You are on page 1of 8

This article was downloaded by: [Universitat Politècnica de València]

On: 27 October 2014, At: 02:10


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office:
Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Aging & Mental Health


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription
information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/camh20

Memory enhancement training for older


adults with mild cognitive impairment: A
preliminary study
a a b
S. Rapp , G. Brenes & A. P. Marsh
a
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Medicine, Wake Forest University
School of Medicine , Wake Forest University , USA
b
Department of Health and Exercise Science , Wake Forest University , USA
Published online: 09 Jun 2010.

To cite this article: S. Rapp , G. Brenes & A. P. Marsh (2002) Memory enhancement training for older
adults with mild cognitive impairment: A preliminary study, Aging & Mental Health, 6:1, 5-11, DOI:
10.1080/13607860120101077

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13607860120101077

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”)
contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our
licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or
suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication
are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor &
Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently
verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any
losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities
whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or
arising out of the use of the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial
or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or
distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use
can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
Aging & Mental Health 2002; 6(1): 5–11

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Memory enhancement training for older adults with mild cognitive


impairment: a preliminary study

S. RAPP1, G. BRENES1 & A. P. MARSH2


1Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Medicine, Wake Forest University School of Medicine & 2Department of
Health and Exercise Science, Wake Forest University, USA

Abstract
Downloaded by [Universitat Politècnica de València] at 02:10 27 October 2014

‘Mild cognitive impairment’ (MCI) in older adults refers to a significant decline in memory function but not other cognitive
functions. Pharmacological and non-pharmacological treatments for MCI are needed. The present randomized clinical trial
tests the efficacy of a cognitive and behavioral treatment to improve memory performance and participants’ attitudes about
their memory. A multi-faceted intervention that included education about memory loss, relaxation training, memory skills
training, and cognitive restructuring for memory-related beliefs was compared to a no-treatment control condition.
Outcomes included memory performance and appraisals of memory function and control. Results indicate that the treated
group had significantly better memory appraisals than controls at the end of treatment and at a six-month follow-up. There
were no differences between groups on memory performance at post-test but at follow-up the trained individuals showed a
trend toward better word list recall than controls. Findings suggest that individuals with MCI can benefit from multi-
component memory enhancement training. Further development of such training programs and tests of their efficacy alone
and in combination with medications are needed.

Introduction et al., 1992) and subjective memory (Floyd &


Scogin, 1997). Most studies, however, have trained
The term ‘mild cognitive impairment’ (MCI) has healthy older adults who do not have existing
been used to describe an isolated memory deficit in memory impairment.
older adults in the context of otherwise normal It is also important to modify how they appraise
cognitive functioning. A classification of MCI typi- their memory function and to enhance their sense
cally requires a memory complaint and abnormal of control over their memory abilities (Verhaeghen,
memory function (e.g., >1.5 standard deviations 2000) because one’s sense of control can influence
below expected performance for age on neuropsy- motivation and memory performance (West et al.,
chological tests) but unimpaired activities of daily 2000; Lachman, 2000). Lachman et al., (1992)
living and normal global cognitive function demonstrated significant improvement in memory
(Petersen, 1995; Petersen et al., 1997; Petersen et al., control beliefs following a skills training plus
1999). Research suggests that this population is at appraisal modifying intervention in healthy older
high risk for developing dementia with 10%–15% per adults. West & colleagues (1992) compared a
year converting to Alzheimer’s dementia (AD) memory skills training with practice to memory
compared to 1%–2% of unimpaired older adults skills training plus memory control beliefs enhance-
(Petersen et al., 1999). ment condition and found that both groups
Identifying and treating this at-risk group of improved on measures of memory control beliefs
older adults is important as treatment may prevent and on at least one memory performance task.
or delay the progression to AD and reduce func- Caprio-Prevette & Fry (1996) compared a cogni-
tional impairment (Petersen et al., 1997). In tive restructuring (CR) intervention aimed at
addition to pharmacological treatments, meta- modifying participants’ control beliefs regarding
analytic studies of memory skills training their memory to the traditional memory skills
interventions in the elderly have revealed that (TMS) training in healthy older adults. By follow-
training can improve both objective (Verhaeghen up nine weeks later the CR group outperformed the

Correspondence to: Stephen R. Rapp, PhD., Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Medicine, Wake Forest University
School of Medicine, 1 Medical Center Boulevard, Winston-Salem, NC 27157. Tel: (336) 716 6995. Fax: (336) 716 6830.
E-mail: srapp@wfubmc.edu

Received for publication 12th March 2000. Accepted 17th April 2001.
ISSN 1360–7863 print/ISSN 1364–6915 online/02/010005–07 © Taylor & Francis Ltd
DOI: 10.1080/1360786012010107 7
6 S. Rapp et al.

TMS group on several memory measures and The MFQ consists of 64 items, all rated by the
perceived greater control over their memory. respondent on a seven-point Likert scale. Subscales
The present study tests a multifaceted interven- include general frequency of forgetting (Freq.),
tion for older adults with MCI. The training change relative to retrospective functioning (Retro-
package includes education on memory, relaxation spective), seriousness of forgetting problems
skills training, mnemonic strategies, and cognitive (Seriousness), and mnemonics usage (Mnemonics).
restructuring to change beliefs about memory Mean item scores were used. Higher scores indicate
control. It was expected that compared with the no less memory problem and less mnemonic usage.
treatment control condition, the intervention group
would improve in memory performance and have Perceived control over memory. This was measured
stronger and more positive attitudes regarding their using the Memory Controllability Inventory
memory. (Lachman et al., 1995), which consists of 12 items
forming four subscales (present ability, potential
improvement, inevitable decrement, effort utility) of
Methods three items each. Respondents rate each item on a
seven-point Likert scale to reflect their agreement or
Sample disagreement.
Downloaded by [Universitat Politècnica de València] at 02:10 27 October 2014

Participants for this study were community Mood. To control for the influence of mood states on
dwelling, older adults meeting criteria for MCI memory performance and appraisals (Smith et al.,
(Petersen et al., 1999) including (1) a self-reported 1996), the Profile of Mood States (McNair et al.,
memory complaint, (2) a score on a standardized 1992) was administered at each assessment.
memory test at or below the 10th percentile, (3)
scores on tests of all other cognitive functions
greater than the 10th percentile, (4) normal global Procedures
cognitive functioning, (5) no ADL or IADL defi-
cits, and (6) the absence of dementia. Prospective participants were screened by tele-
phone and were excluded from the study if they
reported a diagnosis of dementia or other cognitive
Measures disorder reported any ADL/IADL impairments;
were currently being treated for major psychiatric
Cognitive function. The Consortium for the Registry disorders, cancer, myocardial infarction or other
of Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD) (Morris et al., heart diseases; if they were taking medications
1989) neuropsychological battery (Welsh et al., (e.g., donepezil, antidepressants) or herbal
1991) was used in assessing cognitive functions mixtures (e.g. gingko biloba) likely to affect cogni-
(verbal fluency, naming, constructional praxis, tive function; or if they had received memory
attention and concentration, executive function and training previously. Prospective participants signed
memory). a written informed consent in accordance with the
WFUSM institutional review board procedures
Global cognitive function. This was assessed with the and were administered the CERAD battery by a
Mini-Mental State Exam (Folstein et al., 1975). trained technician. Informants verified questions
regarding ADLs/IADLs. Individuals meeting the
Memory performance. Outcome measures included criteria for MCI were then administered the four
four memory tasks, each with immediate and memory performance study measures. Persons
delayed recall parts (Scogin et al., 1998). A word list meeting all study criteria were then randomized to
task involved a visual presentation of a list of 20 either the treatment or the control group.
high-imagery, concrete nouns. The grocery list task
is an oral presentation of a 20-item grocery-shopping Treatment condition. The treatment consisted of six,
list presented one at a time. The names and faces weekly group meetings lasting two hours each
task is a self-paced presentation of 15 facial photo- administered by two clinical geropsychologists (SR,
graphs each with a unique surname. The paragraph GB). The first session included a presentation on
recall task is an oral presentation of a brief story dementia, memory and the factors that can influence
(Wechsler, 1981). The scores for both immediate memory performance (e.g., fatigue, anxiety, motiva-
and delayed recall were the total number of elements tion, appraised importance). Participants were
recalled. Higher scores indicate better memory taught a relaxation skill (systematic breathing) and
performance. encouraged to use it to reduce tension, especially
associated with remembering. Each participant also
Perceptions of memory impairment. This was assessed received a copy of Improving Your Memory (Folger
with the Memory Functioning Questionnaire & Stern, 1994) with chapter assignments. There was
(MFQ) (Zelinski et al., 1990; Gilewski et al., 1990). general discussion of the memory problems
Memory training and MCI 7

participants found most disturbing. During the memory skills training. The change in R2 indicates
remaining sessions, the trainers described and the relative effect size of the intervention.
demonstrated specific memory skills (cueing, cate-
gorization, chunking, method of loci) and led group
exercises using them. Homework employing the Results
trained skill was assigned.
Of the one hundred and sixty-eight persons
No-treatment control condition. Participants in this interested in the study, 77 were eligible for initial
group received no memory education or training. At testing. Forty-three individuals had memory func-
the end of the study, each person was given a copy of tion above the threshold for inclusion. Of the 25
the book and other printed materials distributed to remaining persons with MCI, 19 agreed to partici-
the trained participants. pate and were randomized to either the treatment
Within two weeks following completion of the group (n = 9) or the control group (n = 10). The
training, participants in both groups were retested overall sample had slightly more females (58%) and
by the research technician, who was blinded to had a mean age of 75.1 (7.0) years. The sample was
group assignment. Six-months following post- predominantly Caucasian (95%) and well educated
testing participants were retested. (74% having more than 12 years of education).
Downloaded by [Universitat Politècnica de València] at 02:10 27 October 2014

Approximately half (47%) of the sample was


married. The mean MMSE scores were all within
Statistical analyses the normal range (i.e., > 24). Table 1 shows sample
characteristics by group.
T-test and chi-square analyses are used to compare There were no statistically significant differences
groups at baseline on demographic variables. between the treatment and control groups with
Variables with significant pre-treatment group respect to age, race, marital status, education,
differences were retained as covariates in all income, global cognitive function or mood at base-
analyses. To prevent the loss of participant data due line or medication use at any time point (all ps >
to missing data at follow-up for three subjects (two 0.10). There was a significantly greater number of
experimental, one control), multivariate regression males in the control group (p = 0.02); hence,
analyses were used instead of repeated measures gender was entered as a covariate in all analyses.
MANOVA to examine the effects of the memory Between post-treatment and follow-up assess-
skills training on the outcomes after controlling for ments, three participants (two treatment, one
any groups differences at baseline. To test for control) dropped out of the study (2) or could not
training effects between pre- and post-test, pre- tested (1).
intervention scores for the dependent variable (post-
training score) and covariates were entered in the
first step followed in the second step by a group Memory task performance
variable (intervention group = 1, control group = 0).
In separate analyses of pre- to follow-up changes, Table 2 presents the group means and standard
the pre-training score was entered in step 1 to deviations for dependent variables at pre-test, post-
predict follow-up scores. A significant change in the test and follow-up. Regression analyses revealed that
F for the second step indicates a significant effect of following treatment there were no significant

TABLE 1. Sample demographic characteristics by group


Variable Trained group (n = 9) Control group (n = 10)
Mean SD % Mean SD %
Age 73.33 6.61 75.10 7.03
Gender
Male 11 70
Female 89 30
Marital status
Married 44 50
Widowed 33 30
Divorced 22 20
Education
High School graduate 33 20
Some college 22 50
College graduate 11 10
>16 years 33 20
Race (White) 100 90
MMSE 28.0 1.5 27.3 1.88
8 S. Rapp et al.

TABLE 2. Means and standard deviations for memory performance by treatment condition
Treatment Pre-test M (SD) Post-test M (SD) Follow-up M (SD)
Word list-immediate
Training 8.11 (3.02) 11.56 (2.83) 8.71 (3.35)
Control 5.10 (0.99) 7.80 (3.22) 8.33 (2.18)
Word list-delayed
Training 3.56 (2.92) 8.44 (4.22) 6.71 (3.99)*
Control 1.90 (1.45) 4.70 (3.62) 5.89 (3.22)
Shopping list-immediate
Training 7.89 (2.03) 6.33 (1.58) 7.14 (1.77)
Control 7.60 (2.59) 6.10 (1.45) 5.78 (3.11)
Shopping list-delayed
Training 5.44 (1.94) 4.11 (2.37) 5.86 (3.53)
Control 5.40 (2.01) 5.40 (2.22) 5.89 (2.85)
Names and faces-immediate
Training 5.78 (3.35) 4.33 (2.50) 5.43 (3.69)
Control 3.90 (3.70) 2.60 (2.80) 3.89 (3.41)
Names and faces-delayed
Training 4.33 (3.12) 3.22 (2.44) 4.14 (3.85)
Downloaded by [Universitat Politècnica de València] at 02:10 27 October 2014

Control 2.70 (3.23) 1.60 (2.50) 3.33 (3.87)


Paragraph-immediate
Training 10.11 (3.72) 12.11 (4.04) 11.86 (4.06)
Control 9.90 (3.76) 12.00 (2.90) 13.78 (2.49)
Paragraph-delayed
Training 8.56 (4.28) 11.11 (4.17) 11.14 (5.49)
Control 8.30 (4.92) 9.00 (3.59) 11.56 (2.96)
*= p < 0.07. At pre-and post-treatment assessments the Training group had 9 participants and the Control group had 10
participants. At follow-up the ns were 7 and 9, respectively.

differences between groups on the measures of had stronger beliefs in the potential for improvement
memory performance after adjusting for gender (all (MCI-Potential Improvement: p = 0.005, R2 = 0.20)
ps > 0.25). There was a statistical trend at follow-up, and there was a trend toward expecting less inevitable
however. The trained group had higher scores on the decline in memory (MCI Inevitable decline: p = 0.06,
word list task delayed recall than the control group R2 = 0.12). By follow-up, however, there were no
(p = 0.08, R2 = 0.11). There was evidence of a differences between trained participants and untrained
practice effect on the word list and paragraph tasks participants. Examination of the means over time
with scores in both groups improving at each admin- suggests that trained participants’ ratings of control
istration, but not on the shopping list or names and returned to baseline levels by follow-up while
faces tasks. untrained participants ratings at follow-up were lower
than baseline levels.

Perceived memory ability


Memory skill development
There were significant pre-to-post training effects
on participants’ perceptions of their memory The two groups did not differ in their reported use of
ability. The trained group perceived their current mnemonics at the end of treatment (p = 0.48). By
memory ability to be better than controls on the follow-up, the control group was reporting greater use
MCI-Present Ability scale (p = 0.008, R2 = 0.23) of mnemonics than the trained group (MFQ-
and there was a trend their memory was better than Mnemonics: p = 0.008, R2 = 0.30). Untrained partici-
it was in the past (MFQ-Retrospective Func- pants were using ‘to-do’ lists more (p = 0.02) and
tioning, p = 0.07, R2 = 0.20). Training appeared to planning their daily schedule more (p = 0.03) than
have no differential effect on frequency of forgetting trained participants. Trained individuals were using
(p = 0.23). By follow-up, the trained group still grocery lists more than untrained participants
reported significantly better current memory ability (p = 0.06). There were no differences in their use of
than the control group (MCI-Present Ability: appointment books, written reminders, mental repeti-
p = 0.05, R2 = 0.17). There were no other group tion, associations with other things, or keeping things in
differences in perceived ability at follow-up. prominent places (all ps > 0.10). See Table 3.

Perceived control over memory Perceived impact of memory problems

The intervention also improved participants’ beliefs in Both groups characterized their memory problems
their ability to control their memory. The trained group as somewhat serious and causing problems for them
Memory training and MCI 9

TABLE 3. Means and standard deviations for perceived memory and mnemonics use
Treatment Pre-test M (SD) Post-test M (SD) Follow-up M (SD)
Perceived memory ability
MCI-Present ability
Training 3.94 (1.46) 5.36 (0.93)*** 4.86 (0.83)**
Control 3.85 (1.14) 3.98 (0.52) 3.81 (1.08)
MFQ-Frequency of forgetting
Training 4.51 (0.86) 4.55 (0.68) 4.44 (1.39)
Control 4.17 (0.98) 4.04 (0.59) 4.02 (0.82)
MFQ-Retrospective functioning
Training 3.61 (1.21) 4.17 (0.91) 3.46 (0.98)
Control 3.80 (0.85) 3.55 (0.50) 3.33 (0.77)
Perceived impact of memory functioning
MFQ-General functioning
Training 3.83 (0.98) 3.78 (0.97) 3.67 (1.03)
Control 3.88 (1.13) 3.30 (0.82) 3.44 (0.88)
MFQ-Seriousness
Training 4.34 (1.65) 4.52 (1.19) 3.60 (1.34)
Control 3.78 (1.04) 4.20 (0.90) 3.20 (1.16)
Downloaded by [Universitat Politècnica de València] at 02:10 27 October 2014

Memory skill use


MFQ-Mnemonic use
Training 2.19 (0.72) 2.15 (0.71) 2.32 (0.62)***
Control 2.48 (0.94) 2.93 (0.74) 2.18 (0.60)
Perceived control over memory
MCI-Potential improvement
Training 5.15 (0.88) 6.00 (0.65)*** 5.14 (0.69)
Control 4.77 (1.14) 4.37 (0.88) 4.19 (1.03)
MCI-Inevitable decline
Training 3.52 (1.28) 2.70 (1.12)* 3.57 (1.08)
Control 3.67 (1.50) 4.00 (0.63) 3.93 (0.70)
MCI-Effort utility
Training 5.33 (1.15) 5.96 (0.99) 5.29 (0.78)
Control 5.23 (1.57) 5.57 (0.52) 5.11 (1.07)
* = p < 0.07, **= p < 0.05, ***= p < 0.01. At pre-and post-treatment assessments the Training group had 9 participants
and the Control group had 10 participants. At follow-up the ns were 7 and 9, respectively.

at baseline. Groups did not differ at the end of treat- component targeting participants’ beliefs about their
ment or by follow-up in either the perceived impact memory and their ability to control it was effective.
of memory problems (MFQ General Functioning, Lachman et al., (1992) also showed that combining
ps = 0.44, 0.96, respectively) or in the seriousness cognitive training with a cognitive-behavioral inter-
of their memory problems (MFQ-Seriousness, vention targeting control beliefs produced improved
ps = 0.88, 0.41). beliefs about memory control and efficacy. Floyd &
Scogin (1997) concluded from their meta-analysis of
the effects of memory training on subjective memory
Discussion functioning that the effect size for changes in subjec-
tive memory is greater in studies that include both
Individuals with significant memory impairment training in mnemonics and expectancy modification.
represent a highly vulnerable subpopulation of older Our finding of no change in laboratory memory
adults. This study tested a multi-faceted tasks following training is disappointing. In their
intervention that included training in specific meta-analysis of 31 mnemonic training studies of
memory skills, education about memory and healthy older adults, Verhaeghen et al., (1992)
dementia, and a component intended to modify found pre-to-post-test gains associated with
participants’ beliefs about memory control and training equal to 0.73 standard deviations indi-
performance. cating that training can improve memory
We are encouraged by the impact the intervention performance. Younger age, pre-training designed
had on participants’ appraisals of their current and to enhance the learning of mnemonics, training in
future memory abilities. By the end of treatment, groups, and shorter session length were all
trained participants rated their memory ability more associated with training gains. We trained our
favorably than controls. Training also led to stronger participants in groups and pre-trained them by
expectations for future improvement and lower providing information about dementia and memory
expectations for cognitive decline. By six months impairment, by teaching them relaxation skills, and
following treatment, trained participants still rated by encouraging them not to view all memory diffi-
their present memory ability more favorably than culty as catastrophic. The age of our sample (75.1
controls. Thus, it appears that the psychological years), however, was greater than the mean age of
10 S. Rapp et al.

samples Verhaeghen et al., studied (69 years) and persons seek to learn mnemonic strategies would be
the duration of our sessions at two hours was lightly helpful.
longer than the mean session time (1.49 hours, A significant limitation of our study is the small
range: 0.33 hours to 2.5 hours). These factors may number of participants, especially for the follow-up
have made our skills training intervention less analysis. Moreover, we did not correct for multiple
effective. The most important difference between comparisons since the study is exploratory and
our study and those reviewed by Verhaeghen et al., underpowered. As a result our findings should be
(1992) however, may be our selection of individ- viewed as preliminary and merely suggestive. The
uals with documented mild cognitive impairment. R2s from the regression analyses do suggest that
For these individuals, shortening the duration of marginal p-values (0.05 < p < 0.10) were associated
sessions, increasing the number of sessions, with reasonable effect sizes, however. We used
including ‘booster’ sessions, and providing more research diagnostic criteria for MCI developed by
extensive pre-training might improve memory Petersen et al., (1999) but other criteria have been
performance. proposed and might have altered subject selection
Evidence that participants may have needed and results. Lastly, since we did not track the daily
more training sessions came from spontaneous use of the mnemonic techniques, we cannot say
comments made by them during post-testing. precisely how much they were used.
Downloaded by [Universitat Politècnica de València] at 02:10 27 October 2014

Several participants commented to the technician In sum, this study has demonstrated that a brief,
that they were unsure which mnemonic strategy to cognitive and behavioral group intervention
use during memory tasks. This indicates that they targeting memory performance and memory
understood when to use mnemonics but they could appraisals can be effective at changing perceptions
not quickly and easily select or apply them. West & of memory ability in a high-risk population of older
associates (2000) report results similar to ours from adults with cognitive impairment. The results
a pilot study of 40 “at risk” older women i.e., suggest that older adults with cognitive impairment
women who scored one standard deviation below may need more skills training, however, to achieve
their peers on a test of object location recall. Their and maintain performance improvements. An
five-week intervention compared a memory skill intriguing question in light of these results is
whether combining memory enhancement training
plus memory control belief training program to a
and pharmacological interventions for memory
delayed treatment control condition. The trained
would provide benefits beyond monotherapy.
group showed improved control beliefs and
reported greater use of the cognitive strategies, but
both groups showed comparable improvement in
Acknowledgments
memory performance. They concluded that more
practice with memory exercises might have helped
We wish to express our appreciation to Heather
performance.
Uncapher, PhD, for her assistance with the design of
A curious finding was that trained participants
this study and to Stacey Gibson for her help with
reported using mnemonics less than untrained
data collection, and to Forrest Scogin, PhD, for
participants, by the six-month follow-up testing.
permission to use the memory tests.
The difference in scores is due to a slight decrease
in use by trained participants between post-test and
follow-up and a modest increase in use by the
References
controls during the same period. While we clearly
expected trained participants to increase their use CAPRIO-PREVETTE, M.D. & FRY, P.S. (1996). Memory
of mnemonics, some research suggests that enhancement program for community-based older
mnemonics is associated particularly with poorer adults: development and evaluation. Experimental Aging
memory. For example, Small et al., (1995) have Research, 22, 281–303.
reported greater mnemonic usage measured with FLOYD, M. & SCOGIN, F. (1997). Effects of memory
training on subjective memory functioning and mental
the MFQ predicts lower frontal lobe glucose health of older adults: a meta-analysis. Psychology and
metabolism and greater subsequent cognitive Aging, 12, 151–160.
decline (Small et al., 1997). Thus, mnemonics use FOLGER, J. & STERN, L. (1994). Improving your memory.
may indicate poorer perceived memory function, Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.
not better. We may have unintentionally intervened FOLSTEIN, M.F., FOLSTEIN, S.E. & MCHUGH, P.R.
(1975). ‘Mini Mental State’: a practical method for
in contradictory ways. If our intervention reduced grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician.
their appraisals of current and future memory Journal of Psychiatry, 12, 189–198.
impairment, participants in the trained group may GILEWSKI, M.J., ZELINSKI, E.M. & SCHAIE, K.W. (1990).
have felt less need to use mnemonics. The signifi- The Memory Functioning Questionnaire for assessment
of memory complaints in adulthood and old age.
cantly higher perceived memory ability scores for Psychology and Aging, 5, 482–490.
trained participants supports this interpretation. LACHMAN, M.E. (2000). Promoting a sense of control
Additional research into when memory impaired over memory aging. In R.D. HILL, L. B ACKMAN &
Memory training and MCI 11

A.S. N EELY (Eds.), Cognitive rehabilitation in old age [erratum appears in International Psychogeriatrics, 9 (3),
(pp. 106–122). New York: Oxford University Press. 226]. International Psychogeriatrics, 9, 47–56.
LACHMAN, M.E., B ANDURA, M., WEAVER, S.L. & SMITH, G.E., PETERSEN, R.C., I VNIK, R.J., MALEC, J.F. &
ELLIOTT, E. (1995). Assessing memory control beliefs: TANGALOS, E.G. (1996). Subjective memory
the memory controllability inventory. Aging and Cogni- complaints, psychological distress, and longitudinal
tion, 2, 67–84. change in objective memory performance. Psychology
LACHMAN, M.E., WEAVER, S.L., BANDURA, M., ELLIOT, and Aging, 11, 272–279.
E. & LEWKOWICZ, C.J. (1992). Improving memory and VERHAEGHEN, P. (2000). The interplay of growth and
control beliefs through cognitive restructuring and self- decline: theoretical and empirical aspects of plasticity of
generated strategies. Journal of Gerontology Psychological intellectual and memory performance in normal old age.
Sciences, 47, P293–P299. In R.D. HILL , L. BACKMAN & A.S. NEELY (Eds.),
MCNAIR, D.M., LORR, M. & D ROPPLEMAN, L.F. (1992). Cognitive rehabilitation in old age (pp. 3–22). New York:
Profile of Mood States Manual. San Diego, CA: Educa- Oxford University Press.
tional and Industrial Testing Service. VERHAEGHEN, P., MARCOEN, A. & G OOSSENS, L. (1992).
MORRIS, J., H EYMAN, A., MOHS, R. et al. (1989). The Improving memory performance in the aged through
Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s mnemonic training: a meta-analytic study. Psychology
Disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsycholog- and Aging, 7, 242–251.
ical assessment of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 39, WECHSLER, D. (1981). Manual for the Wechsler Adult
1159–1165.
Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R). New York: Psycho-
PETERSEN, R.C. (1995). Normal aging, mild cognitive
logical Corporation.
Downloaded by [Universitat Politècnica de València] at 02:10 27 October 2014

impairment, and early Alzheimer’s disease. The Neurolo-


WELSH, K., BUTTERS, N., HUGHES, J.P., MOHS, R. &
gist, 1, 326–344.
HEYMAN, A. (1991). Detection of abnormal memory
PETERSEN, R.C., SMITH, G.E., WARING, S.C., I VNIK, R.J.,
KOKMEN, E. & TANGALOS, E.G. (1997). Aging, decline in mild cases of Alzheimer’s disease using
memory and mild cognitive impairment. International CERAD neuropsychological measures. Archives of
Psychogeriatrics, 9, 65–69. Neurology, 48, 278–281.
PETERSEN, R.C., SMITH, G.E., WARING, S.C., I VNIK, R.J., WEST, R.L., BRAMLETT, J.P., WELCH, D.C. & BELLOTT,
TANGALOS, E.G. & KOKMEN, E. (1999). Mild cognitive B. (1992). Memory training for the elderly: an intervention
impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. designed to improve memory skills and memory self-evalua-
Archives of Neurology, 56, 303–308. tion. Paper presented at the Cognitive Aging
SCOGIN, F., PROHASKA, M. & WEEKS, E. (1998). The Conference, Atlanta.
comparative efficacy of self-taught and group memory WEST, R.L., WELCH, D.C. & YASSUDA, M.S. (2000).
training for older adults. Journal of Clinical Geropsy- Innovative approaches to memory training for older
chology, 4, 301–314. adults. In R.D. HILL, L. BACKMAN & A.S. NEELY
SMALL, G.W., LA RUE, A., KOMO, S., K APLAN, A. & (Eds.), Cognitive rehabilitation in old age (pp. 81–105).
MANDELKERN, M.A. (1995). Predictors of cognitive New York: Oxford University Press.
change in middle-aged and older adults with memory ZELINSKI, E.M., G ILEWSKI, M.J. & A NTHONY -B ERGSTONE,
loss. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 1757–1764. C.R. (1990). Memory Functioning Questionnaire:
SMALL, G.W., LA RUE, A., KOMO, S., KAPLAN, A. & MAN concurrent validity with memory performance and self-
DELKERN, M.A. (1997). Mnemonics usage and cogni- reported memory failures. Psychology and Aging, 5, 388–
tive decline in age-associated memory impairment. 399.

You might also like