You are on page 1of 6

R E V I E W

Drug and Alcohol Review (2011) DOI: 10.1111/j.1465-3362.2011.00307.x

Elite athletes estimates of the prevalence of illicit drug use: Evidence for the false consensus effect
dar_307 1..6

MATTHEW DUNN, JOHANNA O. THOMAS, WENDY SWIFT & LUCINDA BURNS


National Drug and Alcohol Research Centre, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia

Abstract Introduction and Aims. The false consensus effect (FCE) is the tendency for people to assume that others share their attitudes and behaviours to a greater extent than they actually do. The FCE has been demonstrated for a range of health behaviours, including substance use. The study aimed to explore the relationship between elite athletes engagement in recreational drug use and their consensus estimates (the FCE) and to determine whether those who engage in the behaviour overestimate the use of others around them. Design and Method. The FCE was investigated among 974 elite Australian athletes who were classied according to their drug use history. Results. Participants tended to report that there was a higher prevalence of drug use among athletes in general compared with athletes in their sport, and these estimates appeared to be inuenced by participants drug use history. While overestimation of drug use by participants was not common, this overestimation also appeared to be inuenced by athletes drug use history. Discussion and Conclusions. The results suggest that athletes who have a history of illicit drug use overestimate the prevalence of drug use among athletes.These ndings may be helpful in the formulation of normative education initiatives. [Dunn M, Thomas JO, Swift W, Burns L. Elite athletes estimates of the prevalence of illicit drug use: Evidence for the false consensus effect. Drug Alcohol Rev 2011] Key words: sport, false consensus effect, recreational drug use, illicit drug, athlete. Introduction The false consensus effect (FCE) is the tendency for people to assume that others share their attitudes and behaviours to a greater extent than they actually do [1]. The FCE is thought to result from a bias in social perception [2] and multiple theories have been examined in an attempt to discern how it operates. Marks and Miller in 1987 examined the FCE in relation to four theoretical perspectives, which included selective exposure and cognitive availability, whereby perceptions of similarity are affected by the ease with which evidence of similarity is accessed from memory; salience and focus of attention, in which the focus of attention on ones preferred position may increase estimates of consensus for ones position; logical information processing, where active reasoning and rational processes in the form of casual attribution underlies perceptions of similarities; and motivation, whereby perceiving similarity between self and particular targets may bolster perception social support and validate the correctness or appropriateness of a position [2]. The authors concluded that biases are inuenced by a host of variables and that each theoretical perspective appears to have its own domain of application, albeit with some degree of overlap into other domains [2]. Furthermore, they concluded that two or more mechanisms may operate at the same time to produce assumed similarity [2]. The FCE has been demonstrated for a range of health behaviours, including substance use [36]. The FCE has been investigated in the sporting context, most notably with investigating substance use and tertiary student athletes. These studies have found that student athletes with high rates of alcohol use overestimate alcohol consumption among their athletic and non-athletic peers [710]. Recent research has also investigated the FCE regarding doping in elite athletes [11,12]. For instance, Petrczi and colleagues [12] found that the estimation of doping made by those athletes using performance-enhancing drugs (PED) exceeded that made by non-PED users, and the authors

Matthew Dunn BA (Psych), PostGradDipPsych, GCertPopH, PhD, Lecturer, Johanna O. Thomas BA (Psych), MPH, Research Ofcer, Wendy Swift BA (Hons), MPH, PhD, Senior Lecturer, Lucinda Burns BA (Hons), MPH, PhD, Grad Cert Health Policy, Senior Lecturer. Correspondence to Dr Matthew Dunn, National Drug and Alcohol Research Centre, University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW 2052, Australia.Tel: +61 (0)2 9385 0167; Fax: +61 (0)2 9385 0222; E-mail: m.dunn@unsw.edu.au Received 24 October 2010; accepted for publication 8 January 2011.
2011 Australasian Professional Society on Alcohol and other Drugs

M. Dunn et al.

concluded that the FCE may be a successful way of estimating the prevalence of PED use where self-report may be distorted by socially desirable responses. To date, there has been a lack of research investigating the FCE in regards to illicit drug use among elite athletes. Compared with PED, such as anabolicandrogenic steroids (AAS) and human growth hormones (HGH), athletes are more likely to come into contact with illicit drugs, such as ecstasy, cannabis and cocaine [13]; thus, their perception of the number of other athletes engaging in the use of these drugs could inuence their own decision to use. The aim of the present research was therefore to explore the relationship between elite athletes engagement in illicit drug use and their consensus estimates (the FCE) and to determine whether those who engage in the behaviour overestimate the use of others around them. If this is found to be the case, it will provide direction for more effective prevention campaigns that disavow this myth. Method Participants and procedure Participants were elite athletes from eight national sporting organisations (rugby league, rugby union, athletics, hockey, softball, netball, diving and triathlon) and one national sporting institute (the Australian Institute of Sport) who were recruited as part of a larger study investigating illicit drug use in sport. An athlete was considered elite if eligible for state or national selection in their sport. During the survey period (July 2008 to May 2009), a total of 974 usable surveys were returned and are used in the current analysis. Surveys were completed at team meetings or competitions. A member of the research team informed athletes the purpose of the project; that all information provided was condential and anonymous and that they could decline to participate at any stage. Surveys were self-completed and returned to the researcher. The recruitment and survey implementation process was consistent with two exceptions, where survey completion for netball and softball was coordinated through team managers at team meetings prior to a national tournament and returned to the research team on the day of or after competition. More information on the methodology can be found elsewhere [13,14]. Measures Standard demographic information was collected. Lifetime and recent drug use information was collected for six substances (cannabis, ecstasy, meth/ amphetamine, cocaine, GHB and ketamine) using questions adapted from the National Drug Strategy Household Survey (Australian Institute of Health and Welfare, 2009).
2011 Australasian Professional Society on Alcohol and other Drugs

Participants were asked In your experience, what proportion of athletes in your sport do you feel use illicit drugs? followed by What proportion of athletes in general do you feel use illicit drugs? Categorical responses for both questions included None, Under 2%, 35%, 610%, 1120% and 20%+. These questions were adapted from a study investigating substance use among English footballers [15]. The layout of the survey was such that participants made their estimations prior to questions about their own drug use; however, participants were advised prior to the commencement of the survey that there were questions about their own use in the survey. Data analysis Percentages are presented for categorical variables and means or medians presented for continuous variables. Multinomial logistic regressions were conducted to make comparisons between those who reported never using a drug, those who had used a drug but not in the past year and those who had used a drug in the past year. All analyses were conducted using PASW Statistics Version 18 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). Results Characteristics of the sample The majority of the sample was male (76%) with a mean age of 23.1 years (range 1844 years); 78% were aged 2029 years.The majority (96%) spoke English as their main language at home. Most had completed secondary education (66%) and one-quarter had obtained a tertiary qualication (28%). Virtually all (92%) participated in a team sport and most (76%) trained all of the time with other athletes. Half (51%) indicated that they were a full-time athlete, one-third (29%) indicated they were a full-time athlete also engaging in other work and one-fth (19%) indicated that they were a part-time athlete. Previous analysis of data obtained for this study showed that 7% of participants reported the use of at least one of the six illicit drugs under investigation in the past year (see Dunn et al. [13] for more information). Thus, participants were divided into three groups: those who had never used any of the six drugs under investigation (no-users; n = 744; 76%); those who had used at least one of the six drugs under investigation but not in the past year (lifetime users; n = 163; 17%); and those who had used at least one of the six drugs under investigation in the past year (recent users; n = 67; 7%). Table 1 presents the demographic characteristics for the three groups. Compared with the non-users, life-

False consensus effect and athletes

Table 1. Demographic and drug use characteristics of non-users, lifetime users and recent users Non-users n = 744 (95% CI) 74.6 (7278) 23 [3.6; 1839] 94.1 (9496) 50 (4654) 18.1 (1518) Lifetime users n = 163 (95% CI) 79.1 (7385) 25* [4.1; 1844] 94.5 (9198) 48.5 (4156) 37.4 (3045)*** Recent users n = 67 (95% CI) 79.1 (6989) 24* [2.6; 1829] 88.1 (8096) 73.1 (6384)*** 62.7 (5174)***

Variable Male (%) Age (mean) [SD; range] Completed secondary education or obtained tertiary qualication (%) Full-time athlete (%) Know other athletes who use drugs (%)

*Signicant at the P < 0.05 level, using non-users as the baseline. **Signicant at the P < 0.01 level. ***Signicant at the P < 0.001 level. CI, condence interval.

time users were more likely to know other athletes who use drugs. Compared with non-users, recent users were more likely to be a full-time athlete and to also know other athletes who use drugs. Both lifetime users and recent users were older than the non-users. False consensus effect Participants were asked to indicate the proportion of athletes both in their sport, and in sports in general, they felt used illicit drugs. There was a trend for participants to report that more athletes in general (i.e. those involved in sports other than their own) used illicit drugs than athletes in their sport13% felt that no athletes in their sport used illicit drugs; 28% felt that under 2% used such drugs; 17% felt that 35% used such drugs; 13% felt that 610% used such drugs; 8% felt that 1120% used such drugs; and 7% felt that 20%+ used such drugs. In comparison, 7% felt that, in general, no athletes used illicit drugs; 13% felt that under 2% used such drugs; 18% felt that 35% used such drugs; 20% felt that 610% used such drugs; 14% felt that 1120% used such drugs; and 14% felt that 20%+ used such drugs. Table 2 presents estimates of drug use for the three groups. Compared with non-users, both lifetime and recent users were more likely to believe that athletes in their sport were using illicit drugs. Similarly, compared with non-users, lifetime users were more likely to note that athletes in general were using illicit drugs. Compared with non-users and lifetime users, recent users were more likely to indicate that 20%+ of athletes in general were using illicit drugs. Directional accuracy The proportion of athletes who reported using illicit drugs in the past year was 7% (95% condence interval = 59%). Using this as the true level of illicit drug use, the proportion of the sample that overestimated the prevalence of drug use among athletes in

Table 2. Estimates of other athlete drug use by participants drug use status Non-users n = 744 Lifetime users n = 163 Recent users n = 67

Variable

Proportion of athletes in your sport who use drugs (%) None 15.3 4.3** 3.0*** Less 2% 28.0 36.2 11.9 35% 16.4 21.5 16.4 610% 11.8 19.0 16.4 1120% 7.4 6.7 22.4 20%+ 5.1 10.4** 22.4*** Proportion of athletes in sport generally who use drugs (%) None 9.0 3.1** 0 Less 2% 12.5 14.1 9.0 35% 17.7 23.9 9.0 610% 19.6 22.7 22.4 1120% 12.9 17.2 20.9 20%+ 12.1 13.5 29.9*** *Signicant at the P < 0.05 level, using non-users as the baseline. **Signicant at the P < 0.01 level. ***Signicant at the P < 0.001 level.

their sport and in general was 16% and 28% respectively. Figure 1 presents the proportion of athletes who estimated the prevalence of athletes in their sport and athletes in general who use illicit drugs as being over 11%+ for the three groups. Compared with the proportion of non-users who estimated the prevalence of athletes in their sport who use illicit drugs as being over 11%+, recent users were more likely to endorse the proportion as being over 11%+ (odds ratio = 0.2, 95% condence interval = 0.10.3; P < 0.001) (Figure 1). Similarly, compared with the proportion of non-users who estimated the prevalence of athletes in general who use illicit drugs as being over 11%+, recent users were more likely to endorse the proportion as being over 11%+ (odds ratio = 0.3, 95% condence interval = 0.20.5; P < 0.001) (Figure 1). No effect was found for lifetime use.
2011 Australasian Professional Society on Alcohol and other Drugs

M. Dunn et al.

Figure 1. Prevalence estimates of other athletes who use illicit drugs as being over 11%+. Athletes in their sport, Athletes in general.

Discussion The current study provided evidence for the FCE in regards to estimates of illicit drug use among elite athletes. Participants tended to report that there was a higher prevalence of drug use among athletes in general compared with athletes in their sport, and these estimates appeared to be inuenced by participants own drug use history. Similarly, while overestimation of drug use by participants was not common, this overestimation also appeared to be inuenced by athletes drug use history. The ndings from this study can help inuence the construction of normative educational messages aimed not only at athletes competing at the elite level but also junior athletes who may have had limited occasion to come into contact with recreational drugs. Overestimation of drug use by athletes in participants sport and athletes generally was not common; the prevalence of athletes overestimating drug use was lower than that found in other studies investigating estimations of substance use. Findings from collegebased samples have found that those who report illicit drug use overestimate the proportion of other students who engage in use [35,1620], with some studies showing that between 5070% of participants overestimate peer substance use [4,21]. Despite this, when athlete estimations were considered according to their drug use history, there was a higher proportion of participants overestimating athlete drug use among those who had ever or recently used illicit drugs. In accordance with the World Anti-Doping Agencys various education commitments, many sporting organisations in Australia conduct annual drug information seminars for their athletes. Australian athletes have indicated that presentations and pamphlets are their preferred formats of education [14] and these formats are ideal for presenting normative education messages. For instance, presentations to athletes could present prevalence gures regarding drug use among athletes to help correct biases that athletes might hold.
2011 Australasian Professional Society on Alcohol and other Drugs

Understanding how the FCE operates can assist not only in helping to understand why a particular behaviour may occur but also, as mentioned, help in the design of normative education on drug use in sport. It has been suggested that those who engage in a particular behaviour inate the real consensus so that it normalises their behaviour, thus making it easier to engage in that behaviour [22]. In the context of the current study, this safety in numbers theory [22] suggests that those athletes who engage in drug use may believe that their behaviour is not bad if other athletes are using drugs.While it would seem advantageous for normative education messages to present actual consensus gures for drug use among athletes, there are impediments. First, it has been suggested that there is an absence of a reliable estimate for drug use among athletes [12], and while data from anti-doping organisations [23], national sporting organisations [24] and public health research [13] suggest that low numbers of athletes engage in drug use, these methods rely on either biomedical detection or self-report and thus may be an underestimation. Second, knowing another athlete who uses drugs may have a stronger impact on the decision than can be countered by educational messages. In the current sample, athletes who had used drugs were more likely to indicate knowing other athletes who also use drugs [13]. A study investigating intentions to use AAS among current non-users found that knowing someone who used AAS was related to positive intentions to engage in future use [25]. These issues may need to be taken into account when considering normative education initiatives. Limitations There are limitations to the current study. First, athletes were asked to make estimates about the use of illicit drugs and were not provided with a denition as to what could or could not constitute an illicit drug; therefore, it is not possible to investigate whether the distortion observed was drug-specic. For example, Wolfson [3] found that cannabis and amphetamine users made signicantly higher estimates of cannabis use than nonusers, whereas amphetamine users gave signicantly higher estimates of amphetamine use than non-users and cannabis-only users. Future research may wish to investigate whether the current ndings are replicated when applied to drug-specic scenarios. Second, while this study was specically investigating the use of ecstasy, meth/amphetamine, cannabis, cocaine, ketamine and GHB, participants may have been considering drugs more synonymous with performance enhancement, such as AAS and HGH when making their estimates. Third, this study relied on self-reported drug use, and

False consensus effect and athletes

future research may wish to combine physiological measures to validate self-report. Fourth, the study used self-reported drug use as the true level of drug use. Again, future research may wish to determine this true level of drug use using physiological measures to conrm use. Finally, many of the athletes came from team sports and indicated that they trained most of the times with other athletes; as such, caution should be given when generalising the ndings to athletes who compete in sports with a more individual focus.

Conclusions The present study found that elite athletes tended to report that there was a higher prevalence of drug use among athletes in general compared with athletes in their sport, and these estimates appeared to be inuenced by participants own drug use history. Similarly, while overestimation of drug use by participants was not common, this overestimation also appeared to be inuenced by athletes drug use history. The ndings from this study may be helpful in the formulation of normative education initiatives.

Acknowledgements The current project was funded by the Australian Government Department of Health and Ageing. The authors would like to thank the athletes who participated in the study, as well as staff from the national sporting organisations and Australian Institute of Sport who assisted with this project. The authors would like to thank Mr Mark Deady and Ms Chiara Bucello of NDARC for comments on earlier drafts of this manuscript.

References
[1] Ross L, Greene D, House P. The false consensus phenomenon: an attributional bias in self-perception and social-perception processes. J Exp Soc Psychol 1977;13: 279301. [2] Marks G, Miller N. Ten years of research on the falseconsensus effect: an empirical and theoretical review. Psychol Bull 1987;102:7290. [3] Wolfson S. Students estimates for the prevalence of drug use: evidence for a false consensus effect. Psychol Addict Behav 2000;14:2958. [4] McCabe SE. Misperceptions of nonmedical prescription drug use: a web survey of college students. Addict Behav 2008;33:71324. [5] Juvonen J, Martino SC, Ellickson PL, Longshore D. But others do it!: do misperceptions of schoolmate alcohol and marijuana use predict subsequent drug use among young adolescents? J Appl Soc Psychol 2007;37:740 58.

[6] Otten R, Engels RCME, Prinstein MJ. A prospective study of perception in adolescent smoking. J Adolesc Health 2009;44:47884. [7] Thombs DL. A test of the perceived norms model to explain drinking patterns among university student athletes. J Am Coll Health 2000;49:7583. [8] Dams-OConnor K, Martin JL, Martens MP. Social norms and alcohol consumption among intercollegiate athletes: the role of athlete and nonathlete reference groups. Addict Behav 2007;32:265766. [9] Martens MP, Dams-OConnor K, Duffy-Paiement C, Gibson JT. Perceived alcohol use among friends and alcohol consumption among college athletes. Psychol Addict Behav 2006;20:17884. [10] Turrisi R, Mastroleo NR, Mallett KA, Larimer ME, Kilmer JR. Examination of the mediational inuences of peer norms, environmental inuences, and parent communications on heavy drinking in athletes and nonathletes. Psychol Addict Behav 2007;21:253461. [11] Uvacsek M, Nepusz T, Naughton DP, Mazanov J, Ranky MZs, Petroczi A. Self-admitted behavior and perceived use of performance-enhancing vs psychoactive drugs among competitive athletes. Scand J Med Sci Sports 2009; in press. DOI: 10.1111/j.1600-0838.2009.01041.x. [12] Petrczi A, Mazanov J, Nepusz T, Backhouse SH, Naughton DP. Comfort in big numbers: does over-estimation of doping prevalence in others indicate self-invovlement? J Occup Med Toxicol 2008;3:19. Doi:10.1186/1745-6673-3-19. [13] Dunn M, Thomas JO, Swift W, Burns L. Recreational substance use among elite Australian athletes. Drug Alcohol Rev 2011;30:638. [14] Dunn M, Thomas J, Swift W, Burns L. Attitudes toward, knowledge of, and prevalence of illicit drug use among elite Australian athletes: rst results. Sydney: National Drug and Alcohol Research Centre, University of New South Wales, 2009. [15] Waddington I, Malcolm D, Roderick M, Naik R. Drug use in English professional football. Br J Sports Med 2005; 39:e18. [16] Page RM, Scanlan A. Perceptions of the prevalence of marijuana use among college students: a comparison between current users and nonusers. J Child Adolesc Subst Abuse 1999;9:112. [17] Bon SR, Hittner JB, Lawandales JP. Normative perceptions in telation to substance use and HIV-risky sexual behaviors of college students. J Psychol 2001;135:16578. [18] Perkins HW, Meilman PW, Leichliter JS, Cashin JR, Presley CA. Misperceptions of the norms for the frequency of alcohol and other drug use on college campuses. J Am Coll Health 1999;47:2538. [19] Page RM, Roland M. Misperceptions of the prevalence of marijuana use among college students: athletes and nonathletes. J Child Adolesc Subst Abuse 2004;14:6175. [20] Grossbard JR, Geisner IM, Mastroleo NR, Kilmer JR, Turrisi R, Larimer ME. Is substance use a team sport? Attraction to team, perceived norms, and alcohol and marijuana use among male and female intercollegiate athletes. J Appl Sport Psychol 2009;21:24761. [21] Lai MK, Ho SY, Lam TH. Perceived peer smoking prevalence and its association with smoking behaviours and intentions in Hong Kong Chinese adolescents. Addiction 2004;99:1195205. [22] Suls J, Wan CK, Sanders GS. Falses consensus and false uniqeness in estimating the prevalence of health-protective behaviors. J Appl Soc Psychol 1988;18:6679.
2011 Australasian Professional Society on Alcohol and other Drugs

M. Dunn et al.

[23] Australian Sports Anti-Doping Authority. ASADA annual report 200708. 2008. Available at: http://www.asada. gov.au (accessed February 2011). [24] Australian Football League. 2008 Illicit Drugs Policy results. 2009. Available at: http://a.com.au (accessed February 2011).

[25] Dunn M, Mazanov J, Sitharthan G. Predicting further anabolic-androgenic steroid use intentions with current substance use: ndings from an internet-based survey. Clin J Sport Med 2009;19:2227.

2011 Australasian Professional Society on Alcohol and other Drugs

You might also like