You are on page 1of 34

Weanling infants prefer the odors of green vegetables, cheese and fish when their mothers

consumed these foods during pregnancy and/or lactation

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


Sandra Wagner1, Sylvie Issanchou1, Claire Chabanet1, Christine Lange1, Benoist Schaal1, Sandrine

t
ip
Monnery-Patris1

cr
us
Affiliation: 1Centre des Sciences du Goût et de l'Alimentation, AgroSup Dijon, CNRS, INRA, Université
an
Bourgogne Franche-Comté, F-21000 Dijon, France.
M
ed

Correspondence to be sent to: Sandrine Monnery-Patris, Inra, Centre des Sciences du Goût et de

l’Alimentation, 17 rue Sully, BP 86510 Dijon cedex, France. email: Sandrine.Monnery-Patris@inra.fr


pt
ce
Ac

© The Author(s) 2019. Published by Oxford University Press. All rights reserved. For permissions,
please e-mail: journals.permissions@oup.com
Abstract

Infants' olfactory experience begins before birth and extends after birth through milk and

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


complementary foods. Until now, studies on the effects of chemosensory experience in utero and/or

through human milk focused on experimentally controlled exposure to only one target food bearing

t
ip
a specific odor quality and administered in sizeable amounts. The present study aimed to assess

whether early olfactory experience effect was measurable in everyday conditions of maternal food

cr
intake during pregnancy and lactation, and of infant intake at weaning, leading to expose the infant

us
to corresponding odors as fetus, neonate, and infant up to 8 and 12 months of age. Infants’ early

food exposures were assessed by asking mothers to fill out diaries about their food consumption
an
during pregnancy and breastfeeding, and about their infant’s consumption during complementary
M
feeding. To test odor liking, odorants representing a priori pleasant and unpleasant food odors, as

well as odorless stimuli, were presented. The infant’s exploratory behavior toward odorized bottles
ed

and non-odorized control bottles was measured in terms of mouthing duration, which is thought to

reflect attraction and/or appetence. At age 8 months only, positive correlations were found between
pt

liking of some unpleasant odors and early exposure to these odors through mother’s diet. No
ce

correlations were found between infants’ liking of the pleasant odors and early exposures to the

foods bearing these odors. This study highlights that early exposure to unpleasant food odors may
Ac

increase subsequent liking (or reduce subsequent dislike) of these food odors at least until the age of

8 months.

Key words: Olfaction, amniotic fluid, breast milk, complementary feeding, liking, perinatal

exposure.
Introduction

In human infants, as in other mammalian infants, olfactory experience begins well before direct

exposure to solid foods and related odorous constituents (Schaal 2015, 2016). Some odor-active

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


compounds from the pregnant and lactating mother’s diet are indeed transferred into the prenatal

environment and into mother's milk (Hauser et al. 1985; Mennella 1995; Mennella and Beauchamp

t
1999; Mennella et al. 1995). Such olfactory cues passing the placenta are detectable by the fetus

ip
(Marlier et al. 2001; Schaal 2015) as they subsequently influence the newborns’ reactions to

cr
corresponding odor in aerial conditions of stimulation (Hepper 1995; Schaal et al. 2000). For instance,

us
human fetuses born to mothers who have eaten anise-flavored foods exhibit after birth higher

appetence and attraction to the odor of anethole than non-exposed infants (Schaal et al. 2000).
an
Similarly, odorants transferred from the mothers’ diet to her breast milk are detected by infants and

influence their suckling behavior (Mennella and Beauchamp 1991a, b; 1993 a, b; 1996, 1999). The
M

effect of early experience also extends to the period of weaning or complementary feeding when

infants begin to be directly exposed to solid foods. For example, infants whose mothers drank carrot
ed

juice during pregnancy or breastfeeding exhibit fewer negative facial responses than unexposed
pt

infants when they were fed cereals prepared with carrot juice than when they were fed cereals

prepared with water (Mennella et al. 2001). Prenatal odor experience has been shown to influence
ce

behavior even after several years; thus, 8 to 9 year-old children whose mothers consumed garlic
Ac

during pregnancy eat significantly more garlic-flavored potato purée than non-exposed children

(Hepper et al. 2013). Early odor experience during milk-feeding has also been shown to influence

odor selectivity for more than one year (Delaunay-El Allam et al. 2010) and even into adulthood (e.g.,

Haller et al. 1999).

Food-related odors are known to be important drivers of food liking and consumption in adults

(Engel et al. 2006; Font-i-Furnols 2012; Jaeger et al. 2012) and, before, during, and after actual

ingestion, they provoke a cascade of psychophysiological and metabolic responses (Mattes 1997;
Yeomans 2006). Thus, olfaction may have a rather essential role in the control of the appetence and

intake dimensions of eating behavior. Converging results exist in children and infants. For example,

food neophobia is more a matter of olfactory reactivity than of taste reactivity, suggesting that the

appreciation and rejection of unfamiliar foods, which is common in children and infants, may be

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


prevalently caused by odor cues (Monnery-Patris et al. 2015). Therefore, it is relevant to better

understand how and when early experience with food-related odorants in the pre-weaning (amniotic

t
and lacteal) environment influences the appetitive value of odors conveyed in solid foods.

ip
cr
The aim of the present study was to investigate the relative influence of exposure in the early

succession of niches constituted by the amniotic, lacteal, and the initial post-lacteal food

us
environments on the liking of food-related odor in 8 and 12 month-old French infants. In France, 100
an
% of infants aged on average 8 months have been repeatedly exposed to foods other than human

and/or formula milks (Turberg-Romain et al. 2007). By age 12 months, the infants’ food repertoire
M

has begun to progressively shift from baby foods to table foods (Briefel et al. 2004). Thus, 8 and 12

months can be considered as key ages for the establishment of the food repertoire.
ed

The couple of previous studies focusing on the prenatal shaping of odor liking assessed
pt

subsequent effects in the very short term of several hours or days (Hepper 1995; Schaal et al. 2000).

The present study aimed to investigate longer-term effects in attempting to assess whether such
ce

early exposure effects occur in conditions where the infants are introduced to a wide panel of foods
Ac

that characterizes the culture of their parents. Most studies investigating early odor or flavor

exposure required indeed that participant pregnant or lactating mothers ingest one specific food in

consequent amounts [e.g., 300 mL of carrot juice 4 days/week during 3 weeks (Mennella et al. 2001);

75 g of hummus with 30 mg d-carvone every third day over a period of 28 days (Hausner et al.

2010)]. The originality of the present study was thus to evaluate the consequence of early odor

exposure on the infant through the mothers’ spontaneous consumption of a wide spectrum of foods,

without inducing them to consume any specific foods bearing a given dominant odor. Spontaneous
diets were recorded during late pregnancy and lactation and for the infant, during the first months of

diversified feeding. Early exposure to given odor qualities was inferred from the foods eaten by

mothers (late fetal and lacteal periods) or infants (early post-lacteal period). When the infants were 8

and 12 months of age, their responses to different odor qualities conveyed by several categories of

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


foods were measured to evaluate the magnitude of odor exposure on their hedonic responses to

related odors.

t
ip
Because previous studies suggest positive effects of early odor exposure on later odor

cr
liking in infants (Hepper 1995; Mennella et al. 2001; Schaal 2016; Schaal et al. 2000), we

hypothesized that the more frequently infants were exposed to the target olfactory stimuli

us
during the three periods described above, the higher their subsequent liking of
an
corresponding odors.
M

Experimental
ed

Participants
pt

The present data were collected in the context of the ‘Observatory of Food preferences in Infants
ce

and Children’ (OPALINE study), which aims to understand the formation of food preferences from
Ac

birth up to 2 years of age. Participating mothers were recruited before or during the last trimester of

pregnancy using leaflets left at health professionals and in daycare centers. To be included in the

cohort, both parents had to be older than 18 years (legal age of majority), and their infant had to be

void of any previous medical complication.

The aims and methods of the study were explained to both parents in great detail, after which

they gave their written informed consent. The study was conducted in accordance with the
Declaration of Helsinki for human experimentation and was approved by the local ethics committee

(Comité Consultatif de Protection des Personnes dans la Recherche Biomédicale de Bourgogne).

Mother-infant dyads (n=235) participated in olfactory testing when the infants were 8 and 12

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


months old (at the mean ages (± SD) of 239 ± 13 days, and 372 ± 12 days, respectively). The mothers

had a mean age (± SD) of 32 ± 4 years when infants where 8 months old. The infants were devoid of

t
any oro-nasal affections (cold or allergy) at the time of the tests. Only the infants for whom mothers

ip
provided complete information on their diet during pregnancy and breastfeeding, and on their

cr
infant’s diet during complementary feeding, were entered into the analyses. Thus, the data set

us
included data of 100 mother-infant dyads at the infants’ age of 8 months. Their demographic

characteristics are described in Table 1. Complete data were available at 12 months for 71 of these
an
100 mother-infant dyads.
M

Measurement of infants’ odor liking


ed

Odor stimuli
pt

The stimuli consisted of 8 odorants selected to represent diverse food qualities. Based on previous
ce

literature on early odor preferences (Wagner et al. 2013), 4 odorants were selected to represent

foods considered a priori as pleasant (strawberry, peach/apricot, apple, and vanilla), and 4 odorants
Ac

represented foods considered a priori as unpleasant (2-isobutyl-3-methoxypyrazine, trimethylamine,

dimethyl disulfide, butyric acid) [see Table 2 for the correspondence between odorants and

associated foods]. Control stimuli consisted of mineral oil. All the stimuli were soaked in 11 x 5 cm

strips of scentless absorbent (3M, Liège, Belgium) placed in the bottom of nipple-less, transparent

infant-ergonomic bottles (12 x 6 cm, opening diameter of 2.3 cm; Tex, Carrefour, France).
Procedure of the olfactory tests

The procedure of the olfactory tests, which is described in full details elsewhere (Wagner et al. 2013),

is only summarized here. Infants were individually welcomed in the presence of one parent in a

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


laboratory room dedicated to infant testing. To control for the infants’ hunger state, parents were

asked not to feed them 1.5 hour before the visit to the lab. Parents’ compliance with this instruction

was checked before the beginning of the testing session by asking when they last fed their infant. To

t
ip
accustom the infants to the room and experimenters, a familiarization phase took place before

cr
engaging the tests.

us
For the tests themselves, the infants were exposed to 4 series of 3 test-bottles, sequentially

ordered to present them a scentless control, a pleasant, and an unpleasant odor (Wagner et al.
an
2013). This order of presentation was chosen to avoid the infant refusing to pursue the test after

smelling first an unpleasant odor as was noted in previous studies (Schmidt and Beauchamp 1988)
M

and in our own pilot tests. Exposure to a bottle meant that a given bottle was put under the nose of

the infant during 5 s and then placed on the table in front of the infant who was left free to explore it
ed

by any means. Within series of bottle triplets, bottles were presented separately during 60 s with a
pt

15-s interval between bottles. Between each series of bottle triplets, we managed a 5- to 15-min

break.
ce
Ac

Behavioral analyses and odor-liking scores

All odor tests were videotaped for subsequent analysis of the infants’ spontaneous exploratory

behavior toward the bottles. We focused on “mouthing” behavior, which was previously shown to be

a reliable indicator of hedonic discrimination in infants within the age range considered (Delaunay-El

Allam et al. 2010; Mennella and Beauchamp 1998; Soussignan et al. 1997). Mouthing behavior was

defined as direct contact between the infant’s perioral and/or perinasal areas with the opening of
the bottle. The videos were analyzed frame-by-frame with the Observer software (Noldus,

Wageningen, The Netherlands) by trained observers who were blind to the nature of the odor

conveyed by the bottle. Ten video sequences were randomly selected to check inter-observer

reliability in coding mouthing behavior. The average percentage of agreement was higher than 0.90.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


When relevant, mouthing duration was corrected by the duration of inaccessibility of the bottle (for

example, if dropped onto the floor).

t
ip
The analysis was restricted to the infants who mouthed the bottles for a combined total duration of

cr
at least 10 seconds. Thus, the data corresponding to 10 infants at 8 months of age and to 7 infants at

12 months of age were not considered.

us
For each test odorant, mouthing duration was converted into an odor liking score. These scores were
an
defined as the ratio between mouthing duration for the odorized bottle and the sum of mouthing

durations for the odorized bottle and corresponding control bottle in a triplet. Whereas a score of 0.5
M

indicates indifference for the odorant, a score higher than 0.5 indicates attraction and a score lower

than 0.5 indicates avoidance of the odorant compared to the control.


ed
pt

Recording of dietary data for deriving of odor exposure data


ce

Mothers were asked to fill out food diaries during pregnancy, breastfeeding, and the period of infant
Ac

complementary feeding. The diaries were organized by day (for 7 consecutive days) and by meal. A

very detailed description of the meal was required to allow a precise characterization of odor, taste

and flavor of the food items composing each meal. Mothers were also asked to give details about the

ingredients and seasonings for home-made preparations and to provide the label with the list of

ingredients for commercial dishes (Lange et al. 2007). The instructions were detailed to the mothers

during individual sessions, and the diaries included written instructions with examples.
Maternal diet during pregnancy and breastfeeding

Mothers had to indicate in a qualitative way all the food or beverage items they consumed each day

during one week of the 8th gestational month, and during the 4 weeks of the last gestational month

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


(depending on the infant’s gestational age at birth, there were records for 1 to 4 weeks). The

mothers also completed such diaries daily during one week per month during the breastfeeding

period. For the analyses, we considered first the breastfeeding period from birth to 7 months in

t
ip
order to take into account all exposures up to the first olfactory test at 8 months; then we considered

cr
the breastfeeding period from birth to 11 months included, to take into account all exposures up to

the second olfactory test at 12 months. For the infants who were not breastfed, the breastfeeding

us
exposure was set to 0.
an
M
Infant diet during complementary feeding

The mothers were asked to complete diaries daily about their infants’ qualitative consumption of
ed

foods during one week per month. As the olfactory tests occurred at 8 and 12 months of age, we

derived the food exposure data from two periods: the target infant’s beginning of complementary
pt

feeding (as decided and reported by the mother) to 7 months, and then from the beginning of
ce

complementary feeding to 11 months.


Ac

Selection of food items for deriving infant odor exposure

The frequency at which each infant was exposed to given odorants over the three periods studied

was derived from the mothers’ and infants’ dietary diaries. We extracted from these diaries all food

items carrying dominant odor qualities corresponding to the test odorants. The number of

consumption occurrences was calculated from the mothers’ diaries for each food category and each
period (pregnancy and breastfeeding). The number of occurrences of infants’ direct exposure to

given food odor stimuli was inferred for each food category from the infants’ ingestion of non-milk

foods.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


For the odorants characteristic of apple, strawberry, peach/apricot and vanilla, all food items

composed of apple, strawberry, peach, apricot or vanilla or related artificial aromas were selected

from the diaries. For the other odorants, three chemical data bases of volatile compounds were

t
ip
systematically screened to assess their presence in food items reported in the diaries: the Flavor-

cr
Base© 2010 (http://www.leffingwell.com), the VCF Database 14.1 available through TNO Triskelion

(VCF Volatile Compounds in Food), and the CSGA odor database. Each test odorant was associated

us
with one food category, except for dimethyl disulfide that was associated with two food categories.
an
The correspondences between the test odorants and the food items containing them used for infant

exposure inference are presented in Table 2.


M

Statistical analyses
ed

The statistical analyses were carried out using the R software version 3.0.1.
pt

Odor liking
ce

At each time point, we calculated the individual median odor liking scores for pleasant odors and for
Ac

unpleasant odors. A paired Wilcoxon test was performed to assess whether the median odor liking

scores were significantly different between a priori unpleasant and a priori pleasant odors. Kendall

correlations were calculated to assess whether odor liking scores at 8 and 12 months were

correlated. As positive correlations were expected based on previous studies, unilateral tests were

performed.
Mothers’ food consumption during pregnancy and breastfeeding

Friedman tests were performed to assess whether there were significant differences of

consumptions between the food categories; when the Friedman test was significant Wilcoxon test

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


for paired data with Bonferroni corrections were performed to compare the different food categories

two by two. To assess the continuity of food consumption during pregnancy and breastfeeding,

Kendall correlations were calculated between the consumption of each food category during

t
ip
pregnancy and during breastfeeding. As positive correlations were expected, unilateral tests were

cr
performed.

Relationships between odor liking and early exposures


us
an
The relationships between early exposures during each period and odor liking scores of the related
M

odorant at 8 months (and 12 months) were assessed by calculating Kendall correlations. The

relationships were also assessed for the cumulative periods of exposure, i.e., prenatal plus lactational
ed

and, prenatal plus lactational and complementary feeding. According to our hypothesis, positive

correlations were expected between early exposure and odor liking score, justifying the use of
pt

unilateral tests.
ce
Ac

Results

Liking of food odors

The bottles containing odorants considered as unpleasant were mouthed for marginally shorter

durations than the bottles containing pleasant odorants at 8 months [P = 0.09 – median (Q1-Q3) for

pleasant/unpleasant stimuli were: 0.41 (0.26-0.55)/ 0.39 (0.08-0.51)] and at 12 months, even if the
difference was not significant (P = 0.11), the duration of mouthing [median (Q1-Q3)] for the a priori

pleasant odors was 0.45 (0.18-0.53) versus 0.35 (0.24-0.47) for the a priori unpleasant odors.

For the 71 infants for which data were available at 8 and 12 months, their individual liking scores for

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


all odorants at the two age points were not correlated (all P-values > 0.11).

t
ip
Food items consumed during pregnancy and breastfeeding

cr
The occurrences of mothers’ consumption differed between the target food categories during

pregnancy (Friedman = 452.7, p<0.0001) and during breastfeeding (Friedman = 452.8, p<0.0001). The

us
mothers’ most consumed food category (Figure 1A) during pregnancy was “apple products”
an
(occurrence: median, Q1, Q3 = 33.5, 21, 47), whereas “vanilla desserts” was one of the least

consumed food categories (occurrence: median, Q1, Q3 = 6, 2, 11). During breastfeeding, a similar
M

pattern was found (Figure 1B): the most consumed food category were “apple products”

(occurrence: median, Q1, Q3 = 20.5, 4, 48.5), while “strawberry products” (occurrence: median, Q1,
ed

Q3 = 3, 0, 9.5), “vanilla desserts” (occurrence: median, Q1, Q3 = 4, 0, 9.25) and “sulfurous cheeses”

(occurrence: median, Q1, Q3 = 4, 0, 10) were amongst the least consumed food categories. It is
pt

worth noting that consumption patterns during pregnancy and breastfeeding were weakly but
ce

significantly correlated for each food category (Kendall correlations between 0.16 and 0.34, all P-

values < 0.01), suggesting a qualitative consistency in mothers’ consumption during pregnancy and
Ac

the postpartum period.

[insert Fig. 1 about here]


Food exposures during complementary feeding

The occurrences of infants’ consumption differed between the target food categories from the

beginning of complementary feeding (which is variable and depends on the mother’s decision) to 7

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


months (Friedman = 392.9, P < 0.0001) and to 11 months (Friedman = 408.0, P < 0.0001). From the

beginning of complementary feeding to 7 months, the food categories studied were introduced to at

least 25% of the infants, except for fish, sulfurous cheeses and butyric cheeses, which were only

t
ip
introduced to 9, 2 and 6 infants, respectively. Among the other food categories considered here

cr
(Figure 2A), the infants were the most exposed to “apple products” [occurrence: median (Q1-Q3) = 6

(3-12)], and the least exposed to “vanilla desserts” [occurrence: median (Q1-Q3) = 0 (0-1)]. From the

us
beginning of complementary feeding to 11 months (Figure 2B), all the food categories studied were
an
introduced to at least 89% of infants. Infants were the most exposed to “apple products”

[occurrence: median (Q1-Q3) = 27 (17.5-32)], and the least exposed to sulfurous and butyric cheeses
M

[occurrence: median (Q1-Q3) = 0 (0-0) for both categories) and to fish (occurrence: median (Q1-Q3) =

0 (0-0.5)].
ed
pt

[insert Fig. 2 about here]


ce
Ac

Relationships between early exposures and subsequent odor liking

Relationships between in utero exposure and subsequent odor liking

At 8 months, infants whose mothers reported eating more green vegetables during pregnancy

displayed higher liking scores for the corresponding odorant (Table 3). Additionally, in utero exposure

to sulfurous cheese odor led to a marginally significant correlation with the odor liking score for

dimethyl disulfide odor at 8 months. No significant correlations were found between in utero
exposures and the corresponding odor liking scores at 12 months (Table 4).

Relationships between breastfeeding exposure and subsequent odor liking

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


Infants whose mothers reported eating more green vegetables during their breastfeeding period

displayed a higher liking of the corresponding odor at 8 months (Table 3). Additionally, exposure to

t
ip
fish odor during breastfeeding led to a marginally significant correlation with the odor liking score for

trimethylamine odor at 8 months. No significant correlations were found between exposures during

cr
breastfeeding and the corresponding odor liking scores at 12 months (Table 4).

us
an
Relationships between complementary feeding exposure and subsequent odor liking
M

Direct exposures of infants to the real foods during complementary feeding were not correlated with

odor liking scores at 8 or 12 months (Tables 3 and 4).


ed
pt

Relationships between the cumulative periods of exposure and subsequent odor liking
ce

Exposure to green vegetables odor during the perinatal period (i.e., cumulating exposures in utero

and during breastfeeding) led to a significant correlation with green vegetables odor liking score at 8
Ac

months (tau = 0.28, P <0.001). A significant correlation was also found for this odor when we

considered cumulative exposures during the perinatal period and complementary feeding (tau =

0.27, P <0.001).

Regarding the other odors, no significant correlations were found between the perinatal exposure or

the cumulated exposures over the perinatal and complementary feeding periods and the

corresponding odor liking scores at 12 months.


Discussion

This study aimed to assess longitudinally the association between early exposure to specific dietary

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


odorants, inferred from the diet consumed spontaneously by the pregnant-lactating mother and by

the weanling infant, and subsequent liking of these odors. The relationship between maternal food

t
ip
consumption during pregnancy and breastfeeding and her infant’s subsequent liking for

cr
corresponding odors was based on the assumption that odorous compounds of the mother’s diet are

transferred to the fetus through the placenta and to the nursling through milk. Based on previous

us
studies on human fetuses and neonates (Hepper 1995; Mennella et al. 2001; Schaal et al. 2000), we
an
hypothesized that the perinatal olfactory experience linked with the mother’s ingestion of a given

food leads to higher liking for related food odors at a later age.
M

In the conditions of the present study, such exposure-liking relationships were found significant

only for unpleasant odor qualities, and only at 8 months. Prenatal or lactational exposure to foods
ed

bearing unpleasant odors was associated with increased liking (or reduced disliking) responses of
pt

related odorants. However, similar odor exposure-liking relationships did not reach significance when

the food exposure occurred during complementary feeding. Importantly, no relationships were
ce

found between exposures to foods bearing pleasant odor qualities and the liking of related odors.
Ac

Thus, the present study brought only partial support to the hypothesis that early olfactory

experience would lead to an enhanced liking of a related odor separately from the context of the

entire food flavor.


In utero and breastfeeding exposures in relation with odor liking at 8 months
No significant relationships were uncovered between the mothers’ gestational and lactational

consumption frequency of foods carrying pleasant odors and the infants’ later liking for the pleasant

odors at 8 and 12 months of age. Three out of the 4 pleasant odorants considered in the present

study conveyed fruity odors. The rate of trans-placental and mammary transfer of compounds

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


carrying fruity notes has not yet been systematically analyzed, although isolated evidence indicates

that a fruity-smelling ester is only weakly traced into human milk (Hausner et al. 2008). Nevertheless,

t
one study indicated that 4 to 8 month-old infants express higher acceptance of peaches when they

ip
have been breastfed (as opposed to formula-fed), the authors explaining this result by the fruit-rich

cr
diet of breastfeeding mothers in their study (Forestell and Mennella 2007).

us
By contrast, prenatal exposure to odorants characteristic of green vegetables positively
an
influenced the infants’ liking of the corresponding unpleasant odor at 8 months. A similar significant

positive correlation was noted with lactational exposure to green vegetable odorants, and so was it
M

when the cumulative perinatal exposure (in utero and during breastfeeding) was considered. The

compound used here to represent green vegetable odor, 2-isobutyl-3-methoxypyrazine, has been
ed

found in human milk although without controlling whether the mothers effectively ingested green

vegetables (Buettner 2007). Our results are inconsistent with the results of Forestell and Mennella
pt

(2007). They found no clear evidence for any influence of lactating mothers’ consumption of green
ce

beans on infants’ acceptance of this vegetable at the time of complementary feeding and explained

their results by the low rate of consumption of green beans by the mothers and the fact that being
Ac

exposed to the odor is not sufficient.

Other significantly positive exposure/liking relationships were reached in the present study for

sulfurous cheeses and for fish, but only when infants’ liking of the corresponding odors was assessed

at 8 months. The exposure to cheese was linked with ensuing liking for the cheesy odor dimethyl

disulfide only when the mother consumed cheese while pregnant. Similarly, a liking for the fishy odor

trimethylamine only emerged after maternal ingestion of fish during the breastfeeding period. To our
knowledge, the odorant related to sulfurous cheeses, namely dimethyl disulfide, has not been

identified in amniotic fluid, or in human milk after the ingestion of raw garlic by mothers (Scheffler et

al. 2016). However, these authors identified several garlic-derived metabolites, one of which (allyl

methyl sulfide) having a pronounced garlic-like odor. Trimethylamine, the compound used here to

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


represent fish odor, was found in both amniotic fluid and milk (Lichtenberger et al. 1991).

Despite the fact that food intakes by the mothers during pregnancy and breastfeeding were

t
ip
correlated, the highlighted relationships regarding the two periods of early exposures do not concern

cr
the same foods. Moreover, when considering the cumulative exposure over the in utero and

breastfeeding periods, no relationships were noted for fish or sulfurous cheeses. This raises the

us
question of potentially variable rates of transfer of dietary odorants or their metabolites through the
an
placenta and the mammary systems, of differential solvent properties of amniotic fluid and milk for

various odor-active compounds, and/or of heterogeneous consequences of chemosensory exposure


M

during earliest phases of development of the olfactory tract.


ed

Relationships between exposures during complementary feeding and odor liking at 8 months
pt

In the present conditions, no significant associations were found between food exposures during
ce

complementary feeding and odor liking at 8 months. Thus, the most recent olfactory exposure

through direct feeding does not appear to be the strongest determinant of infants’ odor liking. For
Ac

fish, sulfurous cheeses and butyric cheeses, it may be explained by the fact that these 3 food

categories were introduced to less than 10 infants. For the other food categories, one possibility

might be that the time elapsed between the last food exposures and the olfactory test was too short.

Previous studies suggest indeed that a time interval of less than 4 days between the end of exposure

and the test can affect food acceptance score negatively, by the hypothetical effect of sensory

specific satiety or sensory boredom (Mennella and Beauchamp 1999; Mennella et al. 2006).
Absence of relationships between early exposures and odor liking at 12 months

At 12 months, the relationships between food exposures during the different periods considered in

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


our study (prenatal, breastfeeding, complementary feeding) and ensuing odor liking by infants

appeared to weaken or to vanish. Several explanations may be proposed for this result. First, the

t
method used to gauge odor liking may be less adequate in 12 month-olds than in 8 month-olds who

ip
just begun the transition to post-lacteal foods and, hence, remain more reliably demonstrative in

cr
their behavior toward the test-bottles. Nevertheless, this explanation is unlikely since the infants

us
differentiated pleasant and unpleasant stimuli similarly at 12 months and at 8 months. Second, the

effect of exposures during the prenatal period or during the breastfeeding period may be limited in
an
time. This is supported by studies showing that breastfeeding exposure confers an initial advantage

on food acceptance during complementary feeding, but this advantage wanes when other ways
M

enabling infants to experience flavors operate, such as the direct, repeated exposure to the dietary

flavors (Hausner et al. 2010; Maier et al. 2007) without presumable attenuation by maternal and/or
ed

fetal metabolism. Moreover, there is another study finding that the effect of early exposure on
pt

infants’ preferences for basic tastes was evident in younger children still eating baby foods but was

no longer evident in older children who have started to be offered table foods (Mennella et al. 2009).
ce

Third, as noted above, phenomena of sensory specific satiety or of sensory boredom could explain
Ac

the absence of effect between the most recent exposure to complementary feeding foods and liking

responses to corresponding odors. Finally, in studies demonstrating lengthily persisting effects of

olfactory experience in utero or during breastfeeding (Delaunay-El Allam et al. 2010; Hepper et al.

2013; Mennella et al. 2001), the amount of exposure by the mothers was controlled and relatively

high (Mennella et al. 2001). Therefore, it is possible that the persistent effects of early exposure are

only found after a long and repeated exposure to specific and relatively intense odorants.
The fact that significant relationships were found only between very early exposure to given food

odorants, namely, in utero and breastfeeding, and odor liking at 8 months may point to a sensitive

period for the encoding of concurrent odors or flavors. These results are in line with previous

experimental studies which demonstrated that the last weeks of pregnancy, as well as the first weeks

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


of the breastfeeding period (Mennella et al. 2001), are optimal for the perception and long term

memorization of chemosensory cues (Delaunay-El Allam et al. 2010; Hepper 1995; Hepper et al.

t
2013; Schaal et al. 2000).

ip
cr
us
Limitations of the study

The findings of this study should be interpreted in light of potential limitations. The prenatal and
an
breast milk exposures to the different target odorants were inferred from the spontaneous food
M
intake reported by mothers during pregnancy and breastfeeding, respectively. This study is

composed of a first phase of unsupervised odor/flavor exposure recording by mothers and a second
ed

phase of measurement of ensuing effects in infants. Thus, no causality can be inferred and other

non-controlled factors might account for the observed relationships. For example, a genetic
pt

predisposition of the parents could have been transmitted to their infant and could have influenced
ce

infant odor liking. It is also known that the transfer of volatile compounds of mothers’ diet to the

amniotic fluid or breast milk may differ between compounds and between mothers (Hausner et al.
Ac

2008; Schaal 2005) and this could explain that different associations were found for the exposure in

utero and during breastfeeding. However, considering the number of mothers who participated in

the study, it would not have been possible to analyze the composition in volatile compounds in the

breast milk of each mother during the whole breastfeeding period, and analyzing the amniotic fluid

composition was not at all possible. During the complementary feeding period, odor exposures were

inferred from the occurrences of consumption because mothers were not asked to report the

quantities of the different foods eaten by the infants. However, one may wonder which parameter
(number of occurrences, frequency, intensity of each exposure) is the most significant to describe the

effect of olfactory exposure on odor liking. Otherwise, despite some limitations of the olfactory test,

such as presentation in triplet which are discussed elsewhere (Wagner et al. 2013), the strength of

the present study lays in the recording of the usual food intake of a large range of foodstuffs by

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


mothers in the last months of pregnancy or while breastfeeding, and for the infants during the

complementary feeding period, followed by an experiment conducted with a quite large number of

t
infants.

ip
cr
us
In conclusion, the present study highlights a positive association of prenatal and postnatal

olfactory exposure and enhanced liking, or reduced disliking, of odors later in life. Specifically, early
an
chemosensory exposures to green vegetables, sulfurous cheeses, and fish in utero and/or during

breastfeeding correlate positively with responses indicating acceptance of related dominant odors at
M

the beginning of complementary feeding. From a practical point of view, our work is important

regarding the long-term persistence of perinatal odor impressions and the transmission of food
ed

culture. Further, it gives new highlights to promote the early exposure and acceptance of foods
pt

recommended for their health benefits (e.g., Anzman-Frasca et al. 2018).


ce
Ac

Funding

This work was supported by the Conseil Régional Bourgogne, Franche-Comté (PARI grant) and the

FEDER (European Funding for Regional Economic Development), the Institut Fédératif de Recherche

92, and the Agence Nationale de la Recherche under the ‘Programme National de Recherche en

Alimentation et Nutrition Humaine’ (project ANR-06-PNRA-028, OPALINE). The present study,

labelled by Vitagora® (The Taste, Nutrition and Health Innovation Pole), was also supported by grants

from Blédina, Nestlé, Symrise and Cedus. The present work was part of a PhD work (S. Wagner)
supported by a grant funded by the Nutrition, Chemical Food Safety and Consumer Behavior Division

of INRA (the French National Institute for Agronomical Research) and the Conseil Régional

Bourgogne, Franche-Comté.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


Acknowledgements

t
ip
The authors wish to thank the infants and parents who took part in the present study, and also C.

cr
Laval for recruitment, A.Vincent, A. Fornerol, R. Bouhalassa, E. Szleper, J. Pierard, V. Feyen, F. Durey;

us
V. De Anfrasio for the odor liking data collection; C. Gulluscio for behavioral analyses; S. Jacob and S.

Crevoisier for the food liking data collection; and the whole OPALINE team for support and advice.
an
The authors thank Symrise, IFF and Firmenich for graciously providing the odorants.
M
ed
pt
ce
Ac
References

Anzman-Frasca S, Ventura AK, Ehrenberg S, Myers KP. 2018. Promoting healthy food preferences

from the start: a narrative review of food preference learning from the prenatal period through early

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


childhood. Obes Rev. 19:576-604.

Briefel RR, Reidy K, Karwe V, Jankowski L, Hendricks K. 2004. Toddlers' transition to table foods:

t
Impact on nutrient intakes and food patterns. J Am Diet Assoc. 104:S38-S44.

ip
Buettner A. 2007. A selective and sensitive approach to characterize odour-active and volatile

cr
constituents in small-scale human milk samples. Flavour Fragr J. 22:465-473.

us
Delaunay-El Allam M, Soussignan R, Patris B, Marlier L, Schaal B. 2010. Long-lasting memory for an
an
odor acquired at the mother's breast. Dev Sci. 13:849-863.

Engel E, Martin N, Issanchou S. 2006. Sensitivity to allyl isothiocyanate, dimethyl trisulfide, sinigrin,
M

and cooked cauliflower consumption. Appetite. 46:263-269.


ed

Font-i-Furnols M. 2012. Consumer studies on sensory acceptability of boar taint: A review. Meat Sci.

92:319-329.
pt

Forestell CA, Mennella JA. 2007. Early determinants of fruit and vegetable acceptance. Pediatrics.
ce

120:1247-1254.
Ac

Haller R, Rummel C, Henneberg S, Pollmer U, Köster EP. 1999. The influence of early experience with

vanillin on food preference later in life. Chem Senses. 24:465-467.

Hauser GJ, Chitayat D, Berbs L, Braver D, Mulbauer B. 1985. Peculiar odors in newborns and maternal

pre-natal ingestion of spicy foods. Eur J Pediatr. 144:403.

Hausner H, Bredie WLP, Mølgaard C, Petersen MA, Møller P. 2008. Differential transfer of dietary

flavour compounds into human breast milk. Physiol Behav. 95:118-124.


Hausner H, Nicklaus S, Issanchou S, Mølgaard C, Møller P. 2010. Breastfeeding facilitates acceptance

of a novel dietary flavour compound. Clin Nutr. 29:141-148.

Hepper PG. 1995. Human fetal "olfactory" learning. Int J Prenat Perinat Psychol Med. 7:147-151.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


Hepper PG, Wells DL, Dornan JC, Lynch C. 2013. Long-term flavor recognition in humans with

prenatal garlic experience. Dev Psychobiol. 55:568-574.

t
ip
Jaeger SR, Pineau B, Bava CM, Atkinson KR, McRae JF, Axten LG, Chheang SL, Beresford MK, Peng M,

Paisley AG, Reinbach HC, Rouse SA, Wohlers MW, Jia Y, Newcomb RD. 2012. Investigation of the

cr
impact of sensitivity to cis-3-hexen-1-ol (green/grassy) on food acceptability and selection. Food Qual

us
Pref. 24:230-242. an
Lange C, Laval C, Boggio V, Nicklaus S, Issanchou S, Chabanet C, Monnery-Patris S, Schlich P. 2007. A

tool for characterizing a diet in terms of sensory variety and flavour exposure. In: 7th Pangborn
M

Sensory Science Symposium, August 12-16, 2007, Minneapolis, USA.


ed

Lichtenberger LM, Gardner JW, Barreto JC, Morriss FH. 1991. Evidence for a role of volatile amines in

the development of neonatal hypergastrinemia. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr. 13:342-346.


pt

Maier A, Chabanet C, Schaal B, Issanchou S, Leathwood P. 2007. Effects of repeated exposure on


ce

acceptance of initially disliked vegetables in 7-month old infants. Food Qual Pref. 18:1023-1032.
Ac

Marlier L, Schaal B, Gaugler C, Messer J. 2001. Olfaction in premature human newborns: detection

and discrimination abilities two months before gestational term. In: Marchlewska-Koj A, Lepri JJ,

Müller-Schwarze D, editors. Chemical Signals in Vertebrates 9. New-York: Kluwer Academic, p. 205-

209.

Mattes RD. 1997. Physiologic responses to sensory stimulation by food: nutritional implications. J Am

Diet Assoc. 97:406-413.

Mennella JA. 1995. Mother's milk: a medium for early flavor experiences. J Hum Lact. 11:39-45.
Mennella JA, Beauchamp GK. 1991a. The transfer of alcohol to human milk. Effects on flavor and the

infant's behavior. N Engl J Med. 325:981-985.

Mennella JA, Beauchamp GK. 1991b. Maternal diet alters the sensory qualities of human milk and the

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


nursling's behavior. Pediatrics. 88:737-744.

Mennella JA, Beauchamp GK. 1993a. The effects of repeated exposure to garlic-flavored milk on the

t
nursling's behavior. Pediatrics Res. 34:805-808.

ip
Mennella JA, Beauchamp GK. 1993b. Beer, breast-feeding, and folklore. Dev Psychobiol. 26:459-466.

cr
Mennella JA, Beauchamp GK. 1996. The human infants' responses to vanilla flavors in human milk

us
and formula. Infant Behav Dev. 19:13-19.
an
Mennella JA, Beauchamp GK. 1998. Infant's exploration of scented toys: effect of prior experiences.

Chem Senses. 23:11-17.


M

Mennella JA, Beauchamp GK. 1999. Experience with a flavor in mother's milk modifies the infant's
ed

acceptance of flavored cereal. Dev Psychobiol. 35:197-203.


pt

Mennella JA, Johnson A, Beauchamp GK. 1995. Garlic ingestion by pregnant women alters the odor

of amniotic fluid. Chem Senses. 20:207-209.


ce

Mennella JA, Jagnow CP, Beauchamp GK. 2001. Prenatal and postnatal flavor learning by human
Ac

infants. Pediatrics. 107:e88.

Mennella JA, Kennedy JM, Beauchamp GK. 2006. Vegetable acceptance by infants: Effects of formula

flavors. Early Hum Dev. 82:463-468.

Mennella JA, Forestell CA, Morgan LK, Beauchamp GK. 2009. Early milk feeding influences taste

acceptance and liking during infancy. Am J Clin Nutr. 90:780S-788S.


Monnery-Patris S, Wagner S, Rigal N, Schwartz C, Chabanet C, Issanchou S, Nicklaus S. 2015. Smell

differential reactivity, but not taste differential reactivity, is related to food neophobia in toddlers.

Appetite. 95:303-309.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


Schaal B. 2005. From amnion to colostrum to milk. Odour bridging in early developmental transitions.

In: Hopkins B, Johnson S, editors. Prenatal development of postnatal functions Advances in infancy

research. New York: Praeger, p. 51-102.

t
ip
Schaal B. 2015. Prenatal and postnatal human olfactory development: influences on cognition and

cr
behavior. In: Doty RL, editor. Handbook of Olfaction and Gustation. Hoboken, NJ (USA): John Wiley &

us
Sons, Inc, p. 305-336.

Schaal B. 2016. How amniotic fluid shapes early odor-guided responses to colostrum and milk (and
an
more). In: Etiévant P, Guichard E, Salles C, Voilley A, editors. Flavor: From Food to Behaviors,
M
Wellbeing and Health (460 p). Cambridge (England): Elsevier Ltd., p. 52-72.

Schaal B, Marlier, Soussignan R. 2000. Human foetuses learn odours from their pregnant mother's
ed

diet. Chem Senses. 25:729-737.


pt

Scheffler L, Sauermann Y, Zeh G, Hauf K, Heinlein A, Sharapa C, Buettner A. 2016. Detection of


ce

volatile metabolites of garlic in human breast milk. Metabolites. 6:18.

Schmidt HJ, Beauchamp GK. 1988. Adult-like odor preferences and aversions in 3-year old. Child Dev.
Ac

59:1136-1143.

Soussignan R, Schaal B, Marlier L, Jiang T. 1997. Facial and autonomic responses to biological and

artificial olfactory stimuli in human neonates: Re-examining early hedonic discrimination of odors.

Physiol Behav. 62:745-758.

Turberg-Romain C, Lelievre B, Le Heuzey M-F. 2007. Conduite alimentaire des nourrissons et jeunes

enfants âgés de 1 à 36 mois en France : évolution des habitudes des mères (Evolution of feeding
behavior in mothers of infants and young children from 1 to 36 months old in France). Arch Pediatr.

14:1250-1258.

Wagner S, Issanchou S, Chabanet C, Marlier L, Schaal B, Monnery-Patris S. 2013. Infants’ hedonic

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


responsiveness to food odours:a longitudinal study during and after weaning (8, 12 and 22 months).

Flavour. 2:19.

t
Yeomans MR. 2006. Olfactory influences on appetite and satiety in humans. Physiol Behav. 87:800-

ip
804.

cr
us
an
M
ed
pt
ce
Ac
Table 1. Characteristics of mothers and infants (n=100)

Infants

Gender (N girl / N boy) 42 / 58

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


Feeding mode at birth (N)*

Exclusive bottle-feeding 12

t
ip
Exclusive breastfeeding 81

cr
Bottle- and breastfeeding 5

us
an
Duration of exclusive breastfeeding (days; mean ± SD) 90 ± 60
M

Duration of total breastfeeding (days; mean ± SD) 170 ± 152

Age at onset of complementary feeding (days; mean ± 164 ± 27


ed

SD)
pt

*This information is missing for 2 infants.


ce
Ac
Table 2. Correspondences between the qualities of the test odorants and the foods conveying similar

qualities
Test odorant Food category Foods given to infants

Food items containing vanillin, such as


a
Vanillin Vanilla desserts vanilla cakes or cookies, vanilla dairy
products

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


Apple, Food items containing apple,
b
Apple Apple products such as apple compote or apple pie,
apple juice

t
ip
Peach, Apricot, Food items containing
c Peach or apricot
Peach/apricot peach or apricot, such as peach cake or

cr
products
apricot pie, peach/apricot juice

us
Strawberry, Food items containing
d
Strawberry Strawberry products strawberry, such as strawberry yoghurt
an
or pie, strawberry syrup
M

Dried cod, whitefish, herring in oil, dog


d fish, fresh salmon, tuna, sardine,
Trimethylamine Fish
mackerel, fish soup, shellfish, sea shell,
ed

breaded fish
pt

Raclette, Emmental, Gruyère, Comté,


Beaufort, Mont d'or, Saint Marcellin,
Camembert, Brie, Cantal, Morbier,
ce

a
Butyric acid Butyric cheeses Tomme, Saint Nectaire, Munster,
Epoisses, blue cheese, Cancoillotte,
 
sheep cheese, Bonbel , Babybel , Port
Ac


Salut

Garlic and chives cheese, Cancoillotte,


d
Dimethyl disulfide Sulfurous cheeses Camembert, Munster, Epoisses, blue
cheese

Broccoli, cabbage, cauliflower, Brussels


d
Dimethyl disulfide Sulfurous vegetables sprouts, leek, chive, garlic, onion,
shallot, capers
a
2-isobutyl-3-methoxypyrazine Green vegetables Green pepper, green bean, salad, peas

a
Purchased from Sigma-Aldrich, Saint Quentin Fallavier, France; b provided by Firmenich, Geneva, Switzerland; c provided by IFF, Dijon, France;
d
provided by Symrise, Clichy la Garenne, France.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


t
ip
cr
us
an
M
ed
pt
ce
Ac
Table 3. Kendall correlations (unilateral tests) between odor liking scores for each odorant at 8 months and early exposures to the corresponding odors

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


(significant correlations are shown in bold).
Odorants Food Infants* Liking score** Pregnancy Breastfeeding Complementary
category
feeding

t
ip
Medians τ P τ P τ P
(Q1-Q3) value value value
Odors considered a priori as unpleasant

cr
Trimethylamine Fish 72 0.51 (0.33-0.67) -0.09 0.85 0.12 0.07 -0.15 0.93

us
Butyric acid Cheeses 79 0.39 (0-0.71) 0.04 0.31 -0.02 0.61 -0.07 0.76

an
Dimethyl disulfide Sulfurous 0.12 0.06 -0.04 0.71 -0.06 0.75
cheeses
81 0.41 (0.13-0.64)

M
Dimethyl disulfide Sulfurous -0.01 0.54 -0.12 0.94 -0.12 0.91
vegetables
2-isobutyl-3- Green 79 0.51 (0.33-0.81) 0.30 <0.001 0.16 0.02 -0.07 0.79
methoxypyrazine vegetables

Odors considered a priori as pleasant


ed
pt
Vanillin Vanilla 86 0.51 (0.28-0.72) 0.09 0.11 -0.05 0.73 -0.03 0.64
ce

Apple Apple 77 0.54 (0.32-0.74) 0.07 0.18 0.05 0.28 -0.06 0.76

Peach/apricot Peach/apricot 75 0.54 (0.28-0.75) -0.04 0.69 -0.05 0.72 0.06 0.23
Ac

Strawberry Strawberry 76 0.48 (0.19-0.67) 0.07 0.18 0.03 0.36 0.002 0.49

*The number of infants varied between 72 and 86 because, in some cases, infants did not complete the olfactory test because they became fussy during the session, thus

data were missing for some odorants.


** A score of 0.5 indicates indifference for the odorant, a score higher than 0.5 indicates attraction and a score lower than 0.5 indicates avoidance of the odorant compared

to the control. Here, none of liking scores was significantly different from 0.5.
Table 4. Kendall correlations (unilateral tests) between odor liking scores for each odorant at 12 months and early exposures to the corresponding odors

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


(Statistically significant results are shown in bold).
Odorants Food Infants* Liking score** Pregnancy Breastfeeding Complementary
category
feeding
Medians (Q1-Q3) τ P τ P τ P
value value value
Odors considered a priori as unpleasant

Trimethylamine Fish 52 0.43 (0.12-0.57) 0.11 0.12 -0.04 0.67 0.08 0.23

Butyric acid Cheeses 58 0.42 (0.25-0.64) 0.07 0.23 -0.001 0.50 -0.11 0.85

Dimethyl disulfide Sulfurous -0.13 0.92 -0.17 0.96 -0.10 0.83


cheeses
Dimethyl disulfide Sulfurous 58 0.40 (0.12-0.6 -0.22 0.99 -0.26 0.99 -0.05 0.69
vegetables

2-isobutyl-3- Green 60 0.50 (0.18-0.82) -0.17 0.97 -0.01 0.56 -0.17 0.96
methoxypyrazine vegetables

Odors considered a priori as pleasant

Vanillin Vanilla 58 0.48 (0.26-0.69) 0.07 0.22 -0.16 0.95 -0.04 0.65

Apple Apple 60 0.49 (0.32-0.79) 0.02 0.40 -0.09 0.85 -0.02 0.59

Peach/apricot Peach/apricot 60 0.46 (0.21-0.67) 0.07 0.21 -0.01 0.54 0.08 0.20

Strawberry Strawberry 59 0.49 (0.29-0.74) -0.11 0.89 0.06 0.26 -0.06 0.75

*The number of infants varied between 52 and 60 because, in some cases, infants did not complete the olfactory test because they became fussy during the session, thus

data were missing for some odorants.


** A score of 0.5 indicates indifference for the odorant, a score higher than 0.5 indicates attraction and a score lower than 0.5 indicates avoidance of the odorant compared

to the control. Here, none of liking scores was significantly different from 0.5.
Titles and legends for the figures

Fig. 1. Box-plots depicting the number of occurrences of mothers’ consumption for the different

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


target food categories during the 2 last months of pregnancy from which infants’ in utero exposure to

odorant stimuli were inferred (A) and during breastfeeding from which infants’ exposure to odorant

t
stimuli through human milk were inferred (B). For each box-plot, the bottom and the top of the box

ip
represent respectively the 25th and 75th percentiles and the line within the box the median. The 95%

cr
confidence intervals of the medians are represented. The food categories with a different letter have

us
significant different medians (paired Wilcoxon tests with Bonferroni correction, P < 0.05).
an
Fig. 2. Box-plots depicting food exposures between the beginning of complementary feeding and 7
M

months (A) and between the beginning of complementary feeding and 11 months (B). The 95%

confidence intervals of the medians are represented. The food categories with a different letter have
ed

significant different medians (paired Wilcoxon tests with Bonferroni correction, P < 0.05).
pt
ce
Ac
Figure 1

A B

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


Vanilla desserts d d
Apple products a a
Peach/apricot products b d
Strawberry products d d
Fish d d
Butyric cheeses c b a
Sulfurous cheeses d d
Sulfurous vegetables c b b
Green vegetables c c

0 20 40 60 80 100 0 20 40 60 80 100 120


Number of occurrences of Number of occurrences of
consumption during pregnancy consumption during breastfeeding
Figure 2

A B

Vanilla desserts c d

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/chemse/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/chemse/bjz011/5341973 by guest on 25 February 2019


Apple products a a
Peach/apricot products b b
Strawberry products c c
Fish d e
Butyric cheeses d e
Sulfurous cheeses d e
Sulfurous vegetables c dc
Green vegetables b c b

0 5 10 15 20 25 30 0 10 20 30 40 50 60
Number of occurrences of exposure between Number of occurrences of exposure between
the beginning of complementary feeding the beginning of complementary feeding
and 7 months and 11 months

You might also like