Bennett, E.L.; Diamond, M.C., Effects correlates with increased cortical dendritic branching: a possible
of differential experience on dendritic spine counts in rat cerebral mechanism for the Kennard effect. Behav. Brain Res., 43, 51–56 1991.
cortex. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol., 82, 175–181 1973. 44. Kolb, B.; Gibb, R., Environmental enrichment and cortical injury:
16. Gorski, R.A. Sexual differentiation of the endocrine brain and its behavioral and anatomical consequences of frontal cortex lesions in
control. In: Motta, M., ed. Brain endocrinology, 2nd edn. New York: rats. Cerebral Cortex, 1, 189–198 1991.
Raven Press; 1991:71–104. 45. Kolb, B.; Gibb, R. Experience and the changing brain. I. Age-
17. Greenough, W.T.; Black, J.E.; Wallace, C.S., Experience and brain dependent changes in dendritic growth following enriched rearing at
development. Child Dev., 58, 539–559 1987. different ages. 1997; in preparation.
18. Greenough, W.T.; Chang, F.F. Plasticity of synapse structure and 46. Kolb, B.; Gibb, R.; Gorny, G.; Forgie, M. Experience and the changing
pattern in the cerebral cortex. In: Peters, A.; Jones, E.G., eds. Cerebral brain. II. Sex-dependent changes in dendritic growth following
cortex, vol. 7. New York: Plenum Press; 1988:391–440. enriched rearing at different ages. 1997; in preparation.
19. Greenough, W.T.; West, R.W.; de Voogd, T.J., Sub-synaptic plate 47. Kolb, B.; Gibb, R.; Gorny, G. Experience and the changing brain. III.
perforations: changes with age and experience in the rat. Science, 202, Therapeutic effects of enriched rearing after frontal lesions in infancy.
1096–1098 1978. 1997; in preparation.
20. Greenough, W.T.; Juraska, J.M.; Volkmar, F.R., Maze training effects 48. Kolb, B., Gibb, R., Gorny, G. Experience and the changing brain. IV.
on dendritic branching in occipital cortex of adult rats. Behav. Neural Therapeutic effects of tactile stimulation after cortical lesions in
Biol., 26, 287–297 1979. infancy. 1997; in preparation.
21. Greenough, W.T.; Larson, J.R.; Withers, G.S., Effects of unilateral 49. Kolb, B.; Gibb, R.; van der Kooy, D., Neonatal frontal cortical lesions
and bilateral training in a reaching task on dendritic branching of in rats alter cortical structure and connectivity. Brain Res., 645, 85–97
neurons in the rat motorsensory forelimb cortex. Behav. Neural Biol., 1994.
44, 301–314 1985. 50. Kolb, B.; Gorny, G.; Cote, S.; Ribeiro-da-Silva, A.; Cuello, A.C.
22. Harris, K.M.; Kater, S.B., Dendritic spines: cellular specializations Nerve growth factor stimulates growth of cortical pyramidal neurons
imparting both stability and flexibility to synaptic function. Annu. Rev. in young adult rats. Brain Res., 751, 289–294 1997.
Psychol., 17, 341–371 1994. 51. Kolb, B.; Ladowski, R.; Gibb, R.; Gorny, G. Does dendritic growth
23. Hawrylak, N.; Greenough, W.T., Monocular deprivation alters the underlay recovery from neonatal occipital lesions in rats?. Behav.
morphology of glial fibrillary acidic protein-immunoreactive Brain Res., 77, 125–133 1996.
astrocytes in the rat visual cortex. Brain Res., 683, 187–199 1995. 52. Kolb, B.; Stewart, J., Sex-related differences in dendritic branching of
24. Hebb, D.O., The effects of early experience on problem solving at cells in the prefrontal cortex of rats. J. Neuroendocrinol., 3, 95–99
maturity. Am. Psychol., 2, 737–745 1947. 1991.
25. Hebb, D.O. The organization of behavior. New York: Wiley; 1949. 53. Kolb, B.; Stewart, J., Changes in neonatal gonadal hormonal environ-
26. Jacobs, B.; Scheibel, A.B., A quantitative dendritic analysis of ment prevent behavioral sparing and alter cortical morphogenesis after
Wernicke’s area. I. Lifespan changes. J. Comp. Neurol., 237, 83–96 early frontal cortex lesions in male and female rats. Behav. Neurosci.,
1993. 109, 285–294 1995.
27. Jacobs, B.; Schall, M.; Scheibel, A.B., A quantitative dendritic 54. Kolb, B.; Tomie, J.; Ouellette, A. Bilateral and unilateral motor
analysis of Wernicke’s area. II. Gender hemispheric, and environ- experience alters dendritic length but not spine density in cortical
mental factors. J. Comp. Neurol., 237, 97–111 1993. pyramidal neurons. 1997; in preparation.
28. Jones, T.A.; Schallert, T., Overgrowth and pruning of dendrites in 55. Kolb, B.; Whishaw, I.Q. Fundamentals of Human Neuropsychology.
adult rats recovering from neocortical damage. Brain Res., 581, 156– 4th edn. New York: W.H. Freeman; 1996.
160 1992. 56. Kolb, B.; Whishaw, I.Q; van der Kooy, D. Brain development in the
29. Jones, T.A.; Schallert, T., Use-dependent growth of pyramidal neurons neonatally decorticated rat. Brain Res., 397, 315–326 1986.
after neocortical damage. J. Neurosci., 14, 2140–2152 1994. 57. Leon, M., The neurobiology of filial learning. Annu. Rev. Psychol., 43,
30. Juraska, J.M., Sex differences in dendritic responses to differential 377–398 1992.
experience in the rat visual cortex. Brain Res., 295, 27–34 1984. 58. Munoz-Cueto, J.; Carcia-Segura, L.; Ruiz-Marcos, A., Developmental
31. Juraska, J.M. Sex differences in developmental plasticity of behavior sex differences and effect of ovariectomy on the number of cortical
and the brain. In: Greenough, W.T.; Juraska, J.M., eds. Developmental pyramidal cell dendritic spines. Brain Res., 515, 64–68 1990.
neuropsychobiology. New York: Academic Press; 1986. 59. Napieralski, J.A., Butler, A.K., Chesselte, M.-F. Anatomical and
32. Juraska, J.M. The structure of the cerebral cortex: effects of gender functional evidence for lesion specific sprouting of corticostriatal
and the environment. In: Kolb, B.; Tees, R., eds. The cerebral cortex of input in the adult rat. J. Comp. Neurol, 373, 484–497 1996.
the rat. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1990:483–506. 60. Pasternak, J.F.; Woolsey, T.A., On the ‘‘selectivity’’ of the Golgi-Cox
33. Juraska, J.M.; Fitch, J.; Henderson, C.; Rivers, N., Sex differences in method. J. Comp. Neurol., 160, 307–312 1987.
the dendritic branching of dentate granule cells following differential 61. Prusky, G.; Whishaw, I.Q., Morphology of identified corticospinal
experience. Brain Res., 333, 73–80 1985. cells in the rat following motor cortex injury: absence of use-
34. Juraska, J.M.; Fitch, J.M.; Washburne, D.L., The dendritic mor- dependent change. Brain Res., 714, 1–8 1996.
AGE, EXPERIENCE AND THE CHANGING BRAIN 159

62. Purpura, D.P., Dendritic spine ‘‘dysgenesis’’ and mental retardation. 81. Sirevaag, A.M.; Greenough, W.T., Plasticity of GFAP-immuno-
Science, 186, 1126–1128 1974. reactive astrocyte size and number in visual cortex of rats reared in
63. Purves, D. Neural Activity and the Growth of the Brain. Cambridge: complex environments. Brain Res., 540, 273–278 1991.
Cambridge University Press; 1994. 82. Shulkin, J. Preoperative events: their effects on behavior following
64. Purves, D.; Hadley, R.D.; Voyvodic, J.T., Dynamic changes in the brain damage. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1989.
dendritic geometry of individual neurons visualized over periods of up 83. Skinner, B.F. The behavior of organisms. New York: Appleton-
to three months in the superior cervical ganglion of living mice. J. Century-Crofts; 1938.
Neurosci., 6, 1051–1060 1986. 84. Solkoff, N.; Matuszak, D., Tactile stimulation and behavioral
65. Purves, D.; Hume, R.I., The relation of postsynaptic geometry to the development among low-birthweight infants. Child Psychiatr. Hum.
number of presynaptic axons that innervate autonomic ganglion cells. Dev., 6, 33–37 1975.
J. Neurosci., 1, 441–452 1981. 85. Steward, O. Synapse replacement on cortical neurons following
66. Purves, D.; Lichtman, J.W., Geometrical differences among homo- denervation. In: Peters, A.; Jones, E.G., eds. Cerebral cortex, vol. 9.
logous neurons in mammals. Science, 228, 298–302 1985. New York: Plenum Press; 1991:81–131.
67. Purves, D.; Rubin, E.; Snider, W.D.; Lichtman, J.W., Relations of 86. Stewart, J.; Kolb, B., The effects of neonatal gonadectomy and
animal size to convergence, divergences and neuronal number in prenatal stress on cortical thickness and asymmetry in rats. Behav.
peripheral sympathetic pathways. J. Neurosci., 6, 158–163 1986. Neural Biol., 49, 344–360 1988.
68. Purves, D.; Voyvodic, J.T., Imaging mammalian nerve cells and their 87. Stewart, J.; Kolb, B., Dendritic branching in cortical pyramidal cells in
connections over time in living animals. Trends Neurosci., 10, 398– response to ovariectomy in adult female rats: suppression by neonatal
404 1987. exposure to testosterone. Brain Res., 654, 149–154 1994.
69. Pylyshyn, Z.W., Computation and cognition: issues in the foundations 88. Sullivan, R.M.; Leon, M., Early olfactory learning induces an
of cognitive science. Behav. Brain Sci., 3, 11–69 1980. enhanced olfactory bulb response in rats. Dev. Brain Res., 27, 278–
70. Ramon y Cajal, S. Degeneration and Regeneration of the Nervous 282 1986.
System. London: Oxford University Press; 1928. 89. Sutherland, R.J.; Kolb, B.; Gibb, R., Experience-induced neocortical
71. Reid, S.N.M.; Juraska, J.M., Sex differences in the gross size of the rat dendritic growth is reduced by hippocampal formation damage. Soc.
neocortex. J. Comp. Neurol., 321, 442–447 1992. Neurosci. Abstr., 19, 362 1993.
72. Reid, S.N.M.; Juraska, J.M., Sex differences in neuron number in the 90. Turner, A.M.; Greenough, W.T. Synapses per neuron and synaptic
binocular area of the rat visual cortex. J. Comp. Neurol., 321, 448–455 dimensions in occipital cortex of rats reared in complex, social, or
1992. isolation housing. Acta Sterol. 2 (Suppl. 1):239-244; 1983.
73. Rodriguez, G.; Waarkentin, S.; Risberg, J.; Rosandini, G., Sex 91. Turner, A.M.; Greenough, W.T., Differential rearing effects on rat
differences in regional cerebral blood flow. J. Cereb. Blood Flow visual cortex synapses. I. Synaptic and neuronal density and synapses
Metabol., 8, 783–789 1988. per neuron. Brain Res., 329, 195–203 1985.
74. Rosenzweig, M.R.; Krech, D.; Bennett, E.L.; Diamond, M., Effects of 92. Vokmar, R.F.; Greenough, W.T., Rearing complexity affects branch-
environmental complexity and training on brain chemistry and ing of dendrites in visual cortex of the rat. Science, 176, 1445–1447
anatomy: a replication and extension. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol., 1972.
55, 429–437 1962. 93. West, R.W.; Greenough, W.T., Effect of environmental complexity on
75. Rosenzweig, M.R.; Bennett, E.L. Experiential influences on brain cortical synapses of rats. Behav. Biol., 7, 279–284 1972.
anatomy and brain chemistry in rodents. In: Gottlieb, G., ed. Studies 94. Whishaw, I.Q. The decorticate rat. In: Kolb, B; Tees, R.C., eds.
on the Development of Behavior and the Nervous System. New York: Cerebral cortex of the rat. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1990:239–
Academic Press; 1978:289–387. 268.
76. Rowntree, S. Basic fibroblast growth factor in the injured brain. 95. Whishaw, I.Q.; Pellis, S.M.; Gorny, B.P.; Pellis, V.C., The impair-
Unpublished Master’s thesis, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, ments in reaching and the movements of compensation in rats with
Alta., Canada; 1995. motor cortex lesions: an endpoint, videorecording, and movement
77. Rowntree, S.; Kolb, B. Antibodies to bFGF block functional recovery notation analysis. Behav. Brain Res., 42, 77–91 1991.
and dendritic compensation after motor cortex lesions. Eur. J. 96. Whishaw, I.Q.; Nonneman, A.J.; Kolb, B., Environmental changes can
Neurosci. 1997, in press. improve grooming, swimming and eating in decorticate rats. J. Comp.
78. Seymoure, P.; Juraska, J.M. Vernier and grating acuity in adult hooded Physiol. Psychol., 95, 792–804 1981.
rats: the influence of sex. Behav. Neurosci., 1997; in press. 97. Will, B., Kelche, C. Environmental approaches to recovery of function
79. Schanberg, S.M.; Field, T.M., Sensory deprivation stress and supple- from brain damage: a review of animal studies (1981 to 1991). In:
mental stimulation in the rat pup and preterm human neonate. Child Rose, F.D.; Johnson, D.A., eds. Recovery from brain damage:
Dev., 58, 1431–1447 1987. reflections and directions. New York: Plenum Press; 1992:79–104.
80. Sirevaag, A.M.; Greenough, W.T., Differential rearing effects on rat 98. Woo, C.C.; Leon, M., Sensitive period for neural and behavioral
visual cortex synapses. III. Neuronal and glial nuclei, boutons, response development to learned odors. Dev. Brain Res., 36, 309–313
dendrites, and capillaries. Brain Res., 424, 320–332 1987. 1987.

You might also like