You are on page 1of 10

A Cross-Cultural Study of the Motivational Factors Affecting Individuals Decisions about Participating in Action Sports between Korean College

Students and Their American Counterparts


S. Roger Park (Saint Leo University, USA)

Abstract
The purpose of the study was to examine if there existed significant differences on the motivational factors affecting individuals decisions about participating in action sports between Korean college students and their American counterparts. Four hundred ninety-two action sports participants completed the survey. Of 492 research participants, 275 (55.9%) were Koreans and 212 (43.1%) were Americans with 5 (1%) missing data. The results of MANOVA revealed that American college students had higher levels of participant motivations than their Korean counterparts, including achievement/status-oriented, team-oriented, fitness-oriented, energy release-oriented, miscellaneous reason-oriented, skill development-oriented, friendship-oriented, and fun-oriented, above and beyond what gender, educational level, skill level, and the average number of days of participating in action sports explained.

Introduction
The booming action sports interest has become a worldwide phenomenon. Inline skaters, skateboarders, and BMX riders were estimated at 150 million participants worldwide, and these main three sports have shown a 700% increase in growth over the past 12 years, with a 30% growth in participants each year (LG Mobile Phones, 2003). Professional action sports athletes were estimated at 30,000 people globally (Liberman, 2004). Action sports fans could be found everywhere. A greater number of people have been showing an interest in action sports, and they should no longer be considered a niche market of sport business industry. Sports scholars have shown an increased interest in the area of extreme sports in the past decade and they have applied the motivation and personality theories into extreme sports participants. However, most of these researchers focused only on the traditional extreme sports such as hang-gliding, kayaking, and rock climbing (Doka, Schwarz, & Schwarz, 1990; Shoham, Rose, & Kahle, 1998) excluding action sports such as inline skating, skateboarding, and/or snowboarding. In addition, little research has been done with regard to the motivational factors affecting individuals decisions about participating in action sports comparing and contrasting the similarities and differences between American action sports participants and their Korean counterparts.

Review of Literature
Cross-Cultural Studies in General
A number of research studies support that cultural effect is one of the important factors to explain a certain pattern of individual thought and behavior from the psychological aspects. Yi and Park (2003) found that people with different cultural backgrounds were more likely to have different attitudes and styles of decision making in negotiation, bargaining processes, and problem solving in various social settings because value systems differed. Prior work suggests that the psychological results do not always translate when cultures were so different. Stevenson and Stigler (1992) examined, for example, that there was more belief in malleable intelligence and in the importance of effort among those in the Asian culture than in the American culture when they compared the achievement beliefs of Asian and American school children and their parents. Bracken and Barona (1991) mentioned that obvious cultural influences included beliefs, customs, values, degree of acculturation of assimilation, and generational status of the individual. Furnham, McClelland, and Omer (2003) examined whether ratings of attractiveness and related attributes were indeed pancultural, as evolutionary psychologists have suggested, or cultural-specific as some studies have suggested (Furnham & Alibhai, 1983; Furnham & Baguma, 1994; Zebrowitz et al., 1993). From the perspective of business, a cross-cultural study would still make many contributions to the understanding of the international or multinational business market. Luo, Hoek, and Roos (2001) identified that the ability to effectively manage logistics in a cross-cultural context has become one of the crucial success factors in todays business world in the face of ever-increasing globalization. In addition, they insisted that cross-cultural logistics research could have the benefit of applying the experience learned in cross-cultural research, in general, and through cross-cultural research in management and marketing, through international business.

Cross-Cultural Studies in the Context of Sport


Despite the fact that more and more researchers and marketers all over the world have become interested in cross-cultural studies, relatively few cross-cultural studies exist. Kriska (2000) studied the ethnic and cultural issues in assessing physical activity and explained the substantial differences in the distribution of both chronic diseases and inactivity among the various segments of the population. Not Just a Game (1998) showed that people in various regions enjoy a variety of sports while explaining the worldwide popularity of sporting events and estimating a television audience for the FIFA World Cup Soccer in 1998, 2000 Sydney Olympic Games, Rugby World Cup, two Formula One automobile-racing seasons, and American football. Concerning the two countries of Korea and the United States, Not Just a Game (1998) indicated that the Korean people were more likely to enjoy baseball than any other sports, while the American people were more willing to enjoy football.

Sport Participant Motivation


Gill, Gross, and Huddleston (1983) attempted to measure the sport participants motivations for youth. They created eight motivation factors of sport participation. The initial study showed that the most important reasons for participating were to improve skills, have fun, learn new skills, be challenged, and be physically fit (Gill, 2000). Several others used this measure, or a modification, with other youth sport samples (Gould, Feltz, & Weiss, 1985; Klint & Weiss, 1986; Passer, 1988; Wankel & Kreisel, 1985), and the results were consistent in several ways. Weiss and Chaumeton (1992) cited three common threads. First, several factor analyses yielded consistent factors, including competence, fitness, affiliation, team aspects, competition, and fun. Second, children and adolescents typically indicated that several motives were important. Third, there were minimal age, gender, experience, and sport activity differences. Dwyer (1992) sampled university students using a 5-point response format to examine the measures internal structure. His resulting 6-factor structure (team orientation, achievement/status, fitness, friendship, skill development, and fun/excitement/challenge) was similar to the results with youth samples, and all subscales were internally consistent. The important motives for participating were to (a) maintain fitness; (b) experience fun, excitement, and challenge; and (c) acquire and improve skillsfindings consistent with the youth literature (Gill et al., 1983; Gould et al., 1985; Klint & Weiss, 1987). The least important reasons were friendship-oriented, achievement/status-oriented, and team-oriented factor; these vary from the results with youth.

Instrument Translation and Back-Translation


The instrument translation needs to be loyal to the original context of the source instrument, and it should also reflect a cultural understanding of the target language (Bracken & Barona, 1991). Bracken and Barona (1991) mentioned that the following common translation techniques as (a) interpreters, (b) direct translation, (c) bilingual translation, (d) committee, (e) field-testing, and (f) back-translation. The most commonly applied technique is the back-translation technique. The advantage of the backtranslation technique is that it offers the opportunity for revisions to enhance the reliability and accuracy of the translated instrument (Bracken & Barona, 1991; Geisinger, 1994; Van de Vijver & Hambleton, 1996; Van de Vijver & Leung, 2001). Therefore, the back-translation technique was utilized to obtain consistency by comparing both the Korean and English instrument versions. Researchers have identified the concerns of translating and adapting an instrument from one language to another (Geisinger, 1994; Hui & Traindis, 1985; Van de Vijver & Hambleton, 1996). Brislin (1980) proposed methods, such as back-translation, bilingual, committee, decentering, and pretests. The instruments developed for this current study were translated from the English to the Korean version by concept instead of translating word by word; this is more desirable because the translated items are more meaningful to the Korean population (Geisinger, 1994).

Methodology
Participant Motivations Questionnaire (PMQ)

Participation Motivations Questionnaire (PMQ) (Gill, Gross, & Huddleston, 1983) was used as the research instrument. A 30-item instrument was rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The research instrument consisted of eight dimensions of achievement/status with six items, team-oriented reasons with three items, fitness-oriented reasons with three items, energy release with five items, miscellaneous reasons with three items, skill development with three items, friendships with four items, and fun with three items. The item responses were summed within each sub-dimension to create eight dimensions. The reliabilities of the sub-dimensions were .95, .94, .97, .95, .84, .94, .93, and .95, respectively. The reliability of the eight dimensions was .98. The validity of this instrument was reconfirmed for the current sample.

Demographics
A demographic questionnaire was developed for this study to obtain information concerning personal characteristics, such as gender, educational level (freshman, sophomore, junior, senior, or graduate, etc), skill level, age, nationality, and the average number of days of participating in action sports.

Translation and Back-Translation


There were four steps used in the process of translation and adaptation using the back-translation and bilingual committee methods. Three bilingual committees were selected and they had a minimum of undergraduate degree in the English speaking countries. The first bilingual committee translated the original English version of the instrument into the Korean version (Brislin, 1980). The second bilingual committee back-translated the Korean version into the English version, and the third bilingual committee compared the two English versions. Finally, all three bilingual committees discussed the final version of the instrument.

Participants
Four-hundred ninety-two action sports participants, who were going to 4-year colleges and universities in Kyunggi province in Korea and in the state of Colorado, completed the survey. Data were collected between December 2004 and January 2005 for Korean subjects and between January 2005 and February 2005 for their American counterparts. Of 492 action sports participants, 275 (55.9%) were Koreans and 212 (43.1%) were Americans while 5 (1%) subjects did not disclose their nationality. The research participants averaged 3.20 (SD = 2.09 days) days of participating in action sports per year. The skill levels of the research participants were beginner (35.4%), intermediate level (33.7%), high level (19.1%), professional level (3.0%), and other (1.4%) with 7.3% missing data. In addition, 383 (77.8%) were males and 101 (20.5%) were females while 8 (1.6%) did not answer their gender. The current research participants ranged in age from 18 to 34 years old (M = 23.67, SD = 2.90). The education levels of current research participants were graduate students (7.9%), senior (10.2%), junior (34.3%), sophomore (28.3%), freshman (13.2%), and other (4.5%) with 1.6% missing data.

Data Normality
Test of multivariate data normality was conducted on the research instruments. The main reason for

conducting the normality test was because the data normality would affect the results of statistical procedures (Pedhazur & Schmelkin, 1991; Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). Usually, MANOVA is required to satisfy the assumption of data normality, which means that the observed variables need to be normally distributed (Gravetter & Wallnau, 2000). According to Mardias (1985) suggestions, a skewness or kurtosis value of a variable or an item greater than 2 or smaller than -2 is considered non-normally distributed. Based on the results of Mardias (1985) multivariate normality test, all valuables did fit the assumed distribution of multivariate. In addition, Tabachnick and Fidell (2001) mentioned that sampling distributions of means are normally distributed regardless of the distributions of variables with at least 20 degrees of freedom on the basis of The Central Limit Theorem. Based on The Central Limit Theorem, the normality has been met because each group had more than 20 degrees of freedom. Table 1 shows the mean, standard deviation, and parameters of skewness and kurtosis of the variables for MANOVA for the current sample. The internal consistency for the current research participants estimated by Cronbachs alphas of achievement/status-oriented motivation, team-oriented motivation, fitness-oriented motivation, energy release-oriented motivation, miscellaneous-oriented motivation, skill development-oriented Table 1 Mean, Standard Deviation, and Parameters of Skewness and Kurtosis for MANOVA (N = 475) Variable Age Achievement/Status Fun Friendship Miscellaneous Fitness Skill Team Energy M 23.67 18.63 11.69 14.13 8.38 11.43 10.87 9.35 17.14 SD 2.90 4.57 2.53 3.13 .05 .54 2.89 2.79 3.87 Skewness .05 -.09 -.60 -.34 .21 .07 -.58 -.06 -.29 Kurtosis .22 .04 -.02 .13 .18 .23 -.26 -.23 .18 motivation, friendship-oriented motivation, and fun-oriented motivation were .77, .85, .77, .76, .58, .80, .71, and 75, respectively.

Note. Judgments were made on 5-point scales (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree).

Results
MANOVA was utilized to test if the motivational factors affecting individuals decisions about participating in action sports were different by nationality after blocking the variables of gender, educational level, skill level, and the average number of days of participating in action sports. The

purpose of using blocking variables in MANOVA was to control the effects of educational level, skill level, and the average number of days of participating in action sports. MANOVA was performed on eight dependent variables (DVs): achievement/status oriented, teamoriented, fitness-oriented, miscellaneous-oriented, friendship-oriented, fun-oriented, skill developmentoriented, and energy release-oriented motivations. Independent variable was nationality (Korean and American action sports participants) with blocking independent variables (IVs) of gender, educational level, skill level, and the frequency of participating in action sports. Order of entry of blocking variables was gender, educational level, skill level, and the frequency of participating in action sports. It was critical to block the effects of those variables because the significant differences of DV, the motivational factors affecting individuals decisions about participating in action sports, might bring about by blocking variables not by necessarily nationality. The variable blocking was used basically to control the effects of gender, educational level, skill level, and the average number of days of participating in action sports on DV. Total N of 492 was reduced to 433 with the deletion of a case missing a score on IV and DVs. Tabachnick and Fidell (2001) referred to MANOVA as wasteful if DVs are very highly and positively correlated as well as if DVs are uncorrelated. In order to examine the linear relationship among the DVs, Pearson correlation has been conducted (see Table 2). On the basis of interrelations among the DVs, they were moderately correlated in positive direction (from .38 to .71). Table 2 The Interrelations among the Continuous Variables (N = 479)

Variables 1. Achievement 2. Team 3. Fitness 4. Miscellaneous 5. Friendship 6. Fun 7. Skill 8. Energy


***p < .001.

1 1

2 1

3 .48*** 1

4 .65*** .45*** .52*** 1

5 .64*** .47*** .51*** .45*** 1

6 .56*** .38*** .58*** .47*** .53*** 1

7 .62*** .45*** .59*** .48*** .59*** .71*** 1

8 .59*** .51*** .63*** .52*** .60*** .64*** .57*** 1

.52*** .51***

Based on the results of MANOVA, there was a significant difference on all eight motivational factors affecting individuals decisions about participating in action sports for Korean and American action sports participants by nationality (F(8, 408) = 13.17, p < .001; 2 = .79) after blocking the variables of gender,

educational level, skill level, and the average number of days of participating in action sports. In other words, nationality explained the significant differences on the motivational factors affecting individuals decisions about participating in action sports above and beyond what the gender, educational level, skill level, and the average number of days of participating in action sports explained. This revealed that nationality was a significant variable to differentiate the motivational factors affecting individuals decisions about participating in action sports for Korean and American action sports participants. Table 3 provides means and standard deviations of nationality for Korean and American action sports participants. Table 3 Means and Standard Deviations of Nationality for the Motivational Factors Affecting Individuals Decisions about Participating in Action Sports between Korean Action Sports Participants and Their American Counterparts (N = 433)

Korean Variable Achievement/Status Team-Oriented Fitness-Oriented Miscellaneous Friendship Fun-Oriented Skill Development Energy Release M 17.60 9.03 10.30 7.92 12.67 10.35 9.66 15.44 SD 4.06 2.42 2.16 2.00 2.54 2.13 2.75 3.41

American M 20.12 9.81 12.99 8.99 12.67 13.51 12.54 19.42 SD 4.74 3.14 2.18 2.67 2.68 1.67 2.13 3.15

Discussion
American action sports participants had significantly higher levels of motivational factors affecting individuals decisions about participating in action sports, including achievement/status-oriented, teamoriented, fitness-oriented, friendship-oriented, fun-oriented, miscellaneous-oriented, energy releaseoriented, and skill development-oriented, than their Korean counterparts. In other words, the cultural diversity was a critical factor to differentiate the motivational factors.

Limitations
The first and utmost limitation of this dissertation is the generalizability of the results. This current study adopted a convenient sampling method due to the difficulty in obtaining college action sports participants both in the state of Colorado and in Kyunggi province in Korea. Therefore, it should be careful when generalizing the results of this study. More specifically, the results of this current study might not be generalized beyond the population of college students, who are going to skate parks in the state of Colorado and in Kyunggi province in Korea. However, the study still added more information in the

understanding of globalization of action sports to the existing literature.

Implementation
Action sports consumers are diverse in terms of demographics and psychographics in different consumer markets. However, eight motivations such as achievement/status, team-oriented, fitness-oriented, miscellaneous, friendship, fun-oriented, skill development-oriented, and energy release-oriented can be used as common motivational factors to identify why people enjoy action sports in Korea and in the USA.

Recommendations for Future Study


This current study opened the door for the action sports researchers and business managers to conduct multinational-comparison study to identify the motivational factors affecting individuals decisions about participating in action sports. More and more international levels of action sports events have been born all over in the world. Therefore, it is expected to see more multi-national and/or multi-cultural studies in the area of the action sports industry. Third, quantitative research methodology was beneficial for conducting a cross-cultural study comparing and contrasting the motivational factors between Korean and American action sports participants. However, for future studies, qualitative research methods, including interviewing, would be of use in analyzing the personality of action sports participants because of that methods propensity to dig deep into personality from the psychological perspective.

References
Bracken, B. A., & Barona, A. (1991). State of the art procedures for translating, validating and using psychoeducational tests in cross-cultural assessment. School Psychology International, 12, 119-132. Brislin, R. W. (1980). Translation and content analysis of oral and written materials. In H. C. Triandis & H. W. Berry (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 389-444). Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Doka, K., Schwarz, E. E., & Schwarz, C. (1990). Risky business: Observations on the nature of death in hazardous sports. OMEGA: The Journal of Death and Dying, 21, 215-223. Dwyer, J. J. M. (1992). Informal structure of participation motivation questionnaire completed by undergraduates. Psychological Reports, 70, 283-290. Furnham, A., & Alibhai, N. (1983). Cross-cultural differences in the perception of female body shapes. Psychological Medicine, 13, 829-837. Furnham, A., & Baguma, P. (1994). Gender and locus of control correlates of body image dissatisfaction. European Journal of Personality, 8, 183-200. Furnham, A., McClelland, A., & Omer, L. (2003). A cross-cultural comparison of ratings of perceived fecundity and sexual attractiveness as a function of body weight and waist-to-hip ratio. Psychology, Health & Medicine, 8(2), 219-230. Geisinger, K. F. (1994). Cross-cultural normative assessment: translation and adaptation issues influencing the normative interpretation of assessment instruments. Psychological Assessment, 6(4), 304312.

Gill, D. L. (2000). Psychological dynamics of sport and exercise (2nd ed.). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Gill, D. L., Gross, J.B., & Huddleston, S. (1983). Participation motivation in youth sports. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 14, 1-14. Gould, D., Feltz, D., & Weiss, M. R. (1985). Motives for participating in competitive youth swimming. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 6, 126-140. Gravetter, F. J., & Wallnau, L. B. (2000). Statistics for the Behavior Sciences (5th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Hui, C. H., & Traindis, H. C. (1985). Measurement in cross-cultural psychology: A review and comparison of strategies. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 16(2), 131-152. Klint, K. A., & Weiss, M. R. (1986). Dropping in and dropping out: Participation motives of current and former youth gymnasts. Canadian Journal of Applied Sport Sciences, 11, 106-114. Klint, K. A., & Weiss, M. R. (1987). Perceived competence and motives for participating in youth sports: A test of Harters competence motivation theory. Journal of Sport Psychology, 9, 55-65. Kriska, A. (2000). Ethnic and cultural issues in assessing physical activity. Research Quarterly for Exercise & Sport, 712), 47-53. LG Mobile Phones. (2003). LG action sports [briefing document]. Seoul, Korea: Author. Liberman, N. (2004, January 26-February 1). New alt-sports group wants to sign up athletes and fans. SportsBusiness word=alt-sports Luo, W., Hoek, R. I., & Roos, H. H. (2001). Cross-cultural logistics research: A literature review and propositions. International Journal of Logistics: Research and Applications, 4(1), 57-78. Mardia, K. V. (1985). Mardias test of multinormality. In S. Kotz & N. L. Johnson (Eds.), Encyclopedia of statistical science (pp. 217-221). New York: Wiley. Not just a game. (1998, June 6). Economist, 347(8071), 5-6. Passer, M. W. (1988). Determinants and consequences of childrens competitive stress. In F. Smoll, R. Magill, & M. Ash (Eds.), Children in sport (3rd ed., pp. 203-228). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Pedhazur, E. J., & Schmelkin, L. P. (1991). Measurement, design, and analysis: An integrated approach. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Shoham, A., Rose, G. M., & Kahle, L. R. (1998). Marketing of risky sports: From intention to action. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 26(4), 307-321. Stevenson, H. W., & Stigler, J. W. (1992). The learning gap: Why our schools are failing and what we can learn from Japanese and Chinese education. New York: Summit. Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics (4th ed.). Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Van de Vijver, F., & Hambleton, R. K. (1996). Translating tests: Some practical guidelines. European Journal. Retrieved October 24, 2004, from http://www.sportsbusinessjournal.com/index.cfm?fuseaction=search.show_article&articleId=36346&key

Psychologist, 1(2), 89-99. Van de Vijver, F., & Leung, K. (2001). Methodological issues in psychological research on culture. Journal of Cross-Culture Psychology, 31, 33-51. Wankel, L. M., & Kreisel, P. J. (1985). Factors underlying enjoyment of youth sports: Sport and age group comparisons. Journal of Sport Psychology, 7, 51-64. Weiss, M. R., & Chaumeton, N. (1992). Motivational orientations in sport. In T. Horn (Ed.), Advances in sport psychology (pp. 61-99). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Yi, J. S., & Park, S. (2003). Cross-cultural differences in decision-making styles: A study of college students in five countries. Social Behavior and Personality, 31(1), 35-48. Zebrowitz, L. A., Montepare, J. M., & Lee, H. K. (1993). They dont all look alike. Individuated impressions of other racial groups. Journal of Personal and Social Psychology, 65, 85-101.

You might also like