You are on page 1of 4

NEUROSCIENCE

ELSEVIER Neuroscience Letters 210 (1996) 161-164


LETTERS
Alterations in cortical thickness and neuronal density
in the frontal cortex of Albert Einstein

B r i t t A n d e r s o n a,b,*, T h o m a s H a r v e y
aDepartment of Neurology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, AL 35294-0007, USA
bNeur6,1ogy Service, Birmingham Department of Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Birmingham, AL, USA

Received 11 March 1996; revised version received 26 April 1996; accepted 26 April 1996

Abstract

Neuronal density, neuron size, and the number of neurons under 1 mm 2 of cerebral cortical surface area were measured in the right
pre-frontal cortex of Albert Einstein and five elderly control subjects. Measurement of neuronal density used the optical dissector
technique on celloidin-embedded cresyl violet-stained sections. The neurons counted provided a systematic random sample for the
measurement of cell body cross-sectional area. Einstein's cortex did not differ from the control subjects in the number of neurons under
1 mm 2 of cerebral cortex or in mean neuronal size. Because Einstein's cortex was thinner than the controls he had a greater neuronal
density.

Keywords: Intelligence; Brain; Einstein; Neurons; Cerebral cortex

Humanity is fascinated by the brains of geniuses [6,9, genius. Using the optical dissector technique neuronal
16,19,21] and scientists have measured brain size and gyri density, the number of neurons under 1 mm 2 of cortex,
to explain the intellectual bounty of great men. While and the cross-sectional area of a random selection of cor-
such neuromorphologic investigations may now seem tical neurons were measured and compared to five elderly
quaint, at odds with politically popular beliefs of general men.
intellectual parity and the modularity of mental opera- Einstein died in 1955, at age 76 years, of a ruptured
tions, these prejudices deserve re-examination. Intelli- abdominal aortic aneurysm. An autopsy was performed
gence is not a mystical psychological process which con- 7 h postmortem. The brain was fixed in formalin for sev-
fers smartness in proportion to its presence [7], but a psy- eral months. After fixation the cerebral cortex was di-
chometric index for the effects of neurobiological features vided into numbered blocks and embedded in celloidin.
which affect the efficiency of neural operations [1,23]. The brain region from which the Einstein block was taken
The presence of such biological features is indicated by is shown in the diagram in Fig. 1. The fact that the block
the anatomical and physiological correlations between is no. 9 and probably includes brain tissue from Brod-
brain and intelligence, .e.g. the brain volume-IQ correla- mann area 9 is coincidental. This diagram was prepared at
tions [2,10,15,22,24-261. the time of the original processing of Einstein's tissue for
Studying the brain of a genius can play a small and celloidin embedding. The celloidin blocks were kept in
titillating role in the quest to identify these neurobiologi- alcohol until use.
cal features. Single case studies, while not definitive, may The control subjects were all men, ages 63, 63, 64, 71,
point the way towards s.olutions and have a long history in and 79 years. All died non-neurological deaths and rou-
cognitive research [4]. These considerations prompted us tine neuropathologic examinations were unremarkable.
to undertake precise c~uantitative measurements of the The area of cortex studied for each subject was right
cerebral cortex of Albert Einstein, our most renowned middle frontal gyrus midway between the frontal tip and
the central sulcus and corresponded topographically with
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 205 9349691; fax: +1 205 9334465; the available area of Einstein's prefrontal cortex. Similar
e-mail: brittuab@aol.com. to Einstein's tissue, all control blocks were immersion

0304-3940/96/SI2.00 © 1996 Elsevier Science Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved


PII: S0304-3940(96) 12693-6
162 B. Anderson, T. Harvey / Neuroscience Letters 210 (1996) 161-164

(>0.95 by Cronbach's a). To calculate the number of neu-


rons under 1 mm 2 of cerebral cortex the neuron density
was multiplied by the cortical depth.
The neuron density for each of the passes through
Einstein's cerebral cortex was compared to similar meas-
ures for the control group by the M a n n - W h i t n e y U-test.
The same test was used to compare the estimates of the
number of neurons under 1 mm 2 of cortex and the size of
the cerebral cortical neurons.
A non-parametric test was used because the normality
of the data could not be established with this small sample
size. The reason for using multiple measurements from
Fig. 1. Schematic prepared at the time that Einstein's cerebral cortex Einstein's and the controls' tissues as individual 'sam-
was originally dissected. The cerebral cortical block used for the cur- ples' was to strengthen the conclusion that Einstein's
rent study was no. 9. greater results for some measures were not due to chance.
With only six specimens the odds that Einstein would
fixed in formalin, followed by celloidin embedding and have had, for example, a greater measured neuronal
maintenance of the celloidin blocks in 80% ethanol. Most density was l/6 by chance alone. However, if Einstein's
of the control tissue was kept in 80% ethanol for only neuronal density was truly the same as the control popu-
weeks whereas Einstein's tissue had been maintained in lation, the odds that repeated assessments of neuronal
this way for decades. density would have been consistently greater than the
Sections were cut at a thickness of 40ktm using a controls should have grown smaller with each repetition.
sliding microtome and stained with cresyl violet. Under By using each measure as an individual 'sample' and ap-
microscopic examination and using the Neurolucida com- plying the M a n n - W h i t n e y U-test it was possible to
puter program (MicroBrightField, Colchester, VT) four quantitate the odds for such results.
measurements of cortical thickness were made on a single Einstein's cortex was thinner than the controls and
section at 200 ktm intervals (see Fig. 2). more densely populated with neurons. The mean distance
With a 100x objective each mark, as illustrated in Fig. through the right frontal cortex of Einstein was 2 1 3 7 / t m
2, was sequentially moved to. A grid of 20 × 20/,tm was and for the control population it was 2659/,tm (P = 0.002,
overlaid and neurons counted while focusing down U = 0.0, n's 20/4). Einstein had more neurons per mm 3 of
through the section. Neurons in focus at the start of the
section were not counted, nor were any neurons counted B,
that touched the lower and left grid exclusion lines. Neu-
rons were counted when their cellular and nuclear mem-
branes came into sharpest focus. A nucleolus was not
required for a neuron to be counted. Neurons were differ-
entiated from glia based on size and staining patterns. The
number of neurons per dissector was recorded as was the
,I ji C.
" //
depth of the dissector. Most dissectors contained zero or "~,,~ {I'i
one neuron. The m a x i m u m was four. For all brains except
one it was necessary to make four passes through the
LI I
cortex to count the 100 neurons that was the study goal.
In one brain, because of a thicker cortex, a sufficient
number of neurons was achieved with only three passes
Fig. 2. The approach for measuringthe anatomical variables. (A) On a
through the cortex. This approach is basically the optical single microscopic section of cortex a flat area of the cortical ribbon,
dissector [8,12,20]. excludinggyral depths, was selected. (B) Along this strip, and using the
While these cells were being counted the cell tracing Neurolucida Program, four roughly parallel transepts were drawn per-
software of Neurolucida was used to trace each cell body pendicular to the cortical surface and separated by 200/.tM. (C) A ran-
dom number between 1 and 80 was determined and the first counting
area at the cell's greatest diameter to provide the cross-
zone (marked by a dot) was marked this many microns along the first
sectional areas of a systematically random selection of transept (a). Additional counting zones (additional dots) were then
neurons. marked every 80/~M along the vertical meridians (b). The marking
Neuron density was computed by dividing the number continuedfor all four meridianswith the distance between the last mark
of neurons counted by the area of the grid square multi- of a meridian and the white matter carried over to the start of the next
meridian (therefore, in this example, c + d = 80/~M). The microscope
plied by the total dissector depth (measured from the mi-
was moved sequentiallyfrom each counting zone to the next by virtue
croscopic section) for all dissectors on each pass through of the Neurolucida program. Neuronal measurements and counts at
the cortex. The reliability of these estimates was high each countingzone followed procedures described in the text.
B. Anderson, T. Harvey / Neuroscience Letters 210 (1996) 161-164 163

cortex with a mean for the four dissector paths through ble to provide a measure o f total cerebral cortical volume
the cortex of 46 995 neurons/mm 3 compared to the mean the available data does not suggest that Einstein's cerebral
for the control population of 3 4 9 6 2 neurons/mm 3 cortical volume was greater than controls. Einstein's brain
(P = 0.015, U = 8, n ' s 19/4 ). weight was 1230 g, which is within the average range, but
Einstein's cortex did not differ in the size of the neu- below the mean, for men of his age [11,14]. In addition,
rons' somas or in the number of neurons contained within the length of the hemispheres (17.2 cm left/16.4 cm right)
a column of cortex 1 mm 2 at the surface. The mean cross- and the width of the hemispheres (7.5 cm left/7.5 cm
sectional area of the neurons in Einstein's right frontal right) are not significantly different from 'eminent' and
cerebral cortex was 63 ~em2 (35 SD), and 68 ~ m 2 (46 SD) non-eminent men born in the 1800s [21].
for the control population (P = 0.85, U = 32,713, n's 501/ An alternative consideration would be that an increase
132). The mean number of neurons in a column of cortex in neuronal density might be advantageous by decreasing
1 mm z at the cortical surface was 98 654 for Einstein and interneuronal conduction time. Interneuronal conduction
87 364 for the controls (P = 0.26, U = 24, n's 19/4). time has been posited as a limiting feature of the devel-
Studies performed on Einstein's brain tissue in the opment of brain size and cortical connectivity [ 17,18]. An
early years after his death were only qualitative, and not increase in neuronal density has been implied as a possi-
showing any important differences from normal subjects, ble mechanism to explain how women and men may
were not published. One prior study of Einstein's cerebral achieve comparable I Q ' s [27] even though w o m e n ' s
tissue has been formally reported. Diamond et al. [5] brains are smaller when corrected for body size [3].
measured the ratio of neurons to glia in the frontal and
parietal cerebral cortex. Diamond and colleagues reported This work would not have been possible without the
a greater glia/neuron ratio for Einstein's tissue. Since they superb technical assistance of Valisa Rutledge.
reported neither the absolute numbers of neurons nor their
size, their results cannot be directly compared with this [1] Anderson, B., G explained, Med. Hypotheses, 45 (1995) 602-
604.
study. The results for neuron density and neuron number [2] Andreasen, N.C., Flaum, M., Swayze, I1, V., O'Leary, D.S., AI-
obtained from this inve,;tigation are in general agreement liger, R., Cohen, G., Ehrhardt, J. and Yuh, W.T.C., Intelligence
with prior estimates [13,.27]. and brain structure in normal individuals, Am. J. Psychol., 150
The differences between Einstein's tissue and the con- (1993) 130-134.
trol tissue is primarily due to Einstein having the same [3] Ankney, C.D., Sex differences in relative brain size: the mismeas-
ure of women, too? Intelligence, 16 (1992) 329-336.
number of neurons packed into a thinner cortex. A g e or [4] Berker, E.A., Berker, A.H. and Smith, A., Translation of Broca's
dementia-related atrophy are not explanations for this 1865 report, Arch. Neurol., 43 (1986) 1065-1072.
finding since Einstein's. cortex was compared to elderly [5] Diamond, M.C., Scheibel, A.B., Murphy, G.M. and Harvey, T.,
controls and because there is no history that Einstein suf- On the brain of a scientist: Albert Einstein, Exp. Neurol., 88
fered from cognitive decline. (1985) 198-204.
[6] Donaldson, H.H., A study of the brains of three scholars: Gran-
The thinner cortex is not due to technical factors. Both ville Stanley Hall, Sir William Osier, Edward Sylvester Morse, J.
Einstein's tissue and the controls' were embedded in cel- Comp. Neurol., 46 (1928) 1-95.
loidin, sectioned, and stained similarly. As the brain tissue [7] Gould, S.J., The Mismeasure of Man, Norton, New York, 1981,
is completely dehydrated in an alcohol-ether solution as 352 pp.
part of the celloidin embedding process, the subsequent [8] Gundersen, H.J.G., Bendtsen, T.E., Korbo, L., Marcussen, N.,
Moiler, A., Nielsen, K., Nyengaard, JR, Pakkenberg, B.,
duration of the storage in 80% ethanol should not result in S0rensen, F.B., Vesterby, A. and West, M.J., Some new, simple
greater tissue shrinkage with longer storage. A precise and efficient stereological methods and their use in pathological
measure of the size of ~the tissue block for Einstein prior research and diagnosis, APMIS, 96 (1988) 379-394.
to its embedding was not available to quantitate tissue [9] Hansemann, D., Ueber das gehirn von Hermann v. Helmholtz,
shrinkage. In general, celloidin embedding is felt to result Zeit. Pyschol. Physiol., 8 (1899) 1-12.
[10] Harvey, I., Persaud, R., Ron, M.A., Baker, G. and Murray, R.M.,
in less tissue distortion than paraffin techniques, and Volumetric MR1 measurements in bipolars compared with
probably frozen sections that are subsequently dehy- schizophrenics and healthy controls, Psychol. Med., 24 (1994)
drated. 689-699.
Could the difference in cortical packing density in any [11] Ho, K.-c., Roessmann, U., Straumfjord, J.V. and Monroe, G.,
way account for Einstein's superior intellectual skills? An Analysis of brain weight, !: adult brain weight in relation to sex,
race, and age, Arch. Pathol. Lab. Med., 104 (1980) 635-639.
increase in neuronal density could translate to an overall [12] Mayhew, T.M., A review of recent advances in stereology for
increase in the total number of cortical neurons if Ein- quantifying neural structure, J. Neurocytol., 21 (1992) 313-328.
stein's brain volume were equal to or greater than the [13] Pakkenberg, B., Total nerve cell number in neocortex in chronic
control cases. Unfortunately, it is not currently possible to schizophrenics and controls estimated using optical dissectors,
provide an accurate measurement of total cerebral cortical Biol. Psychol., 34 (1994) 768-772.
[14] Pakkenberg, H. and Voight, J., Brain weight of the Danes, Acta
volume or surface area for Einstein. This measure is nec- Anat., 56 (1964) 297-307.
essary to translate a neuronal density measure into an [15] Raz, N., Torres, l.J., Spencer, W.D., Millman, D., Baertschi, J.C.
absolute neuronal number measure. While it is not possi- and Sarpel, G., Neuroanatomical correlates of age-sensitive and
164 B. Anderson, T. Harvey/Neuroscience Letters 210 (1996) 161-164

age invariant cognitive abilities: an in vivo MRI investigation, [22] Strauss, E., Wada, J. and Hunter, M., Callosal morphology and
Intelligence, 17 (1993) 407-422. performance on intelligence tests, J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol., 16
[16] Riese, W., Brains of prominent people: history, facts and signifi- (1994) 79-83.
cance, MCVQ, 2 (1966) 106-110. [23} Vernon, P.A., Biological Approaches to the Study of Human
[17] Ringo, J.L., Neuronal lnterconnection as a function of brain size Intelligence, Ablex, Norwood, 1993, 416 pp.
(review), Brain Behav. Evol., 38 (1991) 1 ~ . [24] Wickett, J.C., Vernon, P.A. and Lee, D.H., In vivo brain size,
[18] Ringo, J.L., Doty, R.W., Demeter, S. and Simard, P.Y., Time is of head perimeter, and intelligence in a sample of healthy adult fe-
the essence: a conjecture that hemispheric specialization arises males, Pers. Indiv. Diff., 16 (1994) 831-838.
from interhemispheric conduction delay, Cereb. Cortex, 4 (1994) [25] Willerman, L., Schultz, R., Rutledge, J.N. and Bigler, E.D., In
331-343. vivo brain size and intelligence, Intelligence, 15 (1991) 223-228.
[19] Rodin, A.E. and Key, J.D., Osler's brain and related mental mat- [26] Willerman, L., Schultz, R., Rutledge, J.N. and Bigler, E.D.,
ters, South. Med. J., 83 (1990) 207-212. Hemisphere size asymmetry predicts relative verbal and nonver-
[20] Royet, J., Stereology: a method for analyzing images, Prog. Neu- bal intelligence differently in the sexes: an MRI study of struc-
robiol., 37 (1991) 433-474. ture-function relations, Intelligence, 16 (1992) 315-328.
[21] Spitzka, E.A., A study of the brains of six eminent scientists and [27] Witelson, S.F., Glezer, I.I. and Kigar, D.L., Women have greater
scholars belonging to the American Anthropometric Society, to- density of neurons in posterior temporal cortex, J. Neurosci., 15
gether with a description of the skull of Professor E.D. Cope, (1995) 3418-3428.
Trans. Am. Phil. Soc., 21 (1907) 175-308.

You might also like